DataPDF Available

The Mediational Role of Effortful Control and Emotional Dysregulation in the Link Between Maternal Responsiveness and Turkish Preschoolers' Social Competency and Externalizing Symptoms

Authors:

Abstract

This cross-sectional study relied on circumscribed measures of emotion regulation and dysregulation to examine their role in mediating the associations of maternal responsiveness and effortful control with social competency and externalizing symptoms. We examined those associations in an understudied cultural context, Turkey, with 118 preschoolers. Emotion regulation and dysregulation showed differential associations with broad indices of self-regulation such that emotion dysregulation predicted both low social competency and high externalizing symptoms but emotion regulation was only associated with high social competency. Effortful control was unrelated to emotion regulation but was associated with lower levels of emotion dysregulation. Effortful control had both direct and mediated associations with externalizing and social competency (mediated by lower emotion dysregulation). Findings also showed that maternal responsiveness was associated with better social competency and lower externalizing. Those associations were both singly (through effortful control) and doubly mediated (through effortful control and lower emotion dysregulation), similar to US samples. The study contributes to a better understanding of the factors and mechanisms that speak to children's self-regulation. Copyright (c) 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
The Mediational Role of Effortful
Control and Emotional
Dysregulation in the Link Between
Maternal Responsiveness and Turkish
PreschoolersSocial Competency and
Externalizing Symptoms
Irem Metin Orta
a
, Feyza Corapci
b
, Bilge Yagmurlu
a
and
Nazan Aksan
a,
*
a
Koc University, Istanbul, Turkey
b
Bogazici University, Istanbul, Turkey
This cross-sectional study relied on circumscribed measures of
emotion regulation and dysregulation to examine their role in
mediating the associations of maternal responsiveness and effort-
ful control with social competency and externalizing symptoms.
We examined those associations in an understudied cultural
context, Turkey, with 118 preschoolers. Emotion regulation and
dysregulation showed differential associations with broad indices
of self-regulation such that emotion dysregulation predicted both
low social competency and high externalizing symptoms but
emotion regulation was only associated with high social compe-
tency. Effortful control was unrelated to emotion regulation but
was associated with lower levels of emotion dysregulation.
Effortful control had both direct and mediated associations with
externalizing and social competency (mediated by lower emotion
dysregulation). Findings also showed that maternal responsive-
ness was associated with better social competency and lower exter-
nalizing. Those associations were both singly (through effortful
control) and doubly mediated (through effortful control and lower
emotion dysregulation), similar to US samples. The study contrib-
utes to a better understanding of the factors and mechanisms that
speak to childrens self-regulation. Copyright © 2013 John Wiley &
Sons, Ltd.
*Correspondence to: Nazan Aksan, Dept of Neurology, UIHC, Iowa City IA 52242.
E-mail: nazan-aksan@uiowa.edu
Infant and Child Development
Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
Published online in Wiley Online Library
(wileyonlinelibrary.com). DOI: 10.1002/icd.1806
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
Key words: emotion regulation; emotion dysregulation; effortful
control; social competency; externalizing; maternal responsiveness
Recent reviews place emotion regulation as the central mediating factor that links fa-
milial inuences and child characteristics on the antecedent side to broad indices of
self-regulation such as social competency and adjustment difculties on the outcome
side (Morris, Silk, Steinberg, Myers, & Robinson, 2007). The overarching goal of this
cross-sectional study was to rely on circumscribed measures of emotion regulation
and dysregulation in mediating the associations of maternal responsiveness and effort-
ful control with social competency and externalizing symptoms. The second broad
goal was to examine those associations in an understudied culture, Turkey, and ex-
plore if the associations fundamental to our thinking on the development of childrens
self-regulation apply to other cultural contexts.
Maternal Responsiveness
Maternal responsiveness, a sensitive, warm, supportive, and accepting orientation to
child signals (Darling & Steinberg, 1993), has shown both concurrent and longitudinal
associations with broad indices of childrens self-regulation including social compe-
tency (Eisenberg, Gershoff et al., 2001; Eisenberg et al., 2003, 2005; Spinrad et al.,
2007) and externalizing symptoms (Chang, Olson, Sameroff, & Sexton, 2010;
Eisenberg, Gershoff, et al., 2001; Eisenberg et al., 2005; Mintz, Hamre, & Hateld,
2011; Spinrad et al., 2007). However, the mechanisms that explain links between
responsiveness and those broad indices of self-regulation are poorly understood.
In a recent review, Morris et al. (2007) suggested that childrens emotional
regulatory abilities (ER)
1
may be particularly important in mediating the links
from family contextual variables to self-regulatory outcomes such as social compe-
tency and adjustment difculties. This is a particularly reasonable hypothesis for
the links from maternal responsiveness to social competency and externalizing.
Responsive parenting is linked with secure attachment (DeWolff & Van
Ijzendoorn, 1997). We would expect security to foster the development of inner
resources to cope effectively with negative emotions and experiences when they
arise, down-regulating and modulating difcult emotional experiences appropri-
ately during social interactions with family, peers, and teachers (Sroufe, 1995).
Conversely, low maternal responsiveness may expose children to an environment
that fails to provide support to deal with negative experiences, leading to greater
frustration, acting out, and adjustment difculties (Power, 2004). Responsive par-
enting also models emotional competencies such as reading social partnersaffec-
tive cues accurately and responding empathetically, critical to development of
social competencies (Eisenberg et al., 1996).
Evidence supports associations between maternal responsiveness and ER skills
generally. For example, mothersprompt and appropriate responses to infantscrying
promotes infantsproximity and contact seeking, and decreases crying (Bell & Ains-
worth, 1972; Fish, Stifter, & Belsky, 1991), and maternal responsiveness has been
associated with good cardiac vagal tone, a biological marker of ER (Haley & Stansbury,
2003). In older children, responsiveness to distress is associated with better regulation
of negative emotions (Davidov & Grusec, 2006).
Studies have also shown that effortful control, the ability to inhibit a dominant
response to perform a subdominant response(Rothbart & Bates, 2006, p. 129),
mediates the effects of maternal responsiveness on greater social competency and
lower externalizing (Eisenberg, Gershoff et al., 2001; Eisenberg et al., 2005; Spinrad
et al., 2007). In this framework, effortful control is viewed as an inner resource,
I.M. Orta et al.
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
enabling the focusing and shifting of attention, inhibiting or activating behaviours in
situationally appropriate ways that should assist ER skills (Derryberry & Rothbart,
1997; Rothbart & Sheese, 2007; Zelazo & Cunningham, 2007). Consistent with that
hypothesis, observed effortful control predicts childrens ability to mask negative affect
in accordance with display rules in the disappointing gift paradigm (Carlson & Wang,
2007; Kieras, Tobin, Graziano, & Rothbart, 2005). However, effortful control measures
do not involve emotional responses. Hence, we do not know whether effortful
controls associations with better social competency and lower externalizing are in fact
mediated through better ER skills.
One of the central goals of the current study was to rely on circumscribed measures
of ER and effortful control, and test whether associations of maternal responsiveness
with better social competency and lower adjustment difculties were mediated by
effortful control, which may in turn facilitate better emotion regulation and lower
emotion dysregulation. Figure 1 depicts the hypothesized links from maternal respon-
siveness to social competency and externalizing. Following a delineation of our
conceptualization of ER, we will summarize evidence supporting hypothesized links.
Emotion Regulation
Despite being an intense focus of attention, the construct of ER continues to present
difculties in both denition and measurement (Cole, Martin, & Dennis, 2004; Gross
& Thompson, 2007; Thompson & Meyer, 2007). Most adopt the following denition:
emotion regulation consists of intrinsic and extrinsic processes responsible for
monitoring, evaluating, and modifying emotional reactions, especially their intensive
and temporal features to accomplish onesgoals(Thompson, 1994, pp. 2728). The
measurement of extrinsic sources of inuence such as guidance by parents is not
problematic (e.g. Fox & Calkins, 2003; Gottman, Katz, & Hooven, 1996; Kopp, 1989).
In contrast, measurement of intrinsic sources of inuence has been debated in many
special sections (e.g. Cole et al., 2004; Tamir, 2011). A consensus that emerges from
special sections devoted to ER in recent years is that it may not be possible to
differentiate ER from emotional reactivity or the emotion process itself (Calkins,
2010; Goldsmith, Pollak, & Davidson, 2008; Rothbart & Sheese, 2007; Thompson,
2011). This consensus leads some to reject the notion of ER in favour of emotional
reactivity (Lewis, Zinbarg, & Durbin, 2010) and leads others to view emotional
reactivity and its regulation as part of a dynamic system that unfolds (Campos, Walle,
Dahl, & Main, 2011; Rothbart & Sheese, 2007; Thompson, 2011).
In this study, we adopt the latter perspective that reactivity in an emotional system
such as anger or fear, including its latency, duration, and intensity, simultaneously
reects both the generation and the regulation of an emotional response. Consistent
with this perspective, we adopt the view that emotion dysregulation results when
emotional responses, both positive and negative, have high intensity, rapid rise, and
Figure 1. The hypothesized associations among maternal responsiveness, effortful control,
emotion regulation, social competency, and externalizing.
Emotion Regulation
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
arise in a number of situations that they fail to meet contextual demands and/or
expectations (Campos et al., 2011; Cole et al., 2004; Dunsmore, Booker, & Ollendick,
2012). In contrast, emotion regulation results when emotional responses, both pos-
itive and negative, are contextually appropriate and well-modulated in intensity.
Also consistent with this dynamic process perspective, the relative rank-order of a
child on a temperament scale reects individual differences in both that childs
emotional reactivity and ER. However, temperament instruments do not direct
the rater to evaluate whether the response is appropriate and ts with the
prevailing contextual demands and expectations. In this sense, measures of
temperamental emotional reactivity are ill-suited measures of ER.
