ArticlePDF Available

Research that Helps Move Us Closer to a World Where Each Child Thrives

Authors:

Abstract

Schools are curtailing programs in arts, physical exercise, and play to devote more time and resources to academic instruction. Yet doing that may impede academic success, rather than aid it. Correlational and retrospective studies, personal accounts, case studies, and theoretical arguments suggest that the arts (e.g., music, dance, and theatre) and/or physical activities (e.g., sports, martial arts, and youth circus) can transform kids’ lives. Do they? Causal studies are lacking. There’s enough suggestive evidence; the time is ripe for rigorous research on real-world arts and physical activity programs that permit conclusions about causality to be drawn and that investigate what characteristics of programs account for benefits. Granting agencies should be more open to funding such research.
Research in Human Development, 12: 288–294, 2015
Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 1542-7609 print / 1542-7617 online
DOI: 10.1080/15427609.2015.1068034
Research that Helps Move Us Closer to a World Where
Each Child Thrives
Adele Diamond
University of British Columbia
Schools are curtailing programs in arts, physical exercise, and play to devote more time and resources
to academic instruction. Yet doing that may impede academic success, rather than aid it. Correlational
and retrospective studies, personal accounts, case studies, and theoretical arguments suggest that the
arts (e.g., music, dance, and theatre) and/or physical activities (e.g., sports, martial arts, and youth
circus) can transform kids’ lives. Do they? Causal studies are lacking. There’s enough suggestive
evidence; the time is ripe for rigorous research on real-world arts and physical activity programs that
permit conclusions about causality to be drawn and that investigate what characteristics of programs
account for benefits. Granting agencies should be more open to funding such research.
I would like to encourage the field of human development to take as its charge: How can we help
children (and help the adults in their lives to help the children) navigate their growing up so they
grow into people we would all be proud to know—people who are upright, honest, considerate,
compassionate, kind, caring, self-confident yet humble, proud but not arrogant, conscientious,
bright, capable, playful, full of joy and a sense of wonder, committed to making a contribution—
regardless of a child’s nationality or country of birth, ethnicity, or gender, healthy or challenged,
born into affluence or poverty, strife or peace?
A few things follow from that: (1) We need more studies with clear real-world implications
and/or applications and (2) We need to see more “flesh” on the bones of many studies. I would
like to “hear” from the people who received an intervention or program. What was their experi-
ence in the program like? I’d like to hear what they have to say about ways the program failed
to live up to their hopes or expectations. Were there one or two particularly key moments or
experiences in the program they can describe? I’d like to know if they see themselves any differ-
ently now that they have experienced the program. That is, I would like to see more qualitative
data, more first-person accounts, alongside the “bare bones” of numbers entered into, and gener-
ated from, statistical analyses. And, I would like to hear from any observers or onlookers. What
changes have they observed and what did they see that led them to conclude that a particular
change had occurred?
Address correspondence to Adele Diamond, Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia,
2255 Wesbrook Mall, Vancouver, BC Canada V6T 2A1. E-mail: adele.diamond@ubc.ca
Downloaded by [The University of British Columbia] at 09:51 09 November 2015
RESEARCH THAT HELPS MOVE US CLOSER TO A WORLD WHERE EACH CHILD THRIVES 289
Importantly, (3) we need to see studies that enable us to draw strong causal inferences about
real-world activities that I and many others would predict have a high likelihood of helping chil-
dren grow into the kind of people I have described, activities that kids find fun and meaningful
such as social or community action projects that help one’s neighbors, neighborhood, or make
the world a better place (such as Free the Children), music-making (be it band, orchestra [such
as El Sistema], a drumming circle, choral singing, or something else), team sports (whether bas-
ketball, soccer, rowing crew, or any number of other team sports), martial arts, youth circus,
social communal dance, filmmaking, theatre, caring for an animal, 4-H, orienteering, building
things together, repairing things together, or any number of other comparable activities (Diamond,
2014).
TOPICS THAT NEED RIGOROUS RESEARCH AND THAT FEDERAL FUNDING
AGENCIES SHOULD BE MORE WILLING TO FUND
Sadly, there is a marked dearth of studies on the benefits of (the benefits created by or caused
by) the arts, physical activities, or community-focused activities for healthy development, pride
and self-esteem, feelings of self-efficacy and confidence, and outcomes such as better executive
functions, academic achievement, high school graduation, mental health, or physical health, and
reduced arrests, crime, substance abuse, depression, or unemployment.
The few studies that exist of these sorts of activities tend to be correlational, retrospective, or
case studies, which cannot demonstrate causality (i.e., they cannot demonstrate that these activi-
ties produced benefits). Instead of causal studies of such organic activities, many of which have
been around since the beginning of civilization, we see causal studies of more sterile and one-
sided activities such as computerized cognitive training, treadmill running, or riding a stationary
bicycle. Among studies having characteristics that meet the minimum criteria I present below and
looked at the effects of different activities at any age on executive functions, 29 investigated com-
puterized cognitive training and 15 investigated aerobic walking or running or weight training, but
only one investigated martial arts, only one investigated yoga, and only one investigated theater.
No studies to date (zero) have investigated whether participation in any team sport, music making
of any kind, participating in any social or community service activity, dance, social circus, and so
on produces (causes) any social, emotional, or cognitive benefit.
Studies of these real-world activities are needed!
