ArticlePublisher preview available

Economic Threat as a Determinant of Conversion Rates in Authoritarian and Nonauthoritarian Churches

Authors:
To read the full-text of this research, you can request a copy directly from the author.

Abstract

Analyzed statistical data from authoritarian and nonauthoritarian churches to examine the suggestion that threat is an important contributor to individuals' levels of authoritarianism. A variety of findings seem congruent with this hypothesis; however, virtually all of these supportive data have been generated in laboratory experiments involving relatively peripheral apsects of the behavior of undergraduate students. The impact of these results is thus potentially limited to these somewhat artificial situations. Results of the present investigation extend the validity of this hypothesis by indicating that economic bad times (exemplified both by the great depression and by recent conditions in the Seattle, Washington, area) increase the rate of conversions to authoritarian churches, while economic good times increase the rate of conversions to nonauthoritarian churches. The implications of these data for Marx's description of religion as "the opiate of the people" are also discussed. (19 ref.)
Journal
o/
Personality
and
Social Psychology
1972, Vol.
23, No. 3,
420-428
ECONOMIC
THREAT
AS A
DETERMINANT
OF
CONVERSION RATES
IN
AUTHORITARIAN
AND
NONAUTHORITARIAN CHURCHES
STEPHEN
M.
SALES
*
Carnegie-Mellon
University
It has
often
been suggested
that
threat
is an
important contributor
to
indi-
viduals' levels
of
authoritarianism.
A
variety
of findings
seem congruent with
this hypothesis; however, virtually
all of
these supportive data have been
generated
in
laboratory experiments involving relatively peripheral aspects
of
the
behavior
of
undergraduate students.
The
impact
of
these results
is
thus
potentially limited
to
these somewhat
artificial
situations.
The
present investi-
gation
extends
the
validity
of
this hypothesis beyond such settings
by
indi-
cating
that
economic
bad
times
(exemplified
both
by the
Great Depression
and
by
recent conditions
in the
Seattle, Washington, area) increase
the
rate
of
conversions
to
authoritarian churches, while economic good times increase
the
rate
of
conversions
to
nonauthoritarian churches.
The
implications
of
these
data
for
Marx's description
of
religion
as
"the opiate
of the
people"
are
also
discussed.
In
their highly
influential
book,
The au-
thoritarian personality (Adorno,
Frenkel-
Brunswik,
Levinson,
&
Sanford,
1950),
Adorno
and his
coauthors
(19SO)
devoted
some
effort
to
determining
the
factors which
caused some individuals
to be
authoritarian
and
others
to be
nonauthoritarian. Although
their data regarding this issue seem somewhat
suspect (Brown,
1965),
the
Berkeley group
was
nevertheless able
to
draw
an
apparently
reasonable conclusion
from
their results. Spe-
cifically,
they argued that
the
"threatening
traumatic, [and] overwhelming discipline
[p.
372]"
to
which some children
are
exposed
causes them
to
have highly authoritarian per-
sonalities
at
maturity.
Thus,
Adorno
et
al.
strongly implicated threat
as a
basic determi-
nant
of
authoritarianism.
Rokeach
(1960)
reached
the
same conclu-
sion. However,
in
contrast
to
Adorno
and his
collaborators, Rokeach
felt
that
threat
in the
environments
of
even mature subjects might
influence
these individuals' levels
of
authori-
tarianism.
In
Rokeach's terms, "The more
threatening
a
situation
is to a
person,
the
1
The
author
is
indebted
to
Garlie Forehand
and
Esther
G.
Sales
for
their
helpful
comments upon
an
earlier
version
of
this paper.
Requests
for
reprints should
be
sent
to
Stephen
M.
Sales, Department
of
Psychology, Carnegie-Mel-
lon
University, Schenley Park, Pittsburgh, Pennsyl-
vania
15213.
more
closed
[i.e.,
authoritarian]
his
belief
system
will
tend
to
become
[p.
377]."
This
hypothesis
is
readily testable,
and
various
in-
vestigations have
offered
supportive data.