In contrast, the item content of Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC; Shields &
Cicchetti, 1997) directs the rater to evaluate the appropriateness of the emotional
response given contextual demands. We relied on ERC to measure childrensERand
selected only the subset of items that asked raters to explicitly evaluate emotional
reactions along desirability or appropriateness given contextual demands and expec-
tations. Emotion dysregulation items capture contextually inappropriate expressions
of negative and positive emotions (e.g. taking pleasure from anothersplightand
displaying negative emotions while trying to include others in play) and inability to
modulate emotional intensity (e.g. being disruptive in expressions of enthusiasm
and having temper tantrums for minor disappointments). Emotion regulation items
capture the ability to easily transition from one activity to another, easily recovering
from negative emotions, reading friendly/neutral approaches of adults and peers
accurately, and responding positively, in addition to emotional awareness.
Previous research has shown ERCs emotion regulation and dysregulation
subscales to be moderately correlated. Some have formed a composite considering
regulation and dysregulation as ends of a continuum (e.g. Batum & Yagmurlu, 2007;
Ramsden & Hubbard, 2002; Yagmurlu & Altan, 2010), whereas others have noted their
distinct associations with predictors and outcomes (Dunsmore et al., 2012; Lukenheimer,
Shields, & Cortina, 2007; Miller et al., 2006; Shields & Cicchetti, 1997, 1998). For
example, emotion knowledge predicted better emotion regulation but was unrelated
to emotion dysregulation (Miller et al., 2006). Emotion coaching, a specicfacetof
positive parenting (Gottman et al., 1996), was associated with better emotion
regulation skills but not lower emotion dysregulation (Dunsmore et al., 2012). In this
same study, emotion regulation predicted adaptive skills including positive peer
relations but not adjustment difculties, whereas emotion dysregulation was
associated with both adjustment difculties and adaptive skills.
Those ndings are consistent with the view that reactivity and regulation are part of
a dynamic process such that a child low in emotion dysregulation need not be high in
emotion regulation, as low standing emotion dysregulation could reect generally low
reactivity in emotions and hence less demand to modulate and alter the experience of
emotions, that is, less risk for dysregulation (e.g. Eisenberg, Cumberland, Spinrad,
Fabes, Shepard, Reiser et al., 2001; Eisenberg & Fabes, 2006). In addition, those
ndings raise the possibility that effects of maternal responsiveness on emotion regu-
lation and dysregulation may be distinct. Responsive mothers may foster resources
such as effortful control (e.g. Kochanska & Knaack, 2003) that help their children
down-regulate emotional experiences effectively, lowering emotion dysregulation
and in turn fostering greater competency and lower externalizing. However, respon-
sive parents may not at the same time adopt a proactive attitude such as that found
in emotion coaching to foster explicit emotional awareness, direct instruction about
emotion language, and coping strategies (Gottman et al., 1996) that are shown to foster
emotion regulation (Dunsmore et al., 2012). Once emotions are down-regulated appro-
priately and/or responsive parenting renders the environment predictable enough to
I.M. Orta et al.
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
lower stress exposure and emotional reactivity (Power, 2004), proactive strategies such
as that found in emotion coaching may not be necessary.
On the basis of previous evidence, we would also expect regulation and
dysregulation to have distinct associations with social competency and adjustment
(Dunsmore et al., 2012; Graziano, Keane, & Calkins, 2007). Social competency, skills
and abilities that enable effective social interaction, including ability to cooperate
and function well in peer groups (Rose-Krasnor, 1997), subsumes effective and efcient
ER (Halberstadt, Denham, & Dunsmore, 2011; Saarni, 1990). A socially competent
child needs to read nonverbal and often affective cues of his or her peers accurately
while managing his or her own emotional arousal to cooperate and function effectively
in the peer group. Children who are both low in emotion dysregulation and high in
emotion regulation are likely to meet those dual challenges and function effectively
in peer groups (Eisenberg & Fabes, 2006).
In contrast, emotion dysregulation or failure to regulate emotional reactivity is often
acriticalaspectofmanychildbehaviourproblems. High reactivity in the fear system is
often synonymous with anxiety disorders (Lewis et al., 2010). Poorly regulated
reactivity in anger often results in a constellation of behaviours that includes reactive
aggression, disruptive behaviour, deance, and impulsivity, a central symptom cluster
in externalizing behaviour problems (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). Not
surprisingly, emotion dysregulation has been linked with higher externalizing symp-
toms (e.g. Batum & Yagmurlu, 2007; Blandon, Calkins, Grimm, Keane, & OBrien,
2010; Kim & Cicchetti, 2010). Hence, in this study, we examined regulation and
dysregulation as overlapping but distinct aspects of ER in relation to maternal respon-
siveness and effortful control on the predictor side and social competency and
externalizing on the outcome side.
Culture
The majority of evidence supporting maternal responsiveness in an emotion-regula-
tion and self-regulation promoting role is based on Western samples. The few studies
from non-Westernsociocultural contexts suggest that maternal responsiveness
predicts attachment security in Chile (Valenzuela, 1997), Colombia (Posada et al.,
2002) and Turkey (Sumer, Selcuk, Gunaydın, Salman, & Harma, 2008) and that the
magnitude of associations is similar to Western samples (DeWolff & van Ijzendoorn,
1997). Evidence also suggests that maternal responsiveness may have similar bivariate
associations with broad measures of childrens self-regulation in non-Western contexts.
Keller and colleagues showed that maternal responsiveness in infancy predicted
toddler age compliance in Cameroon, Costa Rica, and Greece (Keller et al., 2004),
and self-reported maternal responsiveness predicted Turkish preschoolersemotion
regulation (Yagmurlu & Altan, 2010).
It is possible that cultural differences are more subtle and evident in the mechanisms
that link parental factors with childrens self-regulation. In US samples, effortful
control mediates the effects of maternal responsiveness/warmth on social competency
and low externalizing (Eisenberg, Gershoff et al., 2001; Eisenberg et al., 2005; Spinrad
et al., 2007). Effortful control has also been examined as a mediator of the inuence of
parenting practices on childrenssocialcompetencyandexternalizinginnon-US
samples including China, Indonesia, and France(Eisenberg,Chang,Ma,&Huang,
2009; Eisenberg, Liew & Pidada, 2001; Eisenberg, Pidada & Liew, 2001; Hofer,
Eisenberg, & Reiser, 2010; Zhou, Eisenberg, Wang, & Reiser, 2004). While those studies
have generally supported a mediating role for effortful control, parenting dimensions
included parenting styles (Eisenberg et al., 2009; Zhou et al., 2004), emotion
Emotion Regulation
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
expressiveness (Eisenberg, et al., 2001), and broadly conceived positive and negative
parenting practices (Hofer et al., 2010). Because those studies vary in both studied
dimensions of parenting and child ages, it is difcult to judge similarity in the
processes that link maternal responsiveness with self-regulation in US versus non-
US samples. In this study, we asked whether the associations of observed maternal
responsiveness and effortful control with better competency and lower externalizing
in Turkish preschoolers were mediated by emotion regulation and dysregulation.
Present Study
In this study, we examined the associations of observed effortful control with teacher
reported social competency and externalizing, and tested the extent to which those
associations could be accounted by effortful controls ER promoting role. Figure 1
depicts the hypothesized associations we tested using concurrent data in a sample of
Turkish preschoolers. Previous evidence has not examined the associations depicted
in Figure 1 to permit a disambiguated understanding of how effortful control
promotes broad indices of self-regulation such as low externalizing and high social
competency, and whether those associations arise from effortful controlsnarrowly
ER promoting role (Morris et al., 2007).
We also evaluated the possibility of both singly and doubly mediated links from
maternal responsiveness to childrens social competency and externalizing (as
depicted in Figure 1). In the single mediation case, associations of maternal responsive-
ness with social competency and externalizing may reect its role in promoting
childrens effortful control similar to patterns found in US samples. In the double
mediation case, maternal responsiveness may be associated with better effortful
control, which in turn predicts better ER, and better ER may in turn be associated with
better adjustment (e.g. Morris et al., 2007). Double mediation has been supported in
samples of French adolescentspeer relationships (Hofer et al., 2010). Alternatively,
effortful control may also moderate the effects of maternal responsiveness on self-reg-
ulation (not depicted in Figure 1). Responsiveness may be particularly relevant to
promoting ER for children low in effortful control.
Finally, because the associations were examined in an understudied culture, we can
gauge the extent to which common assumptions about the roles of central constructs in
human development generalize across cultural contexts. Those questions were
addressed with a cross-sectional sample relying on multiple methods and raters. We
relied on extensive observations to capture childrens effortful control and maternal
responsiveness and teacher reports of ER, social competency, and externalizing
symptoms.
METHOD
Participants
The participants included 118 Turkish preschoolers (68 boys), their mothers, and
preschool teachers. Children were recruited from three private daycare centres and
kindergartens in Istanbul. The mean age for children was 4 years 6months (SD = .92).
Mothersmean age was 36 years (SD = 3.59; ranging from 27 to 44 years), 33% were
homemakers, and the remaining 67% worked full or part-time. Fathersmean age
was 40 years (SD =5.71; ranging from 31 to 71 years), and majority (99%) worked
full-time. Both parents were highly educated; 76% of the mothers and 82% of the
fathers had college degree or higher. Majority of the children (89%) came from intact
I.M. Orta et al.
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
families. Monthly gross family income (converted to US dollars) was distributed as
follows: 18% less than 2000 USD, 42% between 2000 and 4447 USD, and 40% more
than 4447 USD. Overall, the families represented middle-income to high-income
backgrounds.
Overview of Procedure
Mothers and their children were observed during a 2
1/2
to 3-h session in a research
laboratory decorated like a typical living room. Maternal responsiveness was observed
during six dyadic interactive contexts that lasted about an hour cumulatively.
Childrens effortful control was also measured with standard observational batteries.
The sessions were conducted by a female experimenter (E) and videotaped from
behind a one-way mirror for later coding. Inter-rater reliability was established on
20% of the cases, and the coders periodically aligned to prevent drift. Finally, preschool
teachers rated childrens ER, social competency, and externalizing symptoms. The
ratings were provided by teachers who had interacted with the child for at least
3months.
Measures
Maternal responsiveness
Procedure. Following previous research (Kochanska & Aksan, 2004), maternal re-
sponsiveness was observed in six everyday contexts, cumulatively lasting 56min: (i)
warm up/adaptation to the room (6 min); (ii) mother-is-busy episode (i.e. completing
a packet of questionnaires, 15 min); (iii) snack time (10 min); (iv) free play (5 min); (v)
clean-up time (10 min); and (vi) teaching task (10 min).