The different parts of the human being (social, emotional, cognitive, physical, and spiritual)
are fundamentally interrelated (Diamond, 2007,2014). As I wrote in 2010,
Academic achievement, social–emotional competence, and physical and mental health are funda-
mentally and multiply interrelated. The best and most efficient way to foster any one of those ...is
to foster all of them .... We need to see the human being and human development as one whole,
that those who care deeply about developing cognitive competence, social skills, emotional well-
ness, or physical health and fitness are not in competition, that one component is not more important
than any another, and that we have much to learn from the insights and accumulated wisdom of our
counterparts in other fields and specialties. (Diamond, 2010, p. 789)
For example, executive functions (self-control, working memory, cognitive flexibility, focused
attention, and creative problem-solving) suffer first and most if anyone is stressed, sad, lonely, not
Downloaded by [The University of British Columbia] at 09:51 09 November 2015
290 DIAMOND
getting enough sleep, or not physically fit (reviews: Diamond, 2007,2013,2014; Ling, Kelly, &
Diamond, in press). Even relatively mild stress overwhelms prefrontal cortex (but not other brain
regions) with excess dopamine (Cerqueira, Mailliet, Almeida, Jay, & Sousa, 2007) and disrupts
communication between prefrontal cortex and other brain regions (Liston, McEwen, & Casey,
2009), thereby impairing executive functions (Arnsten, 1998). People often can’t think as clearly
or exercise as good self-control when stressed and may appear to have a disorder of executive
functions when they do not. The same is true if you don’t get enough sleep (Huang et al., 2007).
We show less creativity, are less able to think outside the box, and show worse attentional control
when we are sad (Ashby, Isen, & Turken, 1999; Desseilles et al., 2009; Hirt, Devers, & McCrea,
2008). Conversely, we are able to work more flexibly (Murray, Sujan, Hirt, & Sujan, 1990), are
more likely to see potential relatedness among unusual and atypical members of categories (Isen,
Daubman, & Nowicki, 1987; Isen, Johnson, Mertz, & Robinson, 1985), and able to deploy our
attention more efficiently and broadly (Grol, Koster, Bruyneel, & De Raedt, 2014) when we
are happier. (It’s not that sadder people are less creative, but rather that in general an individual
tends to be more creative when happier than when more miserable). We are fundamentally social;
people who feel alone, or are focusing on an anticipation of being alone, usually show poorer EFs
than people who feel, or anticipate feeling, more socially supported (Cacioppo & Patrick, 2008;
Campbell et al., 2006; Tangney, Baumeister, & Boone, 2004). Feeling excluded or as if you do
not belong has been shown in controlled experiments to impair EFs such as reasoning, selective
attention and persistence on difficult problems (Baumeister, DeWall, Ciarocco, & Twenge, 2005;
Twenge, Catanese, & Baumeister, 2002).
People who are more physically active, have better aerobic fitness, and better motor coordi-
nation show better EF performance (Boucard et al., 2012; Prakash, Voss, Erickson, & Kramer,
2015; Scudder et al., 2014; Voelcker-Rehage, Godde, & Staudinger, 2010).
Thus, if we ignore that a child is stressed, lonely, or not healthy, those unmet needs will work
against that child showing the executive functions of which he or she is capable and thus showing
the academic achievement of which the child is capable. Conversely, our executive functions are
at their best when we are not feeling stressed, feel socially nourished, and are happy, well rested,
and physically fit.
Trying to improve executive functions by the narrow strategy of only targeting executive func-
tions (as with computerized training regimens) or trying to improve physical health by narrowly
targeting only aerobic capacity (as in many exercise studies) in people who are feeling sad, lonely,
or stressed is less likely to succeed than approaches that address the whole person. Activities
and programs that should most successfully improve executive functions are those that not only
directly train and challenge executive functions but those that also indirectly support executive
functions by helping to reduce things that disrupt executive functions (e.g., loneliness) and/or
by increasing things that aid executive functions (e.g., happiness). They are precisely the pro-
grams for which it is so difficult to get funding to study and that hardly anyone is studying with
paradigms that permit causal inferences to be drawn.
The real-world activities for which I am calling for study address the whole person. For exam-
ple, making music together, dancing together, team sports, collaborating on a social or community
action project, martial arts, filmmaking, or theatre require, train, and challenge concentration,
focused attention, discipline, perseverance, split-second responses to the unexpected, creativity,
and holding complex sequences in mind (i.e., executive functions). They provide tremendous joy
Downloaded by [The University of British Columbia] at 09:51 09 November 2015
RESEARCH THAT HELPS MOVE US CLOSER TO A WORLD WHERE EACH CHILD THRIVES 291
and build feelings of pride, self-efficacy, and self-confidence. Participants are members of a com-
munity or team (they belong to something bigger than themselves), all working toward the same
shared goal. These activities are hands-on. They require moving, using one’s body, and often
build motor skills, coordination, balance, and/or aerobic capacity.
After the initial studies, we will need studies that hone in on what works best for whom, why,
and what the essential elements are. For example, would learning to play a musical instrument
through private lessons be less beneficial (as I predict) than learning to play a musical instrument
as is done in El Sistema (as a member of an ensemble from the start)? How important is the
presence or absence of competition? How important is the mentoring piece (an adult who really
and truly cares about you, listens when you need an ear and hears what you have to say, is
consistently there for you, respects you, and believes in you)? Will the type of program end up
mattering more or the way it is done? I have predicted that the way it is done will prove the
more critical variable (Diamond & Ling, in press). How important is it to involve participants as
decision makers, giving them a say in what is done and how, granting them a degree of autonomy?
Social-service and community-action projects are acts of generosity and caring. How important is
that element for various desirable outcomes such as self-confidence, pride, better ability to focus
and concentrate, reducing the likelihood of drug addiction or crime, or nurturing compassion?