For
instance, Rokeach reported several posi-
tive correlations between
his
dogmatism scale
and
anxiety,
and
Davids
(1955)
replicated
this result
for the
California
F
Scale. More
directly
relevant
to
Rokeach's
prediction,
Sales
and
Friend
(in
press) reported
that
failure-induced
threat
tended
to
increase sub-
jects' scores
on a
self-report index
of au-
thoritarianism, while success tended
to de-
crease their scores
on
this index. Sales
and
Friend also
found
that
their experimental
conditions
affected
subjects' willingness
to
conform
to an
authority
figure (a
reasonable
behavioral criterion
of
authoritarianism).
Fi-
nally,
Dittes
(1961)
indicated
that
threat
caused individuals impulsively
to
reach clo-
sure
on an
ambiguous task, Zander
and
Havelin (1960) reported that threat
led
indi-
viduals
to
reject persons dissimilar
to
them-
selves,
and
Berkowitz
and
Knurek (1969)
found
that
threat increased subjects' hostil-
ity
toward members
of
experimentally created
outgroups,
All of the
dependent variables
used
in
these latter investigations
are
behav-
iors
which
are
said
to
characterize authori-
tarian
persons;
all of
them were increased
420
... Today, authoritarianism is primarily understood as a willingness to submit to authority and a preference for intense group cohesion and conformity, as opposed to a preference for autonomy and self-regulation (e.g., Duckitt, 1989;Feldman, 2003;Stenner, 2005). Authoritarianism has been characterized as a psychological security mechanism in reaction to various forms of threat (e.g., Brandt & Henry, 2012;Doty et al., 1991;Mirisola et al., 2014;Oesterreich, 2005;Sales, 1972Sales, , 1973. The authoritarian reaction can take various forms: for example, heightened submission to authoritarian leaders and regimes; hostility and aggression toward "deviants," or an orientation toward conformity (e.g., Duckitt, 1989;Marcus et al., 2019;Oesterreich, 2005;Stenner, 2005). ...
... Authoritarianism has been linked to poverty virtually since its inception (e.g., Adorno et al., 1950Adorno et al., /2019. A range of evidence indeed supports the idea: For example, several studies show an increase in authoritarian reactions, such as the support of authoritarian leaders, in times of economic threat (Doty et al., 1991;McCann, 1999;McCann & Stewin, 1987;Sales, 1972Sales, , 1973. Other studies have indicated a negative relationship between income and rightwing authoritarianism (Carvacho et al., 2013), authoritarian child-rearing values (Brandt et al., 2015), preference for submission to authority (Napier & Jost, 2008), or support for anti-democratic leaders (Lipset, 1960(Lipset, /1981. ...
... Previous arguments, although mostly nonempirical, can be subsumed into three themes: stress, anxiety, and shame. For example, Sales (1972Sales ( , 1973, Edelman (2021), or McCann (1999 argue that economic threat and struggle may lead to feelings of stress, defined as "an organism's reaction to environmental demands exceeding its regulatory capacity" (Haushofer & Fehr, 2014, p. 862), which in turn may evoke authoritarian reactions. Indeed, a range of studies provides correlational and causal evidence for the general link between poverty and stress (for an overview, see Haushofer & Fehr, 2014) and a positive relationship between stress and support for authoritarian leadership (Bass, 1992;Fodor, 1976) or authoritarian attitudes (Russo et al., 2020). ...
Article
Full-text available
The literature has widely discussed and supported the relationship between poverty and support for authoritarian leaders and regimes. However, there are different claims about the mediating mechanism and a lack of empirical tests. We hypothesize that the effect of poverty on support for authoritarianism is mediated by shame: People living in poverty frequently experience social exclusion and devaluation, which is reflected in feelings of shame. Such shame, in turn, is likely to increase support for authoritarianism, mainly due to the promise of social re-inclusion. We support our hypothesis in two controlled experiments and a large-scale field study while empirically ruling out the two main alternative explanations offered in the literature: stress and anxiety. Finally, we discuss how the present findings can support policymakers in efficiently addressing the negative political consequences of poverty.
... These factors-lack of well-defined financial losses and absence of a coercive dictatoroverlook critical elements of many important choice tradeoffs in society [12,14,27,38,39], and likely artificially inflate the apparent utility and preference for choice. DeCaro et al. (2020) [13] addressed these concerns by examining coercive exchanges involving well-defined financial losses. ...