Coding and reliability. The macroscopic coding system adapted from Ainsworth, Bell,
and Staytons (1971) by Kochanska (1998) was used in this study. Maternal responsive-
ness was dened along sensitivityinsensitivity, cooperationinterference, and accep-
tancerejection dimensions. Sensitivity describes how aware the parent is of the
childs needs/feelings/whereabouts, and how promptly and appropriately the parent
responds to the childs cues or signals. Cooperation describes parentsrespectforthe
child as an autonomous individual, appropriate to the circumstances. Acceptance
describes how genuine versus perfunctory a parents enjoyment and interest is when
interacting with the child. On the basis of previous work (Kochanska, 1998), two
coders rated maternal behaviours along these three dimensions on a 7-point scale
separately in each of six dyadic contexts. Intraclass correlations among coders ranged
from .65 to .85. The coders who rated maternal responsiveness were different from
those that rated childrenseffortfulcontrol.
Data reduction. A composite score of maternal responsiveness was formed relying on
previous work (Kochanska & Aksan, 2004). First, scores were averaged across the
dyadic contexts to form overall sensitivity, acceptance, and cooperation composites.
Next, those overall scores for sensitivity, acceptance, and cooperation were standard-
izedandaveragedtoformaresponsiveness composite,alpha =.81.
Effortful control
Procedure. Six tasks that measure ability to slow down motor activity (Walk-a-Line
and TurtleRabbit), to delay (Snack Delay and Gift), and to suppress a dominant re-
sponse in favour of a subdominant response (BearDragon and DayNight) were used
to assess inhibitory control (Diamond & Taylor, 1996; Kochanska, Murray, & Harran,
2000; Kochanska, Murray, Jacques, Koenig, & Vandegeest, 1996). Walk-a-Line required
Emotion Regulation
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
the child to walk down a line afxed to the oor as slowly as possible for two trials.
TurtleRabbit required the child to draw a straight line either as slowly as a turtle or
as fast as a rabbit. Snack Delay required the child to keep hands on a mat and wait
for the bell ring in order to eat an M&M placed under a transparent cup for six trials
with delays ranging from 10 to 40 s. Gift task required the child to sit with his or her
back toward the E and not peek while a gift was being wrapped for 60 s in the rst
phase, and in the second phase, the child was asked to wait seated for the bow without
touching the wrapped gift left within armsreach for 3 min. DayNight required the
child to point to a moon picture on Esdayprompts and point to a sun picture on
Esnightprompts. BearDragon required the child to carry out commands of the
friendly bear hand puppet (e.g. touch your nose) but suppress acting on the com-
mands of the mean dragon hand puppet (six trials for each puppet).
Measures of childrens attentional regulation were obtained from two contexts: an
M&M search task (Anderson et al., 2007) and attentional persistence during the
administration of a standardized receptive language test (Berument & Güven, 2010).
During the M&M search task, the child was asked to search for a hidden M&M in a
miniature doll house for three 90-s trials while suppressing the temptation to play with
the gures. During the receptive language test, the child was presented with words
and asked to pick the picture that best corresponded to the word.
Coding and reliability. In Walk-a-Line and TurtleRabbit tasks, the time to complete
slow and fast trials was recorded. In Snack Delay task, trials were coded on a 4-point
Likert scale (1 = eats candy before the bell is lifted; 2= reaches for the candy but the bell
ringsbefores/hecaneatit;3=removeshandsoffthemat,touchesplatecupbutdoesnt
eat M&M; 4 =keeps his or her hands on the mat as asked and waits for the bell). In ad-
dition, for each trial, latencies for dgeting were noted. In the wrapping phase of the
Gift task, childs peeking was coded with a scale from 0 to 4 (0= fully peeks and does
not turn around; 1 =peeks but turns away around; 2 = peeks over the shoulder;
3 =turns head less than 90 degrees toward the present; 4 = does not try to peek), and
latency to each peek score was noted. In the bow phase of the Gift task, touching was
codedfrom1to4(1=opensgift;2=liftsboxbut does not open gift; 3 = touches but does
not lift gift; 4 = does not touch); the latency to each touch score and latency to leave seat
were noted. DayNight task was coded with a scale from 0 to 3 (0 = fails to point, 1 =
incorrect response 2 = self-corrects, 3 = correct response). The dragon trials were coded
with a scale from 0 to 3 (0 = performs full movement/does not inhibit behaviour,
1 =performs a different movement than indicated; 2 = self-corrects; 3 = does not
move/fully inhibits behaviour). The Kappa for categorical scales ranged from .89 to
.91, and intraclass correlations for latency and duration scores ranged from .84 to .99.
All of the inhibitory control episodes were coded by two coders.
Each 5-s epoch of the M&M search task was coded for the presence/absence of fo-
cused attention as well as positive affect. Kappas ranged from .80 to .95. During the
receptive vocabulary test, the total number of items, total number of correct items,
and total duration of the test from beginning to completion were noted. Two coders,
different from those who rated inhibitory control, rated attention focusing episodes.
Data reduction. In Walk-a-Line task, scores of the two slow trials were averaged. In
TurtleRabbit task, time for rabbit trial was subtracted from time for turtle trial. In
Snack Delay, trial scores were averaged, and in Gift task, all latency, touch, and peek
scores were z-transformed and averaged. In DayNight task, scores were summed
across 10 trials, and in BearDragon task, scores for the Dragon trials were summed.
Those raw scores from each of six tasks were z-transformed and averaged to form
inhibitory control composite, alpha = .71.
TherelativefrequencyoffocusedattentionontheM&Msearchtaskwithoutany
explicit displays of discrete positive affect was pooled together with latency to
I.M. Orta et al.
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
disengage attention from the search, and the relative frequency of off-task behaviour
(reversed) into a focused attention composite,alpha =.76. The pace with which children
completed the items in the receptive vocabulary test (attentional persistence) was com-
puted by dividing the total number of items to the total number of minutes elapsed.
This ratio score was not correlated with the number of correct responses (receptive
vocabulary) on the test, r(110) = .04, ns. The attentional persistence ratio score during
the receptive vocabulary test and the focused attention score from the M&M search
task were positively correlated, r(109) = .33, p<.001. Thus, the latter two scores were
standardized and pooled into a composite score of attention focusing.
The inhibitory control composite showed moderate correlations with attention
focusing composite, r(118) = .41, p<.001. The two scores were averaged to form
a composite measure of observed effortful control.
Emotion regulation
The ERC (Shields & Cicchetti, 1997) was used to measure the extent of childrens
emotion regulation and dysregulation. This 24-item checklist was rated by preschool
teachersonaLikertscalefrom1(never)to4(always).TheoriginalformoftheERC
(Shields & Cicchetti, 1997, 1998) and its parent and teacher forms in Turkish version
have been shown to have high internal consistency and discriminant validity in previ-
ous research (Batum & Yagmurlu, 2007; Yagmurlu & Altan, 2010). To better reect ER
as dened in this study, we examined ERC item content and removed all items that did
not ask the rater to evaluate the appropriateness of the emotional response given
contextual demands/expectations. This process eliminated items such as is a cheerful
childand items tapping impulsivity and abilitytodelay.Itemstappingemotional
reactions that were inadequately modulated, which failed to meet contextual
expectations, were averaged into Emotion Dysregulation subscale,alpha =.71.
2
Items
tapping emotional reactions that were socially and contextually appropriate including
emotional awareness, ability to transition between activities easily, and soothability
were averaged and pooled into Emotion Regulation subscale,alpha =.72.Thetwo
subscales were moderately negatively intercorrelated in our sample (r=.35, p<.01).
Social competency and externalizing symptoms
Preschool teachers also completed two subscales of the Social Competence
Behavior EvaluationPreschool Edition, Short Form (LaFreniere & Dumas,
1996). This instrument has been translated into and validated in Turkish samples
(Corapcı, Aksan, Arslan-Yalcin, & Yagmurlu, 2010). Ten items of the Social Compe-
tency subscale showed satisfactory reliability, alpha = .85. Two items tapping reac-
tivity to anger (e.g. quick to get angry and gets angry when asked to switch to
another activity) were removed from the AngerAggressiveness subscale to
prevent articially inated correlations between ERC items and externalizing.
The remaining eight items tapping peer bullying, oppositional conduct toward
adults, physical aggression, and frequent disagreements with peers were averaged
to form Externalizing subscale,alpha =.78.
Statistical Analyses
The analyses proceeded in three stages. First, intercorrelations were examined
among all measures in the study. Second, hierarchical regression analyses were
conducted to test whether effortful control acted as a moderator of the associations
of maternal responsiveness with ER subscales, social competency, and externaliz-
ing. If moderation effects were present, we would modify Figure 1 to include the
Emotion Regulation
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
interaction term between effortful control and maternal responsiveness. Third, we
relied on observed variable structural models with maximum likelihood to test the
hypothesized links in Figure 1. Models were parameterized to permit nested com-
parisons in t associated with the specic constraints of focus. Model tting began
with a highly constrained baseline, null model, in which only the following two
correlations were freely estimated: the intercorrelation between social competency
and externalizing, and the intercorrelation between emotion regulation and
dysregulation subscales. As recommended by Judd and Kenny (1981), models
with several mediated links among variables, as Figure 1, are tested starting from
the most endogenous end of the model. We progressively introduced the hypoth-
esized links in a sequence of nested models.