I fear that exactly the activities needed to help children thrive are being cut from school cur-
ricula and from children’s lives. We in psychology can do something about that; my wish is that
we do.
STUDY CHARACTERISTICS THAT ARE ABSOLUTELY ESSENTIAL
“Studies that enable one to draw conclusions about causality” means (1) studies that are not
simply correlational (e.g., studies that look at one snapshot in time and find, for example, that
children who did A where better at X; we have no way of knowing if they might have been better
at X even if they had not done A). (2) Studies that include control groups (e.g., when studies
document that children who did A got better at X over time but do not have a control group, we
have no way of knowing if the children might have gotten better at X even if they had not done
A). (3) Studies that find differential outcomes between groups (e.g., if children who did not do A
or did B improved as much and performed as well afterwards as children who did A, we cannot
know whether A and B helped equally or neither A nor B helped at all; to draw a causal inference
we need differential benefits). Those three things are minimum criteria that any study that claims
to inform us about the benefits of a program or activity should meet.
STUDY CHARACTERISTICS WE SHOULD ASPIRE TO HAVE
Ideally, an intervention study should also have four other features: First, the control group should
not be passive, waitlist, or business as usual. Such a control group is not without any value; it
allows us to conclude that benefits from the program of interest were not simply due to (1) practice
effects from completing the assessment measures before and after the intervention or (2) the pas-
sage of time or developmental improvements over the period between pre- and posttest. However,
such a control group presents a low bar to pass because anything new might produce better results
than nothing or business as usual; indeed the hopes and expectations for what the intervention
Downloaded by [The University of British Columbia] at 09:51 09 November 2015
292 DIAMOND
could achieve might be the main causal factor rather than the intervention itself. Therefore, ideally
an intervention study should include an “active control group,” which also receives a new pro-
gram for which the hopes and expectations for success are also high, and whose members also
receive as much attention as those in the experimental group—that is, which is comparable to
the experimental group in all the ways that might be pivotal, except for the aspect(s) that the
researcher thinks are key to the intervention’s success.
Second, an intervention study ideally should look not only at immediate postintervention ben-
efits but also at benefits down the road. How long do benefits last? Do some benefits grow larger
over time? This is seldom done. For example, of the 62 studies that met the three minimum criteria
above that examined whether computerized or noncomputerized cognitive training, mindfulness,
aerobic activities, resistance training, or school curriculum might improve executive functions,
only 16% (10 studies) looked at any effects beyond immediately right after the program, only
10% (six studies) looked at benefits 6 months or longer after the program, and only 5% (three
studies) looked at outcomes a year or more after the program (Diamond & Ling, in press). That
is a pretty dismal record. We (i.e., the field) need to do better.
Third, participants need to be assigned to conditions because, if they can choose, one or more
factors that led them to choose different programs might be responsible for any differential out-
comes observed for the programs. The gold standard is random assignment. However, random
assignment should only be expected to yield groups with comparable characteristics when deal-
ing with large numbers, not when the number of participants per group is under 100 and certainly
not when that number is under 50. It is important to stratify randomization by participant vari-
ables potentially important to the results or to pair participants as closely as possible on variables
that might influence the results and then randomly assign one member of each pair to each group.
The other problem with random assignment and lack of choice is that some participants might
not like the activity of interest and might actively resist what the activity is trying is accomplish,
thus reducing the size of one’s effect. One way to address that is to only include children who
express interest in the activity of interest or only include teachers who express interest in teaching
the experimental curriculum and then randomly assign one half to the new activity or curriculum.
It’s true that in the real world teachers often have to teach an assigned curriculum without a
choice. However, after a curriculum has been shown to be effective, many more teachers are
likely to be open to teaching it than before effectiveness has been demonstrated.
Fourth, too often studies rely exclusively on what people tell us on surveys or rating forms, yet
research has shown time and again that what people report can often be inaccurate and unreliable
(Austin, Gibson, Deary, McGregor, & Dent, 1998; Donaldson & Grant-Vallone, 2002; Wells &
Olson, 2003). Questionnaires and rating forms should be complemented by one or more objective
measure. Too often studies rely on laboratory tests that bear little relation to the real world. Better
objective measures that are more relevant to real life are needed.
CONCLUSION
In sum, I am calling for well-designed intervention studies that permit strong causal inferences
to be drawn that examine the possibility that the arts, social service or community action, or/and
physical activities like sports, martial arts, or circus are critical for the outcomes we all want
for our children. I am calling for funding agencies to be far more willing to fund such critical
studies and their longitudinal follow-up. And I am calling for studies that thoughtfully explore
Downloaded by [The University of British Columbia] at 09:51 09 November 2015
RESEARCH THAT HELPS MOVE US CLOSER TO A WORLD WHERE EACH CHILD THRIVES 293
what works best for whom, and why—especially what critical elements must be present for
benefits to be seen. To fully understand and properly interpret the numbers generated by quanti-
tative research, and to generate new hypotheses for quantitative research to test, it is critical that
researchers make greater efforts to elicit first person accounts of participants and insights from
observers.