... It is important to consider the involvement of self-efficacy and security because prior theory suggests there is a crucial link [12,25,29,33]. Preference for coercive, authoritarian leaders, norms, and government systems is thought to be based, in part, on the sense of economic welfare and security those regimes promise to provide to inefficacious individuals [27,28,38,59]. Hence, the prospect of economic security may outweigh the prospect of procedurally fair or autonomy-supportive leaders and decision processes. ...
... This finding supports prior theoretical speculation that security is a determining factor behind individuals' decisions to not only grant decision control to others [12,14], but also specifically to coercive others [25,28,29]. This demonstration is important because prior research has been largely non-experimental [e.g., 36,38,39,44]. Furthermore, the few prior experiments have either focused on benign no-choice conditions that lack an authority figure [e.g., 10] or benevolent authorities [45,53]-not coercive authorities who actively seek to limit personal choice. ...
Article
Full-text available
Individuals typically prefer the freedom to make their own decisions. Yet, people often trade their own decision control (procedural utility) to gain economic security (outcome utility). Decision science has not reconciled these observations. We examined how decision-makers’ efficacy and security perceptions influence when, why, and how individuals exchange procedural and outcome utility. Undergraduate adults (N=77; Mage=19.45 years; 73% female; 62% Caucasian, 13% African American) were recruited from the psychology participant pool at a midwestern U.S. metropolitan university. Participants made financial decisions in easy and hard versions of a paid card task resembling a standard gambling task, with a learning component. During half the trials, they made decisions with a No-Choice Manager who controlled their decisions, versus a Choice Manager who granted decision control. The hard task was designed to be too difficult for most participants, undermining their efficacy and security, and ensuring financial losses. The No-Choice Manager was designed to perform moderately well, ensuring financial gains. Participants felt greater outcome satisfaction (utility) for financial gains earned via Choice, but not losses. Participants (85%) preferred the Choice manager in the easy task but preferred the No-Choice Manager (56%) in the hard task. This change in preference for choice corresponded with self-efficacy and was mediated by perceived security. We used Decision Field Theory to develop potential cognitive models of these decisions. Preferences were best described by a model that assumed decision-makers initially prefer Choice, but update their preference based on loss-dependent attentional focus. When they earned losses (hard task), decision-makers focused more on economic payoffs (financial security), causing them to deemphasize procedural utility. Losses competed for attention, pulling attention toward economic survivability and away from the inherent value of choice. Decision-makers are more likely to sacrifice freedom of choice to leaders they perceive as efficacious to alleviate perceived threats to economic security.
... Though both stems from a predisposition towards social conformity, RWA is rooted in a view of the world being a dangerous place, thereby increasing cognitive rigidity on conservative attitudes (Crowson et al., 2005;Duckitt, 2001;Duckitt & Sibley, 2010;Osborne et al., 2017). Consistent with this argument is that increases in levels of threat cause an increase in authoritarianism, as well as support for authoritarian leaders and policies (Bonanno & Jost, 2006;Duckitt & Fisher, 2003;McCann, 1997;Sales, 1972;Stellmacher & Petzel, 2005). On the other hand, SDO stems from a view that the world is a competitive jungle, creating intergroup struggle for dominance and superiority (Crawford & Pilanski, 2014). ...