Asymptotic variancecovariances were submitted to LISREL 8 (Jöreskog & Sörbom,
1993) with robust maximum likelihood estimation (RML). The adequacy of overall
model t was examined with both SatorraBentler scaling and ordinary ML chi-square
in addition to overall model t indices (Satorra & Bentler, 2001). SatorraBentler scal-
ing permits robust ML estimation when normal-theory assumptions of chi-square
are probably violated. There are several ways in which model t indices can be classi-
ed. Here, we adopted Kaplans framework (Kaplan, 2000) and chose one index from
each of three classes of alternatives to model chi-square. Those indices were as follows:
root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and its 90% condence interval
(CI), expected cross-validation index (ECVI) and its 90% CI, and the comparative tin-
dex (CFI). When the model is well-specied in the population of interest, we expect
RMSEA to range from .05 to .08 and CFI to take on values .90 and higher. The values
for the ECVI are evaluated relative to the value this index takes for the saturated model
that necessarily has a perfect t. When the value of the ECVI in the target model is
lower relative to its value for the saturated model, we have greater condence in our
ability to validate the results in an independent sample of the same size. In addition,
we examined a summary index of standardized residuals, standard root mean resid-
ual. In well-tting models, we expect this index to take on values around .08. All indi-
rect effects from the nal model with adequate t were examined with empirical
bootstrapping (Hayes, 2012; Preacher & Hayes, 2004, 2008).
RESULTS
Intercorrelations
Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics and the intercorrelations among the study
variables. The descriptive statistics for all variables indicate adequate variability in
the sample. Distributions were generally normal, and no outliers were identied.
Table 1 shows that children were relatively high in regulating their emotions and social
competency but low in emotion dysregulation and externalizing symptoms. We also
examined sex differences in all the measures with independent sample t-tests.
Those tests showed signicant differences in observed maternal responsiveness,
t(116) = 2.74, p<.05, and social competency, t(103) = 2.05, p<.05. Girls were
higher in social competency (M=5.06, SD =.58) than boys (M= 4.71, SD =.97),
and girls (M=.28,SD = .71) were observed to have more responsive mothers than
boys (M=.20, SD = 1.07).
Bivariate correlations showed that effortful control was moderately and positively
correlated with maternal responsiveness, indicating that responsive mothers tended
to have children with high effortful control scores. Teacher ratings of emotion regula-
tion and dysregulation were moderately negatively correlated. Emotion regulation
showed strong correlations with social competency and moderate correlations with
I.M. Orta et al.
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
externalizing. In contrast, emotion dysregulation showed strong correlations with
externalizing and moderate correlations with social competency, observer ratings of
effortful control, and maternal responsiveness.
Hierarchical Regression Analyses
Hierarchical regression analyses were performed to test whether effortful control
acted as a moderator of the associations of maternal responsiveness with ER sub-
scales, social competency, and externalizing. We controlled for child age and sex in
the rst step in all four regressions. None of those hierarchical regressions
supported a signicant effect for the interaction between maternal responsiveness
and effortful control, b=.02, ns for emotion regulation, b=.09, ns for emotion
dysregulation, b=.05, ns for social competency, and b= .02, ns for externalizing.
This is not surprising in that the two variables were moderately intercorrelated as
shown in Table 1, making it highly difcult to uncover a moderation effect with
the current sample size.
Model Fitting Analyses
Table 2 presents the overall t statistics associated with each of the models. We rst ta
baseline, null model that only included the correlation between social competency and
externalizing, and the correlation between the two ERC subscales.
3
The overall tof
this model was highly inadequate. Model 1 imposed the constraint that emotion reg-
ulation and dysregulation had differential effects on childrens social competency
andexternalizing.Allthreepathsweresignicant, and their addition improved over-
all model t, Δw
2
(3) = 99.25, p<.001 and Δw
2
sb
(3) = 15.13, p<.001. The standardized re-
sidual and modication index associated with the xed path from emotion regulation
to externalizing was not signicant, supporting the hypothesis that emotion regulation
and dysregulation have distinctive associations with childrens social competency and
externalizing. Model 2 tested two additional hypotheses implied in Figure 1. The rst
hypothesis was that effortful control has direct effects on both emotion regulation and
dysregulation, and the second hypothesis was that effortful control had no direct
Table 1. Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations among the measures
12 34567
1. Age
2. Effortful control .50**
3. Maternal responsiveness .10 .40**
T ratings on the ERC
4. Emotion dysregulation .04 .25** .28**
5. Emotion regulation .09 .01 .01 .35**
T ratings on SCBE
6. Social competency .10 .31** .26** .49** .61**
7. Externalizing .39** .36** .15 .60** .25** .47**
Mean 4.52 .02 .00 1.64 3.35 4.84 1.59
SD .94 .79 .96 .41 .43 .85 .56
Min 2.19 3.84 3.64 1.0 2.22 2.10 1
Max 6.19 1.10 1.45 2.78 4.0 6.0 3
ERC = Emotion Regulation Checklist; SCBE = Social Competence Behavior Evaluation.
Note.
+
p<.10; *p<.05; **p<.01 or better. Minimum N= 105.
Emotion Regulation
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
effects on social competency and externalizing; that is, its effects on the latter two
variables were fully mediated by ER. The addition of the two paths from effortful con-
trol to emotion regulation and dysregulation improved overall model t, Δw
2
(2) = 7.48,
p<.01 and Δw
2
sb
(2) = 13.61, p<.001; however, the path from effortful control to
emotion regulation was not signicant; thus, it was removed. The overall model t
statistics when the path from effortful control to emotion regulation subscale was xed
are shown in Model 2a. The standardized residuals and the modication indices
associated with the constraint that effortful control had no direct effects on social
competency (i.e. full mediation) were elevated. This elevation indicates that partial
mediation was appropriate only in the case of social competency. Model 3 includes the sig-
nicant direct effect of effortful control on social competency, and the addition of this path
improved tsignicantly over Model 2a, Δw
2
(1) = 7.57, p<.01 and Δw
2
sb
(2) = 3.9, p<.05.
Model 4 included the path from maternal responsiveness to effortful control
but, consistent with full double mediation, constrained the paths from maternal
responsiveness to ERC outcomes, social competency, and externalizing. The path
from maternal responsiveness to effortful control was signicant. The nested
model comparisons between Models 4 and 3 when relying on normal-theory chi-
square was signicant, Δw
2
(1) = 15.29, p<.001 and marginally signicant when
relying on SatorraBentler scaling, Δw
2
sb
(1) = 3.17, p<.078. Examination of several
aspects of overall and component t led us to accept Model 4 as the nal model.
Those were as follows: (i) robust ML estimate for the path from maternal
responsiveness to effortful control was signicant; (ii) overall t of Model 4 using
both normal-theory chi-square and SatorraBentler scaling was adequate; and (iii)
component t indices (standardized residuals and modication indices) from
Model 4 were better than those from Model 3. Finally, considering partial
mediation (freeing paths from maternal responsiveness to emotion dysregulation,
social competency, and/or externalizing) led to overtting, for example chi-square
values less than model df (Bollen, 1989). Together, those ndings indicate that
evidence for partially mediated effects of maternal responsiveness on endogenous
variables was weak in this sample. Hence, we accepted Model 4 as the best-tting
model, depicted in Figure 2.
Indirect effects
In order to estimate the indirect effects implied in the best-tting Model 4, depicted
in Figure 2, we relied on empirical bootstrapping (Hayes, 2012; Preacher & Hayes,
Table 2. Overall model t statistics
Model df w
2
pw
2
sb
p
sb
RMSEA
90% CI
ECV
90% CI CFI sRMR
Baseline
model
13 141.53 .000 57.83 .000 .14.23 .52.98 .76 .28
Model 1 10 42.28 .000 34.83 .000 .10.21 .41.76 .86 .16
Model 2 8 34.80 .000 25.23 .002 .08.21 .39.68 .91 .13
Model 2a 9 35.04 .000 26.61 .002 .079.20 .38.68 .90 .13
Model 3 8 27.47 .006 18.53 .018 .045.18 .35.59 .94 .12
Model 4 7 12.18 .095 12.10 .097 .00.16 .34.52 .97 .066
w
2
sb
= SatorraBentler scaling of the overall chi-square; p
sb
=p-value associated with SatorraBentler scal-
ing of the overall chi-square. RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation; CI = condence inter-
val; CFI = comparative t index; sRMR = standardized root mean square residual. Note. Saturated
model expected cross-validation index = .41.
I.M. Orta et al.
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
2004, 2008). The PROCESS module written and available for download from Hayes
website was used to estimate the indirect effects, bootsrapped standard errors, and
bias corrected 95% CIs. Table 3 presents the indirect effect estimates for each path that
map to the focused hypotheses of interest in the nal best-tting Model 4.
AscanbeseenfromTable3,effortfulcontrols indirect effects on both social
competency and externalizing through lower levels of emotion dysregulation were sig-
nicant. In addition, all singly and doubly mediated effects of maternal responsiveness
on social competency and externalizing were signicant. Maternal responsiveness was
associated with increased social competency and lower externalizing through its
positive association with childrens effortful control. Its mediated effects on lower exter-
nalizing can be attributed to ER promoting role of effortful control (double mediation).
DISCUSSION
We relied on circumscribed measures of emotion regulation and emotion
dysregulation to test whether these ER skills mediated the associations of maternal
responsiveness and childrens effortful control on social competency and externalizing
symptoms in an understudied cultural context, Turkey. Our overarching goal was to
disambiguate the meaning of previously reported associations among overlapping
but distinct dimensions of self-regulation by relying on circumscribed measures of
ER. For example, we tested the extent to which effortful controlsassociationwith
social competency and externalizing reected its hypothesized role in promoting ER
and whether the associations of maternal responsiveness with social competency
and externalizing were mediated by effortful control. Furthermore, because we tested
those associations in an understudied culture, we informed the extent to which associ-
ations fundamental to our thinking on the development of childrensself-regulation
apply to other cultural contexts.
Observed maternal responsiveness showed signicant associations with observed
effortful control, teacher ratings of childrens emotion dysregulation, and social
competency in the expected direction. Those results extend earlier ndings to show
that observed maternal responsiveness is negatively associated with circumscribed
measures of emotion dysregulation (e.g. Eisenberg, Gershoff et al., 2001; Eisenberg
Figure 2. The nal best-tting model, robust maximum likelihood estimates of the direct effects.
SCBE = Social Competence Behavior Evaluation.