I have offered some predictions. I predict that the most beneficial activities will be those that
address the whole person: The most beneficial activities, I predict, tap into something the child
is, or becomes, passionate about. They provide the child tremendous joy and pride. They enable
the child to work on something deeply meaningful to that child and to others, who support one
another as part of a community, team, or ensemble, whose members are there for each other and
pull for one another. They enable a child to feel empowered and build the child’s deep-seated
confidence that he or she will eventually succeed. They enable a child to feel heard and deeply,
genuinely cared about, often by an adult mentor. The mentors believe in the kids to succeed while
holding them to uncompromisingly high standards. These activities build many skills including
discipline; the ability to persevere despite setbacks; cooperation; and compromise; seeing things
from others’ perspectives; focused attention and concentration; creative problem solving; and
flexibly adjusting to the unexpected. They keep one physically fit, with good dexterity, coordina-
tion, and balance. An arts, physical-activity, or social-action activity when done well can do all
these things. I have predicted such activities are not only best for well-rounded benefits but for
the best benefits in any individual sphere. If you care only about academic achievement, physical
achievements, or success in business, the best way to produce any of those, I predict, is not to
focus narrowly on only one domain (like more time for academic instruction to improve cognitive
performance). The best way is to also address social, emotional, and physical needs. If the arts
and physical activities were simply frills, they would not have arisen in every society and lasted
for thousands of years. I fear that schools are moving in exactly the wrong direction by squeezing
these out of the school day. Research in developmental science is critically needed to investigate,
with well-designed studies that permit strong causal inferences, the benefits of arts and physical
activities that are rapidly disappearing from children’s lives.
REFERENCES
Arnsten, A. F. T. (1998). The biology of being frazzled. Science,280, 1711–1712. doi:10.1126/science.280.5370.1711
Ashby, F., Isen, A., & Turken, A. (1999). A neuropsychological theory of positive affect and its influence on cognition.
Psychological Review,106, 529–550. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.106.3.529
Austin, E. J., Gibson, G. J., Deary, I. J., McGregor, M. J., & Dent, J. B. (1998). Individual response spread in
self-report scales: Personality correlations and consequences. Personality and Individual Differences,24, 421–438.
doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(97)00175-X
Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Ciarocco, N. J., & Twenge, J. M. (2005). Social exclusion impairs self-regulation.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,88, 589–604. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.88.4.589
Boucard, G. K., Albinet, C. T., Bugaiska, A., Bouquet, C. A., Clarys, D., & Audiffren, M. (2012). Impact of physical
activity on executive functions in aging: A selective effect on inhibition among old adults. Journal of Sport & Exercise
Psychology,34, 808–827.
Cacioppo, J., & Patrick, W. (2008). Loneliness: Human nature and the need for social connection. New York, NY: W. W.
Norton & Co., Inc.
Campbell, W. K., Krusemark, E. A., Dyckman, K. A., Brunell, A. B., McDowell, J. E., Twenge, J. M., & Clementz, B. A.
(2006). A magnetoencephalography investigation of neural correlates for social exclusion and self-control. Social
Neuroscience,1, 124–134. doi:10.1080/17470910601035160
Downloaded by [The University of British Columbia] at 09:51 09 November 2015
294 DIAMOND
Cerqueira, J. J., Mailliet, F., Almeida, O. F., Jay, T. M., & Sousa, N. (2007). The prefrontal cortex as a key target of the
maladaptive response to stress. Journal of Neuroscience,27, 2781–2787. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4372-06.2007
Desseilles, M., Balteau, E., Sterpenich, V., Dang-Vu, T. T., Darsaud, A., Vandewalle, G., & Schwartz, S. (2009).
Abnormal neural filtering of irrelevant visual information in depression. Journal of Neuroscience,29, 1395–1403.
doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3341-08.2009
Diamond, A. (2007). Interrelated and interdependent. Developmental Science,10, 152–158. doi:10.1111/desc.
2007.10.issue-1
Diamond, A. (2010). The evidence base for improving school outcomes by addressing the whole child and by
addressing skills and attitudes, not just content. Early Education & Development,21, 780–793. doi:10.1080/
10409289.2010.514522
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology,64, 135–168. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-
113011-143750
Diamond, A. (2014). Want to optimize executive functions and academic outcomes? Simple, just nourish the human spirit.
Minnesota Symposia on Child Psychology,37, 203–230.
Diamond, A., & Ling, D. (in press). Fundamental questions surrounding efforts to improve executive functions (includ-
ing working memory). In M. Bunting, J. Novick, M. Dougherty, & R. W. Engle (Eds.), An integrative approach to
cognitive and working memory training: Perspectives from psychology, neuroscience, and human development.New
York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Donaldson, S. I., & Grant-Vallone, E. J. (2002). Understanding self-report bias in organizational behavior research.
Journal of Business and Psychology,17, 245–260. doi:10.1023/A:1019637632584
Grol, M., Koster, E. H., Bruyneel, L., & De Raedt, R. (2014). Effects of positive mood on attention broadening for
self-related information. Psychological Research,78, 566–573.
Hirt, E. R., Devers, E. E., & McCrea, S. M. (2008). I want to be creative: Exploring the role of hedonic contingency
theory in the positive mood-cognitive flexibility link. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,94, 214–230.
doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.2.94.2.214
Huang, Y.-S., Guilleminault, C., Li, H.-Y., Yang, C.-M., Wu, Y.-Y., & Chen, N.-H. (2007). Attention-
deficit/hyperactivity disorder with obstructive sleep apnea: A treatment outcome study. Sleep Medicine,8, 18–30.
doi:10.1016/j.sleep.2006.05.016
Isen, A. M., Daubman, K. A., & Nowicki, G. P. (1987). Positive affect facilitates creative problem solving. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,52, 1122–1131. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.52.6.1122
Isen, A. M., Johnson, M. M., Mertz, E., & Robinson, G. F. (1985). The influence of positive affect on the unusualness of
word associations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,48, 1413–1426. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.48.6.1413
Ling, D., Kelly, M. K., & Diamond, A. (in press). Human-animal interaction and the development of cognitive control
(executive functions). In L. Freund, S. McCune, P. McCardle, L. Esposito, & J. A. Griffin (Eds.), Social neuroscience
of human-animal interaction. New York, NY: American Psychological Association Press.