Chapter
In this chapter, a brief history is presented of how psychologists have attempted to understand the concept of authoritarianism. It also explores the relationship between Right-Wing Authoritarianism and Social Dominance Orientation and their association with political attitudes and personality traits.KeywordsPersonalityTrait theoryAuthoritarianismRight-Wing Authoritarianism (RWA)Social Dominance Orientation (SDO)Prejudice
... Considering the role of threat perception introduced in the previous paragraph, it is possible that the same or similar emotions factored in the referendum. This is because issues such as immigration and sovereignty were salient topics; and topics which have all been shown to divide opinion along an authoritarianism fissure (Sales, 1972;Lavine et al., 1999;Hetherington and Weiler, 2009). For example, it may have been possible to influence voters with low authoritarianism, who might otherwise have voted Remain, to vote Leave by increasing their concerns and therefore fears about immigration. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction The results of the United Kingdom’s 2016 referendum on European Union (EU) membership have highlighted deep societal divides. In six studies, we examined the role of personality traits, cognition and cognitive biases in relation to referendum voters’ choices. Methods A total of 11,225 participants completed questionnaires and controlled experiments, which assessed differences in personality traits, levels of authoritarianism, numeracy, thinking styles, and susceptibility to cognitive biases including ideologically motivated numeracy and reasoning, framing, and the Dunning-Kruger effect. Results Participants expressing an intent to vote to leave the EU reported significantly higher levels of authoritarianism and conscientiousness, and lower levels of openness and neuroticism than voters expressing an intent to vote to remain in the EU. When compared with Remain voters, Leave voters displayed significantly lower levels of numeracy and appeared more reliant on impulsive System 1 thinking. In the experimental studies, voters on both sides were found to be susceptible to the cognitive biases tested, with a general trend for Leave voters to show more bias than Remain voters. Discussion These results raise important questions regarding the use and framing of numerical and non-numerical data for public consumption.
... There is empirical evidence in support of this hypothesis. For example, personal and global threats increase support for conservative policies and politicians (Cohen et al., 2005;Landau et al., 2004;, conservative socio-political attitudes (Bonanno & Jost, 2006;Echebarria-Echabe & Fernández-Guede, 2006), and conservative social cognition (McGregor et al., 2001;Sales, 1972;Ullrich & Cohrs, 2007). Research suggests that both threat sensitivity and a heightened threat situation are associated with increased activation of moral foundations associated with conservatism (Wright & Baril, 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
There is considerable research showing that economic threat influences people's social and political views. There are two prevailing perspectives on threat and political attitudes , broadly defined as the Conservative Shift Hypothesis and the Entrenching Hypothesis. The former predicts that threat induces change in the conservative direction (for both conservatives and liberals), whereas the latter predicts that threat causes people to adhere more strongly to their pre-existing political perspective. In two experimental studies (one pre-registered replication), we find evidence in support of the Entrenching Hypothesis. Conservatives responded to Economic Threat with increased endorsement of the conservative moral foundation Purity, whereas liberals responded to Economic Threat with decreased endorsement of the Purity foundation. Economic Threat appears to increase commitment to one's pre-existing political ideology and not conservatism specifically. Implications for psychological theory and future directions are discussed.
... One classic study found that anxiety-provoking and morbid stimuli increased belief in an afterlife among people who had reported high belief in a pre-screen questionnaire (Osarchuk & Tatz, 1973). More recent evidence has confirmed that reminders of mortality increase religious beliefs (Norenzayan & Hansen, 2006) and that people turn to religion after stressful or traumatic experiences such as war (Henrich et al., 2019), natural disasters (Sibley & Bulbulia, 2012), or economic downturns (McCann, 1999;Sales, 1972). ...
Article
Full-text available
Religion makes unique claims (e.g., the existence of supernatural agents) not found in other belief systems, but is religion itself psychologically special? Furthermore, religion is related to many domains of psychological interest, such as morality, health and well-being, self-control, meaning, and death anxiety. Does religion act on these domains via special mechanisms that are unlike secular mechanisms? These could include mechanisms such as beliefs in supernatural agents, providing ultimate meaning, and providing literal immortality. We apply a critical eye to these questions of specialness and conclude that although it is clear that religion is psychologically important, there is not yet strong evidence that it is psychologically special, with the possible exception of its effects on health. We highlight what would be required of future research aimed at convincingly demonstrating that religion is indeed psychologically special, including careful definitions of religion and careful attention to experimental design and causal inference.
Article
The motive domains of control and truth align with conceptions of God's omnipotence and omniscience, but the link between control-versus truth-dominant motives and God concepts remains unclear. A convergence account predicts a positive association between these variables; a divergence account predicts an inverse association. Furthermore, the causal direction of an association under either account is unknown. To test between these competing accounts, Study 1 investigated whether people with God concepts dominant in control versus truth report motives that are also control- (vs. truth-) dominant. To investigate causality, Study 2 manipulated God concepts and measured motive dominance; Study 3 manipulated motives and measured God concepts. Study 4 replicated Study 1 with methodological enhancements. Collectively, results supported the convergence account, indicating that God concepts influence motive predominance. By integrating motivation and religious cognition research, this work elucidates the relations between beliefs about God's attributes and personal motives.