Emotion Regulation
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
Table 3. The indirect effects, standard errors obtained from empirical bootstrapping, and bias corrected 95% condence intervals for the effects of
maternal responsiveness and effortful control on emotion dysregulation, social competency, and externalizing
Indirect effect (SE) 95% CI
Path specication LL UL
1. Effortful control !Emotion dysregulation !Social competency .109 (.050) .042 .240
2. Effortful control !Emotion dysregulation !Externalizing .163 (.062) .318 .064
3. M responsiveness !Effortful control !Emotion dysregulation .024 (.015) .067 .003
4. M responsiveness !Effortful control !Social competency .174 (.064) .073 .324
5. M responsiveness !Effortful control !Emotion dysregulation !Social competency .028 (.018) .005 .084
6. M responsiveness !Effortful control !Emotion dysregulation !Externalizing .044 (.274) .121 .006
Note. The values were obtained from PROCESS module to generate empirically bootstrapped standard errors and bias corrected 95% condence intervals relying on
5000 random samples (Hayes, 2012).
I.M. Orta et al.
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
et al., 2005; Kochanska & Knaack, 2003; Kochanska et al., 2000). Model tting analyses
indicated that maternal responsiveness was associated with better social competency
and lower externalizing in part because of its positive association with effortful con-
trol. Children with highly responsive mothers were observed to be high in effortful
control. And children high in effortful control were also perceived by their teachers
to be high in social competency and low in externalizing. Evidence also indicated that
those associations were further mediated by effortful controlsassociationswithlow
emotion dysregulation. Findings supported both single and double mediation.
Effortful control predicted lower emotion dysregulation but was unrelated to
emotion regulation. This nding suggests that effortful control is associated with
childrens ability to modulate the intensity of emotional arousal and select emotional
responses in ways that do not violate contextual demands and expectations. How-
ever, effortful control does not appear, in parallel, to be associated with increased
emotional awareness, ability to transition easily between activities, and the expres-
sion of other contextually appropriate emotional responses. This interpretation is
consistent with the suggestion that a child low in emotion dysregulation need not
be high in emotion regulation, as low standing in emotion dysregulation could re-
ect generally low reactivity in emotions and hence less risk for dysregulation
(Eisenberg, Cumberland et al., 2001).
More importantly, this is the rst study to our knowledge to demonstrate
that effortful control predicts ER when the latter is measured in a circumscribed
manner. Previous studies had shown that effortful control is associated with the
ability to mask disappointment, a very narrow sampling of functioning from the
domain of ER (Carlson & Wang, 2007; Kieras et al., 2005). Furthermore, ndings
from model tting analyses indicated that effortful controls association with so-
cial competency was both direct and indirect. Children high in effortful control
were rated higher in social competency partly because they were also rated
lower in emotion dysregulation. In contrast, effortful controls association with
externalizing was indirect and fully mediated by lower emotion dysregulation.
This nding provides the rst direct evidence that is consistent with earlier inter-
pretations that effortful controls effects on externalizing symptoms speak to its
ER-related functions (e.g. Eisenberg, Fabes, Guthrie, & Reiser, 2000; Eisenberg,
Gershoff et al., 2001; Eisenberg et al., 2003, 2005; Spinrad et al., 2006, 2007).
Despite shared method variance, emotion regulation and dysregulation subscales
of the ERC had differential associations with social competency and externalizing.
Emotion dysregulation was associated with high externalizing and low social
competency, whereas emotion regulation was associated with high social compe-
tency, replicating the pattern of distinctive associations in earlier studies (Dunsmore
et al., 2012; Miller et al., 2006). The ndings support the suggestion that children who
are both high in emotion regulation and low in dysregulation are likely to meet the
challenges of effective social interactions, managing their own affective arousal while
reading peer cues accurately. In contrast, emotion dysregulation was more relevant
to prediction of externalizing symptoms.
The surprising lack of associations between emotion regulation and all other
constructs with the exception of social competency may have important implica-
tions for future research. It is possible the antecedents of emotion regulation are
highly specic both in terms of childrens prole in temperamental reactivity
and in terms of the parenting factors that foster it. Specic facets of maternal emo-
tion socialization and parental emotional communication styles including emotion
coaching and dismissive attitudes (Dunsmore et al., 2012; Lukenheimer et al.,
2007) may help elucidate the processes that foster emotion regulation. Research
that speaks to interrelations between temperamental reactivity proles and skills
Emotion Regulation
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
pertinent to emotion understanding including recognition and knowledge may
also help us better specify sources of variability in emotion regulation that is dis-
tinct from emotion dysregulation.
Collectively, the ndings suggest that the processes through which respon-
siveness may come to promote overlapping but distinct dimensions of chil-
drens self-regulation may be similar to both single mediation processes
reported in previous studies (Eisenberg, Gershoff et al., 2001; Eisenberg et al.,
2003, 2005; Spinrad et al., 2007) from North American samples and double
mediation processes reported for French adolescents (Hofer et al., 2010). While
some researchers have questioned the cross-cultural similarity in the meaning of
maternal sensitivity (Rothbaum, Weisz, Pott, Miyake, & Morelli, 2000) or
authoritativeauthoritarian parenting (e.g. Deater-Deckard & Dodge, 1997),
our ndings highlighted similarities rather than differences between the Turkish
culture and the industrialized North American countries. Direct behavioural
observations of maternal responsiveness used in the present study pertained
to maternal sensitivity, acceptance, and cooperation dimensions. These dimen-
sions included maternal behaviours that are believed to promote childrens
autonomy. It is important to note that the signicant change in the traditional
family system from being an agricultural, rural society into an increasingly
urban, industrial, modern, and egalitarian one (Kagitcibasi, 2007) is leading to
a gradual shift toward promotion of autonomy, while retaining a strong empha-
sis on emotional interdependence in urban, middle-class Turkish families.
Relatively high levels of maternal responsiveness observed in this sample are
consistent with the SES make-up of the sample. The ndings are also consistent
with other studies that indicate that Turkish mothers increasingly promote
autonomy and self-expression of the child seen in the North American and
European societies (Nacak, Yagmurlu, Durgel, & van de Vijver, 2011).
Strengths and Limitations
The cross-sectional design was a limitation that prevents causal conclusions. In ad-
dition, children in this study were recruited from preschools serving middle to
high socioeconomic backgrounds and therefore represent a restricted sampling of
the Turkish population, limiting generalizability of the associations to the larger
Turkish population. An important strength of the study was reliance on observa-
tional measures of both effortful control and maternal responsiveness that were col-
lected in multiple tasks and situations with varying demands, permitting a robust
assessmentoftheconstructsofcentralinterest, comparable with the best practices
in the eld (e.g. Eisenberg, Gershoff et al., 2001; Kochanska et al., 2000; Spinrad
et al., 2007). Another strength of the study was reliance on multiple methods
andmeasuresofconstructstominimizeinated associations because of shared
method variance, in particular with respect to associations among maternal
responsiveness, effortful control, and teacher ratings (ERC, social competency,
and externalizing).
Implications and Future Directions
The ndings inform the central role afforded to ER as mediating the links be-
tween familial and child factors on the antecedent side and indices of broad
self-regulation such as social competency and externalizing symptoms on the
outcome side (Morris et al., 2007). When circumscribed measures of ER are
I.M. Orta et al.
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
used, the results showed that emotion dysregulation rather than emotion regu-
lation appeared to mediate associations of maternal responsiveness and effortful
control on the antecedent side with childrens social competency and adjustment
difculties on the outcome side. In fact, emotion regulation was only associated
with better social competency and appeared unrelated to either effortful control
or maternal responsiveness. Those ndings encourage both a separate examina-
tion of dysregulation and regulation, and careful selection of ER measures to
permit a disambiguated understanding of the processes of self-regulation.
Given that emotion regulation showed highly specic associations with social
competency, factors that foster those skills need to be explored more broadly in
future studies. Nevertheless, the ndings support the growing conviction that
intervention and prevention programmes need to include modules that speci-
cally target improved ER skills as ndings both from this study and others
clearly show that emotion dysregulation plays a role in adjustment difculties
(Batum & Yagmurlu, 2007; Dunsmore et al., 2012; Miller et al., 2006). Findings
join others in the literature (e.g. Eisenberg et al., 2009; Yaman, Mesman, van
IJzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburgh, 2010), suggesting that programmes that
invest in increasing maternal responsiveness will have positive effects on child
functioning in non-Western contexts.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This study is based in part on the MA thesis of the rst author. Irem Metin is
currently at the Atilim University, and Nazan Aksan is currently at the University
of Iowa. The data were drawn from a larger project funded in part by TUBITAK
109K016 (PI: Nazan Aksan) and Koc University. We would like to thank the
parents who participated in the assessments, and the undergraduate and graduate
students who assisted with both data collection and coding: Senay Cebioglu, Akif
Yerlioglu, Pelin Kılıc, Goksin Erdemli, Gozde Gokozan, and Oya Sakiroglu.
Note
1. Please note that we use the abbreviation ER throughout the manuscript to refer
to the umbrella construct of emotion regulation. To refer to specic meanings
such as emotion regulation and emotion dysregulation, we do not rely on any
abbreviations.
2. One item pertaining to at affect was removed from dysregulation subscale, as
it lowered subscale alpha.
3. Age was residualized from effortful control and externalizing (the only vari-
ables that showed signicant correlations with age) during LISREL model
tting analyses.
REFERENCES
Ainsworth, M. D. S., Bell, S. M., & Stayton, D. J. (1971). Individual differences in strange
situation behavior of one-year-olds. In H. R. Schaffer (Ed.), The origins of human social
relations (pp. 1757). London: Academic Press.
Emotion Regulation
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders
(4th ed., text rev). Washington, DC: Author.
Anderson, S. W., Aksan, N., Kochanska, G., Damasio, H., Wisnowski, J., & Afifi,A.
(2007). The earliest expression of focal damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cortex,
6,667816.
Batum, P., & Yagmurlu, B. (2007). What counts in externalizing behaviors? The contribu-
tions of emotion and behavior regulation. Current Psychology,25, 272294.
Bell, S. M., & Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1972). Infant crying and maternal responsiveness. Child
Development,43, 11711190.
Berument, S. K., & Güven, A. G. (2010). Turkish Expressive and Receptive Language Test:
Receptive Vocabulary Sub-scale (TIFALDI-RT). Turkish Psychological Society.