Liston, C., McEwen, B. S., & Casey, B. J. (2009). Psychosocial stress reversibly disrupts prefrontal processing and
attentional control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,106, 912–917. doi:10.1073/pnas.0807041106
Murray, N., Sujan, H., Hirt, E. R., & Sujan, M. (1990). The influence of mood on categorization: A cognitive flexibility
interpretation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,59, 411–425. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.59.3.411
Prakash, R. S., Voss, M. W., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2015). Physical activity and cognitive vitality. Annual
Review of Psychology,66, 769–797. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015249
Scudder, M. R., Lambourne, K., Drollette, E. S., Herrmann, S. D., Washburn, R. A., Donnelly, J. E., & Hillman, C. H.
(2014). Aerobic capacity and cognitive control in elementary school-age children. Medicine & Science in Sports &
Exercise,46, 1025–1035. doi:10.1249/MSS.0000000000000199
Tangney, J. P., Baumeister, R. F., & Boone, A. L. (2004). High self-control predicts good adjustment, less pathology, better
grades, and interpersonal success. Journal of Personality,72(2), 271–324. doi:10.1111/j.0022-3506.2004.00263.x
Twenge, J. M., Catanese, K. R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2002). Social exclusion causes self-defeating behavior. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology,83, 606–615. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.3.606
Voelcker-Rehage, C., Godde, B., & Staudinger, U. M. (2010). Physical and motor fitness are both related to cognition in
old age. European Journal of Neuroscience,31(1), 167–176. doi:10.1111/ejn.2009.31.issue-1
Wells, G. L., & Olson, E. A. (2003). Eyewitness testimony. Annual Review of Psychology,54, 277–295.
doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145028
Downloaded by [The University of British Columbia] at 09:51 09 November 2015
... The first lesson was organized in November (2013 and in the form of a meeting with the authors of the books/researchers and main goals, organization and implementation of the project were described and discussed. The second lesson (February 2014 and2015) was done at midway between the beginning and the conclusion of the project and provided opportunity for collaborative discussions between teachers and researchers. In the third training lesson (May 2014 and 2015) the teachers were asked to discuss their activities in the form of oral presentation supported by ppt slides; the audience included the other teachers participating to the study. ...
... Activities in the zone of proximal development (Diamond, 2015) The teachers unanimously agreed that: a) the prevalence of activities like drawing, and others involving fine motor skill, lead to a sedentary lifestyle; b) the outdoor environment can provide very good opportunities to improve the level of physical activity; c) structured activity are important for motor development; d) teachers should support children by scaffolding; e) it is important to provide enthusiasm to children with a quiet attitude; f) that both e and f ask for important modification of their teaching behavior and programs. These considerations indicate that the teachers have reached at the end of the study a wide perception of the potential relevance of the connection between physical activities and executive functions and of the need to joint these items in a common teaching program. ...
... While some arts education experiences may be self-directed, even an apparently independent learning experience such as roaming a museum exhibit is guided by curatorial decisions and placard texts. However, many arts education experiences feature a more prominent guide in the form of a teacher or teaching artist, and in these cases that teacher's characteristics become important aspects of the immediate context of arts education (Diamond, 2015). These may include the teacher's personal characteristics (e.g., gender and ethnic identities), training (both as an artist and an educator, including access to and use of professional development), their experience (again, as an artist and educator in general, but also as arts educator in comparable settings), and their role in the institutional setting (e.g., full-time faculty, itinerant faculty, or guest artist). ...
Article
Full-text available
In this paper, we argue that in order for the study of arts education to continue to advance, we must delineate the effects of particular forms of arts education, offered in certain contexts, on specific domains of children’s socioemotional development. We explain why formulating precise hypotheses about the effects of arts education on children’s socioemotional development requires a differentiated definition of each arts education program or activity in question, as well as a consideration of both the immediate and broader contexts in which that program or activity occurs. We then offer the New Victory Theater’s Schools with Performing Arts Reach Kids (SPARK) program as an illustrative example of how these considerations allow for the refinement of hypotheses about the impact of arts education on children’s socioemotional development.
... This network can be considered an internal regulation system through which the brain controls visceromotor, neuroendocrine, and behavioral responses to environmental challenges 28) . Several studies indicate that children and adults, who participate in team sports, tend to adopt a sense of belonging and responsibility toward their community, improvement in their interpersonal and intrapersonal skills, including better verbal and nonverbal communication, and development of self-awareness for understanding their own strengths and weaknesses in addition to the ability to analyze theories and ideas 13) . Research studies have concluded that team sports help develop children and adults' complex skills required for alerting, orienting and executive attention, particularly among highly competitive and skilled teams 14) . ...