Article
Democracy was forged in the furnaces of oppression, whether combatting tyranny or affirming the rights of the individual. As democracy is under threat in many parts of the world, there has never been a more urgent need to understand political thoughts and behaviours. This lucid and accessible book brings together a global group of scholars from psychology, political science, communication, sociology, education and psychiatry. The book's structure, based on Abraham Lincoln's well-known phrase 'Of, by and for' the people, scrutinises the psychological factors experienced by politicians as representatives 'of' the electorate, the political institutions and systems devised 'by' those we elect, and the societies that influence the context 'for' us as citizens. From trust to risk, from political values to moral and religious priorities, from the personality and language of leaders to fake news and anti-democratic forces, this book provides vital new insights for researchers, politicians and citizens alike.
Article
Although personal control is a fundamental human need, research has not yet systematically examined how it functions in consumer and marketplace settings. This article reviews and integrates the existing research on the topic to provide a greater understanding of how personal control and consumer behavior shape and inform one another. We first integrate multiple streams of research to discuss the conceptualization and antecedents of personal control. We then propose an organizing framework that identifies two ways in which feelings of low control shape consumer behavior: through motivating consumers to look for a sense of order and structure in their consumption environments and through motivating consumers to use consumption activities to reestablish feelings of control. We close by highlighting several future research directions for advancing the current understanding of how personal control and marketing relate.
Article
Full-text available
Investigated the relationship between locus of control and frustration within the conceptual framework of J. Rotter's social learning theory. 80 undergraduates were given Rotter's Internal-External Control Scale, Child and Waterhouse's Frustration-Reaction Inventory, the Marlowe-Crowne Social Desirability Scale, the TAT, and a frustrating angle-matching task. Results support the prediction that internal Ss would report more constructive reaction to frustration than external Ss. Sex differences were obtained on the angle-matching task, with internal males and external females behaving similarly. The frustration manipulation did not cause differential reaction to the angle-matching task, but external Ss did report more negative outcomes in TAT scores than internals. Results are discussed within a social learning framework. It is suggested that future work on frustration and locus of control be directed toward children. (21 ref.)
Article
Full-text available
Some persons impose meaning and pattern––closure––on ambiguous stimuli more readily than other persons the hypothesis is proposed that closure generally acquires a self-esteem enhancing reward value and that persons experiencing greater need to maintain or enhance self-esteem are likely to impose more impulsive closure. Self-esteem was manipulated by causing some subjects to experience failure on an ego involving task and others to experience failure on a noninvolving task or to experience success. Ego involving failure resulted in more impulsive closure.
Article
The abstract for this document is available on CSA Illumina.To view the Abstract, click the Abstract button above the document title.
Article
Various authors have argued that the more threatening an individual's environment is, the more authoritarian he is likely to be. This hypothesis was investigated in two separate experiments. In both, Ss were pretested for authoritarianism and then were exposed to one of two conditions. In the first condition, Ss were threatened by failure at two experimental tasks; in the second, they were presumably encouraged by success at these tasks. In both studies, failure increased and success decreased the Ss' levels of authoritarianism. In both studies, these changes in authoritarianism were greater for individuals who attributed their performance to internal causes. In addition, data from the second investigation indicated that these manipulations affected Ss' tendencies to conform to the judgments of an authority figure. The findings appear to support the contention that personality is an open system.
Historical statistics oj the United States: Colonial times to 1957. Washington, D.C.: United States Government Printing Office
  • Bureau
  • Of
  • Census
BUREAU OF THE CENSUS. Historical statistics oj the United States: Colonial times to 1957. Washington, D.C.: United States Government Printing Office, 1960.
Are churches really booming? Christian Century
  • W H Hudson
HUDSON, W. H. Are churches really booming? Christian Century, 1955, 77, 1494-1496.