Blandon, A. Y., Calkins, S. D., Grimm, K. J., Keane, S. P., & OBrien, M. (2010). Testing a
developmental cascade model of emotional and social competence and early peer
acceptance. Development and Psychopathology,22, 737748.
Bollen, K (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley.
Calkins, S. D. (2010). Commentary: Conceptual and methodological challenges to the study
of emotion regulation and psychopathology. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral
Assessment,32,9295.
Campos, J. J., Walle, E. A., Dahl, A., & Main, A. (2011). Reconceptualizing emotion regula-
tion. Emotion Review,3,2635.
Carlson, S. M., & Wang, T. S. (2007). Inhibitory control and emotion regulation in preschool
children. Cognitive Development,22, 489510.
Chang, H., Olson, S. L., Sameroff, A. J., & Sexton, H.R. (2010). Child effortful control as a
mediator of parenting practices on externalizing behavior: Evidence for a sex-
differentiated pathway across the transition from preschool to school. Journal of Abnormal
Psychology,39,7181
Cole, P. M., Martin, S. E., & Dennis, T. A. (2004). Emotion regulation as a scientic construct:
Methodological challenges and directions for child development research. Child Develop-
ment,75, 317333.
Corapcı, F., Aksan, N., Arslan-Yalcin, D., & Yagmurlu, B. (2010). The psychometric
evaluation of the social competence and behavior evaluation scale with Turkish pre-
schoolers. Turkish Journal of Child and Adolescent Mental Health,17,314.
Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting style as context: An integrative model.
Psychological Bulletin,113, 487496.
Davidov, M., & Grusec, J. E. (2006). Untangling the links of parental responsiveness to
distress and warmth to child outcomes. Child Development,77,4458.
Deater-Deckard, K., & Dodge, K. A. (1997). Externalizing behavior problems and discipline
revisited: Nonlinear effects and variation by culture, context, and gender. Psychological
Inquiry,8, 161175.
Derryberry, D., & Rothbart, M. K. (1997). Reactive and effortful processes in the organiza-
tion of temperament. Development and Psychopathology,9, 633652.
DeWolff, MS, & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Sensitivity and attachment: A meta-analysis
on parental antecedents of infant attachment. Child Development,68, 571591.
Diamond, A., & Taylor, C. (1996). Development of an aspect of executive control: Develop-
ment of the abilities to remember what I said and to do as I say, not as I do.Developmen-
tal Psychobiology,29, 315334.
Dunsmore, J. C., Booker, J. A., & Ollendick, T. H. (2012). Parental emotion coaching and
child emotion regulation as protective factors for children with oppositional deant disor-
der. Social Development. DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.00652.x
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. (2006). Emotion regulation and childrens socioemotional compe-
tence. Child psychology: A handbook of contemporary issues (2nd ed., pp. 357381).
New York: Psychology.
Eisenberg, N., Gershoff, E.T., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Cumberland, A.J., Losoya, S. H.,
Guthrie, I. K., & Murphy, B. C. (2001). Mothersemotional expressivity and childrens
behavior problems and social competence: Mediation through childrens regulation.
Developmental Psychology,37, 475490.
I.M. Orta et al.
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
Eisenberg, N., Liew, J., & Pidada, S.U. (2001). The relations of parental emotional expressiv-
ity with quality of Indonesian childrens social functioning. Emotion,1,116136.
Eisenberg, N., Pidada, S., & Liew, J. (2001). The relations of regulation and negative
emotionality to Indonesian childrens social functioning. Child Development,72, 17471763.
Eisenberg, N., Chang, L., Ma, Y., & Huang, X. (2009). Relations of parenting style to Chinese
childrens effortful control, ego resilience, and maladjustment. Development and Psychopa-
thology,21, 455477.
Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Spinrad, T. L., Fabes, R. A., Shepard, S. A., Reiser, M. et al.
(2001). The relations of regulation and emotionality to childrens externalizing and
internalizing problem behavior. Child Development,72, 11121134.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Guthrie, I. K., & Reiser, M. (2000). Dispositional emotionality
and regulation: Their role in predicting quality of social functioning. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology,78, 136157.
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B., Karbon, M., Smith, M., & Maszk, P. (1996). The
relations of childrens dispositional empathy-related responding to their emotionality,
regulation, and social functioning. Developmental Psychology,32, 195209.
Eisenberg, N., Valiente, C., Morris, A. S., Fabes, R. A., Cumberland, A. J., Reiser, M.,
Gershoff, E. T., Shepard, S. A., & Losoya, S. (2003). Longitudinal relations among parental
emotional expressivity, childrens regulation, and quality of socioemotional functioning.
Developmental Psychology,39,319.
Eisenberg, N., Zhou, Q., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Fabes, R. A., & Liew, J. (2005). Relations
among positive parenting, childrens effortful control, and externalizing problems: A
three-wave longitudinal study. Child Development,76, 10551071.
Fish, M., Stifter, C. A., & Belsky, J. (1991). Conditions of continuity and discontinuity in
infant negative emotionality: New born to ve months. Child Development,62, 15251537.
Fox, N. A., & Calkins, S. D. (2003). The development of self-control of emotion: Intrinsic and
extrinsic inuences. Motivation and Emotion,27,726.
Goldsmith, H. H., Pollak, S. D., & Davidson R. J. (2008). Developmental neuroscience
perspectives on emotion regulation. Child Development Perspectives,2, 132140.
Gottman, J. M., Katz, L. F., & Hooven, C. (1996). Parental meta-emotion philosophy and the
emotional life of the families: Theoretical models and preliminary data. Journal of Family
Psychology,10, 243268.
Graziano, P. A., Keane, S. P., & Calkins, S. D. (2007). The role of emotion regulation in
childrens early academic success. Journal of School Psychology,45,319.
Gross, J. J., & Thompson, R. A. (2007). Emotion regulation: Conceptual foundations. In
J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation (pp. 324). New York: Guilford Press.
Halberstadt, A. G., Denham, S. A., & Dunsmore, J. C. (2011). Affective social competence.
Social Development,10,79119.
Haley, D. W., & Stansbury, K. (2003). Infant stress and parent responsiveness: Regulation of
physiology and behavior during still-face and reunion. Child Development,74, 15341546.
Hayes, A. F. (2012). PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable media-
tion, moderation, and conditional process modeling [White paper]. Retrieved from
http://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf
Hofer, C., Eisenberg, N., & Reiser, M. (2010). The role of socialization, effortful control and ego-
resiliency in French adolescentssocial functioning. Journal of Research on Adolescence,20, 555582.
Jöreskog, K. G., & Sörbom, D. (1993). Lisrel 8: Structural equation modeling with the SIMPLIS
command language. Hillsdale, NJ.: Lawrence Erlbaum
Judd, C. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1981). Process analysis: Estimating mediation in treatment
evaluations. Evaluation Review,5, 602619.
Kagitcibasi, C. (2007). Family, self and human development across cultures. Theory and
applications (2nd) ed. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Kaplan, D. (2000). Advanced quantitative techniques in social sciences. Structural equation
modeling: Foundations and extensions.Vol10. CA: Sage.
Keller, H., Yovsi, R., Borke, J., Kärtner, J., Jensen, H., & Papaligoura, Z. (2004). Developmen-
tal consequences of early parenting experiences: Self-recognition and self-regulation in
three cultural communities. Child Development,75, 17451760.
Emotion Regulation
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
Kieras, J. E., Tobin, R. M., Graziano, W. G., & Rothbart, M. K. (2005). You cant always get
what you want: Effortful control and childrens responses to undesirable gifts. Psycholog-
ical Science,16, 391396.
Kim, J., & Cicchetti, D. (2010). Longitudinal pathways linking child maltreatment, emotion
regulation, peer relations, and psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry,
51, 706716.
Kochanska, G. (1998). Motherchild relationship, child fearfulness, and emerging attach-
ment: A short-term longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology,34, 480490.
Kochanska, G., & Aksan, N. (2004). Development of mutual responsiveness between
parents and their young children, Child Development,75, 16571676.
Kochanska, G., & Knaack, A. (2003). Effortful control as a personality characteristic of
young children: Antecedents, correlates and consequences. Journal of Personality,71,
10871112.
Kochanska, G., Murray, K. T., & Harran, H. T. (2000). Effortful control in early childhood:
Continuity and change, antecedents, and implications for social development. Develop-
mental Psychology,36, 220232.
Kochanska, G., Murray, K., Jacques, T. Y., Koenig, A. N., & Vandegeest, K. A. (1996). Inhib-
itory control in young children and its role in emerging internalization, Child Development,
67, 490507.
Kopp, C. B. (1989). Regulation of distress and negative emotions: A developmental view.
Developmental Psychology,25, 343354.
LaFreniere, P. J., & Dumas, J. E. (1996). Social competence and behavior evaluation in
children ages 3 to 6 years: The short form (SCBE-30). Psychological Assessment,4, 442450.
Lewis, A. R. Zinbarg, R. E., & Durbin E (2010). Advances, problems, and challenges in the
study of emotion regulation: A commentary. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral
Assessment,32,8391.
Lukenheimer, E. S., Shields, A. M., &Cortina, K. S. (2007). Parental emotion coaching and
dismissing in family interaction. Social Development,16, 232248.
Miller, A. L., Fine, S. E., Gouley, K. K., Seifer, R., Dickstein, S., & Shields, A. (2006). Showing
and telling about emotions: Interrelations between facets of emotional competence and
associations with classroom adjustment in Head Start preschoolers. Cognition and
Emotion,20, 11701192.
Mintz, T. M., Hamre, B. K., & Hateld, B. E. (2011). The role of effortful control in mediating
the association between maternal sensitivity and childrens social and relational compe-
tence and problems in rst grade. Early Education and Development,22, 360387.
Morris, A. S., Silk, J. S., Steinberg, L., Myers, S. S., & Robinson, L. R. (2007). The role of the
family context in the development of emotion regulation. Social Development,16, 361388.
Nacak, M., Yagmurlu, B., Durgel, E., & van de Vijver, F. (2011). Parenting in metropole and
Anatolia samples: The role of residence and education in beliefs and behaviors. Turkish
Journal of Psychology,26,85100.