Article
Full-text available
Abstract. [Purpose] The purpose of this study to measure four components of executive function: (1) cogni- tive flexibility, (2) inhibition, (3) working memory and (4) processing speed, along with the ability to dual task in recreational athletes. [Participants and Methods] This was a cross-sectional study of (n=102) male and female par- ticipants, between the ages of 18–40 years of age across different levels and types of sport related physical activity. The International Physical Activity Questionnaire (IPAQ), short version, Dual Task Abilities (DTA) were measured utilizing a quantitative, dual task, gait test and Executive Function (EF) was measured through Stroop Color Word Test and Trail Making Test. [Results] Differences in EF and Dual Task-Interference (DTI) in recreational athletes did not show a significant difference between varying types of sport and level of sport related activity, with reported values high across all groups. Males reported better dual task interference abilities than females, though there were no significant differences in executive function between males and females. Executive function performance was the highest among the age group (18–24 years) population, but there were no significant differences between those in the higher age groups (25–34 years) and (35–40 years). [Conclusion] Overall, those participating in the study exhibited high prevalence of strong EF ability, regardless of sport activity type or level. This may suggest that type and level of sport activity may not be important when considering executive function performance maintenance for recreational athletes. Key words: Executive function, Dual task interference, Recreational athletes (This article was submitted Apr. 22, 2020, and was accepted Aug. 4, 2020)
... Martial arts are not simply a physical exercise, but also a complete body exercise and its health effects are therefore multidimensional, such as stress reduction, improved flexibility and balance, posture control and leg endurance (Bin Bu, Han Hai jun , Liu Yong, Zhang Chao hui, Yang Xiao yuan, Maria Fiata-rone Singh, 2010). Martial arts teaching gives priority to self-confidence, spiritual development, physical ability (including aesthetic form and coordination of movement) discipline, self-defense, and sometimes competition (Bin Bu, Han Hai jun, Liu Yong, Zhang Chao hui, Yang Xiao yuan, Maria Fiata-sing Singh, 2010; Adele Diamond, 2015). The martial arts advantages are not only limited to the biological sphere, but are also judged to be extremely high in spiritual and pedagogical values, and teaching practices because they help you overcome weaknesses, fears and abilities for aggression (Władysław Jagiełło, Marcin Dornowski, 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
Η εργασία αυτή έχει τίτλο: ακαδημαϊκό και επαγγελματικό υπόβαθρο των γονέων και η σχέση του με την επιλογή των πολεμικών τεχνών ως αθλητική δραστηριότητα στα παιδιά τους. Η ενασχόληση των παιδιών με τις πολεμικές τέχνες συμβάλλει στην ανάπτυξη της προσωπικότητάς τους. Οι πολεμικές τέχνες είναι αθλητικές δραστηριότητες που δεν γίνονται στο σχολείο αλλά στον ελεύθερο χρόνο τους. Αυτή η επιλογή των παιδιών συνδέεται με το κοινωνικό και πολιτιστικό περιβάλλον της οικογένειας και αποτελεί μέρος των οικογενειακών πρακτικών κοινωνικής αναπαραγωγής σύμφωνα με τον P. Bourdieu. Το παρόν έγγραφο εξετάζει την εμπλοκή των παιδιών προσχολικής και σχολικής ηλικίας με τις πολεμικές τέχνες σε σχέση με την κοινωνική τους προέλευση. Το δείγμα της έρευνας αποτελείται από 268 γονείς των οποίων τα παιδιά εμπλέκονται στις πολεμικές τέχνες στον ελεύθερο χρόνο τους. Το ερωτηματολόγιο χρησιμοποιήθηκε ως μέσο συλλογής δεδομένων. Σύμφωνα με τα αποτελέσματα της έρευνας, τα αγόρια εμπλέκονται περισσότερο στις πολεμικές τέχνες από τα κορίτσια, γεγονός που υπογραμμίζει τη συμβολή των πολεμικών τεχνών στην αναπαραγωγή των διακρίσεων λόγω φύλου. Υπήρξε μια στατιστικά σημαντική σχέση μεταξύ της επιλογής των πολεμικών τεχνών και του επαγγέλματος, του εκπαιδευτικού επιπέδου και του εισοδήματος των γονέων. Φαίνεται ότι τόσο το επάγγελμα όσο και το επίπεδο εκπαίδευσης των γονέων και το ετήσιο οικογενειακό τους εισόδημα επηρεάζουν την επιλογή των πολεμικών τεχνών ως αθλητική δραστηριότητα για τα παιδιά τους. Συμπερασματικά, αν και τα χαμηλότερα κοινωνικά στρώματα, ασχολούνταν με τις πολεμικές τέχνες, σήμερα παρατηρείται η ενασχόληση στα μεσαία και υψηλά στρώματα. Όλο και περισσότεροι μορφωμένοι γονείς, που ανήκουν στις μεσαίες και ανώτερες κοινωνικές τάξεις, κάνουν τα παιδιά τους να συμμετέχουν στις πολεμικές τέχνες, στον ελεύθερο χρόνο τους, δεδομένου ότι έχουν συνειδητοποιήσει τη σημασία των πολεμικών τεχνών και πώς βοηθούν στη δημιουργία και την ανάπτυξη μιας ολοκληρωμένης προσωπικότητας.
... Many contexts within a kindergarten classroom support development of BSR. Primarily, play is a central component of modern kindergarten programs (Ontario Ministry of Education [OME], 2016) and serves as a vehicle to support children's self-regulation (Bodrova, Germeroth, & Leong, 2013;Diamond, 2015) as children in play learn to inhibit impulsive behaviours, follow rules, and develop verbal self-regulation when they are engaged in play with peers (Savina, 2014). In addition to play, children have opportunities to engage in self-regulated behaviours in the context of small and whole group teacher-led activities as well as transitions (e.g. ...
Article
While research consistently demonstrates that behavioral self-regulation (BSR) is associated with early academic abilities, it may not yet account for the multiple ways in which young children symbolically communicate their knowledge. This study builds on previous research in the areas of early learning and self-regulation by including children’s drawings as a method of accessing and assessing young children’s learning and by exploring this relation over a two-year kindergarten program. Measures of BSR and academic skills (math, reading, writing, and drawing) were administered at the end of a two-year kindergarten program (Mage = 70.25 month) to 109 children (49 girls, 60 boys). Results from four separate hierarchical multiple regression analyses demonstrated that BSR significantly predicted academic skills (math, reading, writing, and drawing) after controlling for age and maternal education. Practical implications of these findings highlight how self-regulated behaviour in kindergarten fosters a wide range of academic development including drawing.