Posada, G., Jacobs, A., Richmond, M. K., Carbonell, O. A., Alzate, G., Bustamante, M. R., &
Quiceno, J. (2002). Maternal caregiving and infant security in two cultures. Developmental
Psychology,38,6778.
Power, T. G. (2004). Stress and coping in childhood: The parentsrole. Parenting,4, 271317.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2004). SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect
effects in simple mediation models. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers,
36, 717731.
Preacher, K. J., & Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing
and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behavior Research Methods,
40, 879891.
Ramsden, S. R., & Hubbard, J. A. (2002). Family expressivenessand parental emotion coaching:
Theirroleinchildrensemotionregulationandaggression.JournalofAbnormalChildPsychology,
30,657668.
Rose-Krasnor,L.(1997).Thenatureofsocialcompetence:Atheoreticalreview.SocialDevelopment,
6, 111135.
I.M. Orta et al.
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd
Rothbart,M.K.,&Bates,J.E.(2006).Temperament.InN.Eisenberg&W.Damon(Eds.) Handbookof
child psychology: Vol 3. Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed., pp. 99166). New
York:Wiley.
Rothbart,M. K., & Sheese,B.R. (2007).Temperamentand emotion regulation. InJ. J.Gross (Ed.),
Handbookofemotion regulation (pp. 33150). NewYork:GuilfordPress.
Rothbaum,F.,Weisz,J.,Pott,M.,Miyake,K.,&Morelli,G.(2000).Attachmentandculture:Security
intheUnited States and Japan. American Psychologist,55, 10931104.
Saarni,C. (1990). Emotional competency: How emotions andrelationshipbecomeintegrated.In
R.A. Thompson(Ed.),Nebraskasymposium onmotivationand emotion, 1988:Socioemotionaldevel-
opment,35,115182.Lincoln, NE: University of NebraskaPress.
Satorra, A., & Bentler, P. M (2001). A scaled difference chi-square test statistic in moment
structure analysis. Psychometrika,66, 507514.
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1997). Emotion regulation among school-age children: The
development and validation of a new criterion Q-sort scale. Developmental Psychology,
33, 906916.
Shields, A., & Cicchetti, D. (1998). Reactive aggression among maltreated children: The con-
tributions of attention and emotion dysregulation. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology,27,
381395.
Spinrad, T., Eisenberg, N., Cumberland, A., Fabes, R. A., Valiente, C., Shepard, S. A. et al.
(2006). Relation of emotion-related regulation to childrens social competence: A longitu-
dinal study. Emotion,6, 498510.
Spinrad, T. L., Eisenberg, N., Gaertner, B., Popp, T., Smith, C. L., Kupfer, A.Hofer, C. (2007).
Relations of maternal socialization and toddlerseffortful control to childrens adjustment
and social competence. Developmental Psychology,43, 11701186.
Sroufe, L. A. (1995). Emotional development: The organization of emotional life in the early years.
Cambridge, New York: Cambridge University Press.
Sumer, N., Selcuk, E., Gunaydın, G., Salman, S., & Harma, M. (2008, July). Maternal
sensitivity and child security in Turkish culture. Symposia presentation at 20th Biennial
Meeting of the International Society for the Study of Behavioral Development, Germany.
Tamir, M. (Ed.) (2011). The maturing eld of emotion regulation (Special issue). Emotion
Review,3,37.
Thompson, R. A. (1994). Emotion regulation: A theme in search of denitions. In N. A. Fox
(Ed.), The development of emotion regulation: Biological and behavioral considerations: Mono-
graphs for the Society for Research in Child Development 59 (23, Serial No. 240), pp. 2552.
Thompson, R. A. (2011). Emotion and emotion regulation: Two sides of the developing coin.
Emotion Review,3,5361.
Thompson, R. A., & Meyer, S. (2007). Socialization of emotion regulation. In J. J. Gross (Ed.),
Handbook of emotion regulation, pp. 249268. NY: Guilford.
Valenzuela, M. (1997). Maternal sensitivity in a developing society: The context of urban
poverty and infant chronic undernutrition. Developmental Psychology,33, 845855.
w?>Yagmurlu,B.,&Altan,O.(2010).Maternalsocializationandchildtemperamentaspredictors
ofemotionregulation in Turkishpreschoolers. Infant andChildDevelopment,19,275296.
Yaman, A. & Mesman, J. & van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J. (2010).
Parenting and toddler aggression in second-generation immigrant families: The moderat-
ing role of child temperament. Journal of Family Psychology,24, 208211.
Zelazo, P. A., & Cunningham, W. (2007). Executive function: Mechanisms underlying
emotion regulation. In J. J. Gross (Ed.), Handbook of emotion regulation, pp. 135158. New
York: Guilford Press.
Zhou, Q., Eisenberg, N., Wang, Y., & Reiser, M. (2004). Chinese childrens effortful control
and dispositional anger/frustration: Relations to parenting styles and childrens social
functioning. Developmental Psychology,40, 352366.
Emotion Regulation
Copyright © 2013 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Inf. Child. Dev. (2013)
DOI: 10.1002/icd

File (1)

Content uploaded by Nazan Aksan
Author content
... Items were rated on a 6-point scale (1 = never to 6 = always ). Previous studies including those with Turkish samples have shown that the SCBE-30 has good internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and construct validity ( LaFreniere & Dumas, 1996;Metin Orta, Corapci, Yagmurlu, & Aksan, 2013 ). In the present study, alphas for the AA and SC subscales were 0.87 and 0.67, respectively. ...
Article
This study aimed to better understand the construct of classroom chaos in relation to child care program quality and examine its association with child externalizing problems and social competence. Towards this end, two studies were conducted at child care programs in Turkey. Study 1 was qualitiative and relied on eight focus group interviews with 24 Turkish teachers to gain a deeper understanding of what is meant by quality and chaos. Study 2 included 49 teachers and 251 preschoolers as participants to investigate the concurrent relations of classroom chaos and quality with children’s socioemotional functioning. Results of multilevel linear modeling analyses revealed that teachers’ increased perception of classroom chaos was associated with higher levels of externalizing problems and lower levels of social competence in children, even after controlling for program quality, child age, gender, and temperamental effortful control. Teacher-rated classroom chaos was also ssociated with motherreported externalizing problems. On average, program quality was rated as minimal and showed no significant relations with externalizing problems and social competence. The implications of these findings were discussed with respect to the intervention targets in early child care and education programs.
... Research linking direct assessment of EFs with adult reports of young children's social-emotional behaviors in LMICs has been rare. A notable exception is a study of Turkish preschoolers that linked lower EFs with teacher reports of emotional dysregulation in the classroom (Orta et al., 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Performance-based measures of children's executive functions (EFs) do not capture children's application of these skills during everyday emotionally-laden and socially-mediated interactions. The current study demonstrates the value of using assessor report of self-regulation behaviors (inhibitory control and positive affect/engagement) in addition to EF tasks when studying early childhood experiences and development in a rural lower-middle-income country setting. In a sample of 1302 disadvantaged 4-year-olds living in rural Pakistan, we found that directly assessed EFs were significantly related to assessor observations of children's inhibitory control and positive affect/engagement during a structured assessment protocol. However, EFs and two types of self-regulation behaviors demonstrated unique associations with children's (1) contextual experiences, as indexed by family socio-economic resources, participation in parenting interventions, and children's physical growth; and (2) age-salient developmental outcomes, as indexed by direct assessment of pre-academic skills and maternal report of prosocial behaviors and behavior problems. First, family wealth uniquely predicted only observed positive affect/engagement, whereas maternal education uniquely predicted only EFs. Second, children's antecedent linear growth was a significant predictor of both EFs and positive affect/engagement, but exposure to an enhanced nutrition intervention during the first 2 years of life and preschoolers' hair cortisol concentration were associated only with observed self-regulation behaviors. Finally, both EFs and observed positive affect/engagement uniquely predicted children's pre-academic skills. In contrast, only assessors' ratings of positive affect/engagement uniquely predicted maternal report of prosocial behaviors and only assessors' ratings of inhibitory control uniquely predicted maternal report of behavioral problems.
... In the early development stage, if mothers can respond to children's behavior timely and express positive emotions, their children's social competence will be higher than that of other children (Landry et al., 1998). As parents behave more warmly, children's social competence increases (Orta et al., 2013). Tan (2012) found that mothers with family stress tend to adopt indulgent and authoritarian parenting styles. ...
Article
Full-text available
Since 2011, China has gradually moved from a one-child policy to a comprehensive two-child policy, and this year implemented a three-child policy. In this context, the situation faced by parents has also changed, which has led to more pressure to raise children. This study explored the relationships among mothers’ parenting stress, maternal warmth, preschool children’s social competence, and their sibling relationships. Whether the pressure affect social competence, and whether the sibling relationships and maternal warmth play a chain-mediating role in the relationships. In this study, data were collected in two periods. The second survey was conducted one year later. This study included 180 preschool children and their mothers. The results revealed that mothers’ parenting stress negatively predicted children’s social competence and behavior. Sibling relationships and maternal warmth had a significant chain mediation effect on mothers’ parenting stress and children’s sensitive cooperation and anger attacks. The results suggests mothers’ parenting stress increased, the maternal warmth behavior decreased, sibling hostility increased, then children expressed less sensitive cooperation and increased anger and aggressive behavior.
... SYDD-30'un öğretmen formunun Türkçeye uyarlaması Çorapçı ve diğerleri (2010) tarafından yapılmıştır. Ölçeğin geçerliliğini destekler nitelikte SYDD-30 alt ölçek puanlarının okul öncesi çocukların duygu düzenleme ve çaba gerektiren kontrol becerileri ile beklenen yönde ilişkili olduğu bulunmuştur (Çorapçı vd., 2010;Orta, Çorapçı, Yağmurlu ve Aksan, 2013). Geçmiş çalışmalarda, SYDD-30 alt ölçeklerinin iç tutarlılık katsayıları yüksek bulunmuştur (Çorapçı vd., 2010;Orta vd., 2013). ...
... Esta proliferación de estudios y de instrumentos implica el uso de diferentes términos para referirse a este conjunto de conductas significativas emitidas por el cuidador. Algunas etiquetas frecuentes incluyen el término responsividad, nombrando el constructo como responsividad materna, maternal responsiveness (Kochanska y Kim, 2013;McComish y Visger, 2009;Orta et al., 2013) o maternal responsivity (Popp y Wilcox, 2012;Sterling y Warren, 2014;Wade et al., 2015;Warren y Brady, 2007); y también como responsividad sensible, sensitive responsiveness (Lieberman, 1991;Tharner et al., 2012). Además de esta etiqueta, también se emplean a veces términos de mayor amplitud conceptual como por ejemplo comportamiento materno, maternal behavior (Pederson et al., 1990), o sistema de cuidados, caregiving system (George y Solomon, 2008). ...
Article
The quality of parental response is the most-studied variable in terms of a child´s attachment. This paper presents an d analysis of the evolution of Ainsworth´s construct since its origin (1969), as well as a deion of the most common ambiguities and misinterpretations in its use in the scientific literature. First, this paper offers an analysis of the original dimensions proposed by Ainsworth in her Maternal Care scales. Second, the study s the terminological and conceptual problems that have been associated with the development of this construct. Third, the paper reviews the components highlighted in the most recent research as significant for the formation of the attachment, while expanding and updating the components on maternal care. In conclusion, the paper provides an d proposal of the construct under the term quality of parental responsiveness. Resumen La calidad de la respuesta parental es la variable que más se ha estudiado en relación con el vínculo de apego del niño. Este artículo presenta un análisis actualizado de la evolución del constructo planteado por Mary Ainsworth desde su origen, así como una descripción de las ambigüedades y confusiones más habituales en su uso en la literatura científica. En primer lugar, se ofrece un análisis de las dimensiones originales planteadas por Ainsworth en sus escalas de Cuidado Materno. En segundo lugar, se analizan los problemas terminológicos y conceptuales que se han asociado al desarrollo de este constructo. En tercer lugar, se ofrece una revisión que amplía y actualiza los componentes del cuidado materno, incluyendo los componentes que la investigación más reciente ha destacado como significativos para la formación del vínculo de apego. Como conclusión, se ofrece una propuesta actualizada del constructo bajo el término calidad de la responsividad parental.
... In both tasks, children required to remember a new rule, inhibit the dominant response, and perform the subdominant response. Both the day-night task (Orta et al., 2013) and the peg-tapping task (Korucu et al., 2017) were used in the previous studies conducted with Turkish preschoolers. ...
Article
This study investigates whether preschool-aged institution-reared children’s developmental change within one year in theory of mind, executive function, and receptive language abilities differs from their parent-reared peers from different socioeconomic backgrounds. Data were collected from 73 institution-reared, 30 parent-reared children from low socioeconomic status (SES), 36 parent-reared children from middle-SES and 60 parent-reared children from high-SES at two time points, one year apart. Our findings showed that all children showed a significant increase in theory of mind and executive function, but institution-reared group displayed significantly lower performance than all parent-reared groups at Time 2. Difference score analyses revealed that institutional care predicted poorer developmental change within one year in receptive language, holding age and sex constant, as compared to parental care. Specifically, institution-reared children displayed a significant decline and low-SES children showed no change in their receptive language over time, whereas middle- and high-SES children increased significantly.
Article
Background: Depression is one of the main mental health problems facing primary school students. Temperamental effortful control (EC) has received much attention as a protective factor to depression. The present study aimed to verify the association of effortful control and children's depression and explore the mediating roles of emotion regulation ability (ERA) and strategies (ERSs) in the relationship. Methods: A total of 449 valid primary school students in grades 3 to 6 were included in the study. Self-report and parent-report questionnaires were used to collect data on key variables, including EC, depression, ERA and ERSs. Results: Gender and whether-native-or-not had significant effects on children's depression. EC was significantly and negatively correlated with children's depression; EC and depression were significantly correlated with Emotion Regulation dimension of ERA and cognitive reappraisal; but there was no correlation between EC and Lability/Negativity, as well as between expression suppression and depression. EmotionRegulation and cognitive reappraisal played mediating effects in the relationship between EC and depression parallelly and sequentially. Limitations: The study used cross-sectional design and the sample was restricted to Chinese primary school students. Additionally, the types and research perspectives of ERSs can be further expanded. Conclusions: EC can reduce the level of children's depression by improving the abilities of Emotion Regulation and the use of cognitive reappraisal strategy, as well as the chain mediating effects of the two.
Article
Background: Little is known about the role of emotion regulation in driving. This study analyzed the psychometric properties of one of the most commonly-used tests for the evaluation of emotion regulation in a drivers' sample. Methods: A total of 318 male drivers (M age = 41.6 years, SD = 11.1, age range 20-69 years, half with road traffic offences and half, matched controls), participated in the study by filling out the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ). Data analysis was carried out using the Rasch Rating Scale Model (RSM). Results: The performance of the response categories was inadequate. After collapsing the seven original response categories into three, the categories were functional, the unidimensionality requirement was met, and data-model fit was adequate. Both person reliability and item reliability were adequate for the two subscales of ERQ. There was a significant difference between the two groups in the cognitive reappraisal subscale. Conclusions: Given the adequate psychometric properties of the ERQ, it is suggested that the role of cognitive reappraisal in driving contexts be explored in depth.
Article
Full-text available
Resumen Durante la infancia y la adolescencia la regulación de las emociones tiene repercusiones en diferentes ámbitos, tanto en estas etapas como en la vida adulta. El objetivo de este artículo fue evaluar un test de regulación emocional, Emotional Regulation Checklist (ERC), utilizando el CET-R como marco de referencia. Se analizaron 71 artículos que emplearon el ERC para la evaluación de la regulación emocional. La información se clasificó teniendo en cuenta la descripción del test, su fiabilidad y validez. Todos los resultados de fiabilidad se mostraron mediante el coeficiente alfa de Cronbach. En relación a la validez, fue adecuado el promedio de las correlaciones del ERC con sus criterios, pero con algunas carencias. Es recomendable plantear investigaciones con modelos psicométricos avanzados.
Article
Self-organization can be approached in terms of developmental processes occurring within and between component systems of temperament. Within-system organization involves progressive shaping of cortical representations by subcortical motivational systems. As cortical representations develop, they feed back to provide motivational systems with enhanced detection and guidance capabilities. These reciprocal influences may amplify the underlying motivational functions and promote excessive impulsivity or anxiety. However, these processes also depend upon interactions arising between motivational and attentional systems. We discuss these between-system effects by considering the regulation of approach motivation by reactive attentional processes related to fear and by more voluntary processes related to effortful control. It is suggested that anxious and impulsive psychopathology may reflect limitations in these dual means of control, which can take the form of overregulation as well as underregulation.
Article
Reflecting author Çigdem Kagitçibasi's influential work over the last two decades, this new edition examines human development, the self, and the family in a cultural context. It challenges the existing assumptions in mainstream western psychology about the nature of individuals.
Article
Individual differences in emotionality and regulation are central to conceptions of temperament and personality. In this article, conceptions of emotionality and regulation and ways in which they predict social functioning are examined. Linear (including additive) and nonlinear effects are reviewed. In addition, data on mediational and moderational relations from a longitudinal study are presented. The effects of attention regulation on social functioning were mediated by resiliency, and this relation was moderated by negative emotionality at the first, but not second, assessment. Negative emotionality moderated the relation of behavior regulation to socially appropriate/prosocial behavior. These results highlight the importance of examining different types of regulation and the ways in which dispositional characteristics interact in predicting social outcomes.
Article
The aim of this study is to investigate parenting cognitions, behaviors, and home environment in metropolitan and rural cities (in Anatolia) to explore the generalizability of parenting research that frequently includes high-and/or low-educated populations in big cities. The data were obtained from 162 mothers (88 high-educated) living in Istanbul (metropolis) and 73 mothers (32 high-educated) living in much smaller cities in Anatolia. Mother reports and structured observations were used to measure mothers' developmental expectations, child-rearing practices, and home environment. Results showed that mothers living in Istanbul reported earlier ages of development on all domains of skills except for moral values and lower levels of obedience-demanding and punitive behaviors compared to mothers living in rural cities. The physical environment of home was found to be safer in rural cities. Comparisons based on educational background showed that high-educated mothers in Istanbul reported earlier ages of mastery on all domains, and also lower levels of obedience-demanding and higher levels of permissive behaviors than loweducated mothers. High-educated mothers also provided more learning materials to their children and the physical environment of their homes was safer. Findings regarding the relation between background characteristics (e.g., education and residency), child-rearing environment and parenting behaviors of mothers were discussed in reference to existing literature. The importance of studying parenting issues with respect to socioeconomic characteristics of different groups within one country is stressed.
Article
The relations between mothers' expressed positive and negative emotion and 55-79-month-olds' (76% European American) regulation, social competence, and adjustment were examined. Structural equation modeling was used to test the plausibility of the hypothesis that the effects of maternal expression of emotion on children's adjustment and social competence are mediated through children's dispositional regulation. Mothers' expressed emotions were assessed during interactions with their children and with maternal reports of emotions expressed in the family. Children's regulation, externalizing and internalizing problems, and social competence were rated by parents and teachers, and children's persistence was surreptitiously observed. There were unique effects of positive and negative maternal expressed emotion on children's regulation, and the relations of maternal expressed emotion to children's externalizing problem behaviors and social competence were mediated through children's regulation. Alternative models of causation were tested; a child-directed model in which maternal expressivity mediated the effects of child regulation on child outcomes did not fit the data as well.
Article
In this study, we explored the relations between positive and negative family expressiveness, parental emotion coaching, child emotion regulation, and child aggression. The sample included 120 fourth-grade children and their mothers. Mothers completed the Emotion Regulation Checklist, the Family Expressiveness Questionnaire, and a portion of the meta-emotion interview to assess their awareness and acceptance of, and instruction in managing their child's anger and sadness (3 dimensions of parental emotion coaching). Teachers rated each child's aggression and completed the Emotion Regulation Checklist for each child. The 3 dimensions of parental emotion coaching and positive and negative family expressiveness were not directly related to child aggression. However, both negative family expressiveness and the mother's acceptance of the child's negative emotions were indirectly related to child aggression through the child's emotion regulation.