... For example, Yu, Mo, Tang, Huang, and Tan (2014) implemented a nursing intervention that involved family-centered nursing care along with physical, social, and psychological interventions; findings indicated improved social adaptation capability. Finally, psychosocial intervention fits with the larger goal of helping children (with cancer) attain the skills and abilities that enable them to navigate life resiliently despite the challenges they face (Diamond, 2015). ...
Article
Objectives: Many pediatric cancers are diagnosed in early childhood, a time of significant growth and development that lays the foundations for overall adjustment and functioning. The objective of this article was to characterize the psychosocial functioning of young children with cancer. Method: Data from a sample of young children with cancer ( N = 92) who completed a psychological evaluation that included the Behavior Assessment Scale for Children-second edition (BASC-2) parent report were abstracted from the medical record. Patients were primarily White (70.7%), male (54.3%), and 4.81 ± 0.89 years old at evaluation. Most were treated for brain tumors (64.1%). Results: Overall group means on each of the BASC-2 subscales were within normal limits, though significantly more patients than expected had elevated scores on the Internalizing and Behavioral Symptoms indexes. Patients who were on-treatment had higher mean overall Internalizing Problems scores, as well as greater Anxiety and Somatization scores, than those who were off-treatment (Wilks's λ = 0.75, p < .001). Patients treated for brain tumors had lower mean Activities of Daily Living scores than those with other diagnoses ( F = 15.81, p < .001). Conclusions: Findings from this clinically referred sample indicate that while most young children with cancer are doing well psychosocially, approximately 20% to 30% demonstrated difficulties in at least one area. Findings support the need for monitoring of young children with cancer as well as appropriate intervention services.
... Farrington et al., 2012;International Positive Education Network, 2017) and a host of individual researchers (e.g. Levin, 2012;Diamond, 2015;Duckworth & Yeager, 2015;Ikesako & Miyamoto, 2015;Moore et al., 2015;White, 2016;Park et al., 2017). One study has estimated that the economic benefits of investing in socioemotional learning outweigh the costs by a factor of 11:1 (i.e. ...
Article
Full-text available
Since the 1970s, schools in Ireland have offered an optional programme known as ‘Transition Year’ midway through secondary education, when students are typically 15 years old. It lasts one academic year and aims to promote maturity, social development and vocational exploration, and to give students a chance to develop personal and intellectual interests in the absence of high‐stakes examination pressure. On returning to more conventional classes, participants are expected to be better prepared to study and better prepared for life beyond school. The evolution of Transition Year over nearly half a century has been driven primarily by committed teachers and particular policy decisions, but, despite increasing uptake, it has lacked a strong theoretical grounding. This has contributed to a measure of public ambivalence and vulnerability to political vicissitudes. This paper reviews the extant literature on Transition Year, situates Transition Year in the context of selected aspects of the psychological literature on socioemotional development, highlights avenues for future research and discusses its relevance to social and emotional learning in global education beyond Ireland.
... Farrington et al., 2012;International Positive Education Network, 2017) and a host of individual researchers (e.g. Levin, 2012;Diamond, 2015;Duckworth & Yeager, 2015;Ikesako & Miyamoto, 2015;Moore et al., 2015;White, 2016;Park et al., 2017). One study has estimated that the eco- nomic benefits of investing in socioemotional learning outweigh the costs by a factor of 11:1 (i.e. ...
Article
Full-text available
This guide accompanies the following article: Clerkin, A. Filling in the gaps: A theoretical grounding for an education programme for adolescent socioemotional and vocational development in Ireland. Review of Education, https://doi.org/10.1002/rev3.3112
Chapter
This chapter addresses the stimulation of children and adolescents’ cognitive, emotional, and behavioral skills in the clinical context. Neuropsychological rehabilitation performed in the clinical context seeks to promote active changes in cognitive, behavioral, and emotional skills, aiming to improve performance in daily activities. The therapist, in turn, must have skills such as planning, organization, cognitive flexibility, creativity, and theoretical and practical knowledge to promote a neuropsychological intervention. In addition, it is important to consider patients’ individual demands and promote a safe and organized therapeutic environment. The clinical intervention work must be organized, constantly evaluated, and involve family participation. The author reports that there are challenges in the practice of neuropsychological intervention and that it is important to align expectations, involve the family, and seek neuroplasticity through meaningful experiences.
Article
Full-text available
Four experiments indicated that positive affect, induced by means of seeing a few minutes of a comedy film or by means of receiving a small bag of candy, improved performance on two tasks that are generally regarded as requiring creative ingenuity: Duncker's (1945) candle task and M. T. Mednick, S. A. Mednick, and E. V. Mednick's (1964) Remote Associates Test. One condition in which negative affect was induced and two in which subjects engaged in physical exercise (intended to represent affectless arousal) failed to produce comparable improvements in creative performance. The influence of positive affect on creativity was discussed in terms of a broader theory of the impact of positive affect on cognitive organization.
Chapter
Full-text available
Children, indeed, people of all ages, arc affected in many positive ways by interacting with pets and companion animals. Human-animal interac· tion (HAI) can be socially, emotionally, physically, and cognitively beneficial. Our particular speciality happens to be executive functions (EFs), those vitally important mental processes that form the basis for strategizing, paying attention, prioritizing competing tasks, maintaining self-control, and adjust· ing co ever-changing situations. In this chapter, we examine the potential effects of HAl on EFs, scarring with a broad overview of what EFs are and factors that influence chem. Although there is currendy little, if any, research that specifically demonstrates the link between EFs and HAI, the potential benefits of HAl for EFs hold promise for enhancing EF functioning in both children and adults. http //dx.
Chapter
Full-text available
Executive functions (EFs; which make possible mentally playing with ideas, giving the considered rather than impulsive response, resisting temptations, staying focused, problem solving, and creatively meeting unanticipated challenges) suffer if we are sad, stressed, lonely, or not physically fit. Any of those can cause us to appear to have an EF disorder when we do not. Given that EFs are critical for academic achievement, f we want students to excel we need to take seriously the fundamental interrelatedness of the different aspects of a person. If emotional, social, or physical needs are ignored, those unmet needs will work against academic excellence. The arts and physical activity can challenge EFs (e.g., by requiring focused attention and discipline) and thus improve them, and they can support EFs (and hence optimal school performance) by bringing joy and pride, building community, and whipping the body into shape. Thus promoting the arts and physical activity may be vital for improving academic outcomes. Schools can improve academic outcomes by increasing the joy and well-being of their teachers and students.
Article
Full-text available
The purposes of this study were to determine the impact of physical activity on three different executive functions (shifting, inhibition, and updating) and to examine whether cardiovascular fitness was a good mediator of the positive link(s) between these variables. Sixty-three young adults (18-28 years), 30 young-old adults (60-70 years) and 30 old adults (71-81 years) were divided into physically active and sedentary groups according to physical activity level (assessed from an accelerometer and the Historical Leisure Activity Questionnaire). Cardiovascular fitness was assessed by VO2max from the Rockport 1 mile. Each executive function was assessed through three different experimental tasks. ANCOVAs revealed that the effect of physical activity level was specific to the old adults and significant for inhibition, but not for updating and shifting. Mediation analysis showed that this positive effect in the old adults group was mediated by cardiovascular fitness level. The present findings highlight the positive linkages among physical activity, cardiovascular fitness, and inhibition in aging.
Article
Full-text available
Three experiments test the hypothesis that positive mood facilitates cognitive flexibility in categorization. Study 1 used a sorting task and found that positive mood subjects in relation to subjects in other mood states, formed fewer (broader) categories when focusing on similarities among exemplars and more (narrower) categories when focusing on differences. Study 2 used a within-subject design and assessed more direct measures of flexibility. Study 2 found that compared with neutral mood subjects, positive mood subjects (a) perceived a greater number of both similarities and differences between items, (b) accessed more distinct types of similarities and differences, and (c) listed more novel and creative similarities and differences. Study 3 demonstrated that these effects occur for both positive (mood-congruent) and neutral stimuli and identified intrinsic interest in the task as a possible mediating factor. The implications of these findings for understanding the effects of mood on cognitive organization and processing are discussed. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
We examine evidence supporting the associations among physical activity (PA), cognitive vitality, and neural functioning, and the moderation of these associations by genetic factors. Prospective epidemiological studies provide evidence for PA to be associated with a modest reduction in relative risk of cognitive decline. An evaluation of the PA-cognition link across the life span provides modest support for the effect of PA on preserving and even enhancing cognitive vitality and the associated neural circuitry in older adults; the majority of benefits seen are for tasks that are supported by the prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus. The literature on children and young adults, however, is in need of well-powered randomized controlled trials. Future directions include a more sophisticated understanding of the dose-response relationship, the integration of genetic and epigenetic approaches, inclusion of multimodal imaging of brain-behavior changes, and finally the design of multimodal interventions that may yield broader improvements in cognitive function. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Psychology Volume 66 is November 30, 2014. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/catalog/pubdates.aspx for revised estimates.
Article
The current study examined the relationship between children's performance on the Progressive Aerobic Cardiovascular Endurance Run subtest of the FitnessGram® and aspects of cognitive control that are believed to support academic success. Hierarchical linear regression analyses were conducted on a sample of second- and third-grade children (n = 397) who completed modified versions of a flanker task and spatial n-back task to assess inhibitory control and working memory, respectively. Greater aerobic fitness was significantly related to shorter reaction time and superior accuracy during the flanker task, suggesting better inhibitory control and the facilitation of attention in higher-fit children. A similar result was observed for the n-back task such that higher-fit children exhibited more accurate target detection and discrimination performance when working memory demands were increased. These findings support the positive association between aerobic fitness and multiple aspects of cognitive control in a large sample of children, using a widely implemented and reliable field estimate of aerobic capacity. Importantly, the current results suggest that this relationship is consistent across methods used to assess fitness, which may have important implications for extending this research to more representative samples of children in a variety of experimental contexts.
Article
Studies on cognitive effects of positive emotions have associated positive emotions to broadened attention. Given the widely investigated relationship between self-focused attention and mood, it is important to investigate the effect of positive mood on visuospatial attention for self-related information. We used a performance-based measure to assess fluctuations in attentional broadening from self-related contrasted to not-self-related information. In Experiment 1, we checked that the self-related versus not-self-related stimuli did not evoke differential attention effects in general. In Experiment 2, we manipulated mood and found that an increase in positive mood was associated with a relative broadening of attention for self-related information. These results suggest that the meaning of the target of attention provides an interesting dimension for further investigation into the relation between positive emotions and attentional broadening.
Article
Under stress, our brain works differently. From our own experiences we know this, but now several lines of research are defining the neurobiology behind these changes. In her commentary, Arnsten summarizes recent work that demonstrates that some kinds of memory become enhanced (such as the image of a gruesome accident), while higher order thinking, mediated by a region of the brain called the prefrontal cortex, becomes impaired.