ArticlePDF Available

Abstract and Figures

Different language skills are considered fundamental for successful reading and spelling acquisition. Extensive evidence has highlighted the central role of phonological awareness in early literacy experiences. However, many orthographic systems also require the contribution of morphological awareness. The goal of this study was to examine the morphological and phonological awareness skills of preschool children as longitudinal predictors of reading and spelling ability by the end of first grade, controlling for the effects of receptive and expressive vocabulary skills. At Time 1 preschool children from kindergartens in the Greek regions of Attika, Crete, Macedonia, and Thessaly were assessed on tasks tapping receptive and expressive vocabulary, phonological awareness (syllable and phoneme), and morphological awareness (inflectional and derivational). Tasks were administered through an Android application for mobile devices (tablets) featuring automatic application of ceiling rules. At Time 2 one year later the same children attending first grade were assessed on measures of word and pseudoword reading, text reading fluency, text reading comprehension, and spelling. Complete data from 104 children are available. Hierarchical linear regression and commonality analyses were conducted for each outcome variable. Reading accuracy for both words and pseudowords was predicted not only by phonological awareness, as expected, but also by morphological awareness, suggesting that understanding the functional role of word parts supports the developing phonology–orthography mappings. However, only phonological awareness predicted text reading fluency at this age. Longitudinal prediction of reading comprehension by both receptive vocabulary and morphological awareness was already evident at this age, as expected. Finally, spelling was predicted by preschool phonological awareness, as expected, as well as by morphological awareness, the contribution of which is expected to increase due to the spelling demands of Greek inflectional and derivational suffixes introduced at later grades.
Content may be subject to copyright.
fpsyg-08-02039 November 23, 2017 Time: 15:59 # 1
ORIGINAL RESEARCH
published: 27 November 2017
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02039
Edited by:
Daniela Traficante,
Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore,
Italy
Reviewed by:
Susan Rvachew,
McGill University, Canada
Barbara T. Conboy,
University of Washington,
United States
*Correspondence:
Vassiliki Diamanti
vasiliki.diamanti@isp.uio.no
Specialty section:
This article was submitted to
Language Sciences,
a section of the journal
Frontiers in Psychology
Received: 27 June 2017
Accepted: 07 November 2017
Published: 27 November 2017
Citation:
Diamanti V, Mouzaki A, Ralli A,
Antoniou F, Papaioannou S and
Protopapas A (2017) Preschool
Phonological and Morphological
Awareness As Longitudinal Predictors
of Early Reading and Spelling
Development in Greek.
Front. Psychol. 8:2039.
doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02039
Preschool Phonological and
Morphological Awareness As
Longitudinal Predictors of Early
Reading and Spelling Development
in Greek
Vassiliki Diamanti1,2*, Angeliki Mouzaki1, Asimina Ralli3, Faye Antoniou3,
Sofia Papaioannou4and Athanassios Protopapas2
1Department of Primary Education, University of Crete, Rethymno, Greece, 2Department of Special Needs Education,
University of Oslo, Oslo, Norway, 3Department of Philosophy, Pedagogy, and Psychology, National and Kapodistrian
University of Athens, Athens, Greece, 4Department of Medicine, University of Crete, Rethymno, Greece
Different language skills are considered fundamental for successful reading and
spelling acquisition. Extensive evidence has highlighted the central role of phonological
awareness in early literacy experiences. However, many orthographic systems also
require the contribution of morphological awareness. The goal of this study was to
examine the morphological and phonological awareness skills of preschool children as
longitudinal predictors of reading and spelling ability by the end of first grade, controlling
for the effects of receptive and expressive vocabulary skills. At Time 1 preschool children
from kindergartens in the Greek regions of Attika, Crete, Macedonia, and Thessaly
were assessed on tasks tapping receptive and expressive vocabulary, phonological
awareness (syllable and phoneme), and morphological awareness (inflectional and
derivational). Tasks were administered through an Android application for mobile devices
(tablets) featuring automatic application of ceiling rules. At Time 2 one year later
the same children attending first grade were assessed on measures of word and
pseudoword reading, text reading fluency, text reading comprehension, and spelling.
Complete data from 104 children are available. Hierarchical linear regression and
commonality analyses were conducted for each outcome variable. Reading accuracy
for both words and pseudowords was predicted not only by phonological awareness,
as expected, but also by morphological awareness, suggesting that understanding
the functional role of word parts supports the developing phonology–orthography
mappings. However, only phonological awareness predicted text reading fluency at this
age. Longitudinal prediction of reading comprehension by both receptive vocabulary
and morphological awareness was already evident at this age, as expected. Finally,
spelling was predicted by preschool phonological awareness, as expected, as well as
by morphological awareness, the contribution of which is expected to increase due to
the spelling demands of Greek inflectional and derivational suffixes introduced at later
grades.
Keywords: morphological awareness, phonological awareness, reading, spelling, longitudinal study
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 1November 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 2039
fpsyg-08-02039 November 23, 2017 Time: 15:59 # 2
Diamanti et al. Preschool Predictors of Early Literacy
INTRODUCTION
Reading and spelling are considerable cognitive undertakings
that require the integration of written and spoken language. An
overwhelming body of research evidence suggests that children’s
phonological awareness, which requires conscious reflection
upon and explicit manipulation of the constituent speech sounds
of language, is a necessary requirement for the acquisition of the
alphabetic principle (Byrne, 1996) and a key skill for mastering
decoding (National Reading Panel, 2000;Lonigan et al., 2009)
and spelling across orthographies (Cataldo and Ellis, 1988;Ellis
and Cataldo, 1990;Byrne and Fielding-Barnsley, 1991, 1993;
Porpodas, 1999;Aidinis and Nunes, 2001;Caravolas et al.,
2001, 2005;Cardoso-Martins and Pennington, 2004;Furnes and
Samuelsson, 2010).
On the other hand, morphological awareness plays a
fundamental role in mastering decoding, reading fluency, and
comprehension (Deacon and Kirby, 2004;Kuo and Anderson,
2006;Tong et al., 2011;Kirby et al., 2012;Deacon et al.,
2014;Muroya et al., 2017) and orthographic spelling (Deacon
and Kirby, 2004;Deacon and Bryant, 2005, 2006;Desrochers
et al., 2017) across orthographies (Wei et al., 2014;Rothou and
Padeliadu, 2015;Grigorakis and Manolitsis, 2016;Pan et al.,
2016;Vaknin-Nusbaum et al., 2016a,b;Muroya et al., 2017).
Morphological awareness refers to (a) an explicit understanding
of morphological relations between word forms and meanings,
such as grammatical inflection and productive derivation, and
(b) the ability to manipulate the morphological structure of
words (Carlisle, 1995). The present study aimed to examine the
predictive value of preschool morphological and phonological
awareness in learning to read and spell.
Morphological Awareness and Literacy
Development
It has been forcefully argued that reading comprehension
cannot succeed unless the reader appreciates morphological
word formation, that is, how differences in word forms relate
to differences in meaning (Carlisle, 2003). This suggests that
an explicit understanding of morphological relations, termed
morphological awareness, is a prerequisite to skilled reading.
In fact morphological awareness is related not only to reading
comprehension, but also to spelling (e.g., Deacon et al., 2009;
Casalis et al., 2011), vocabulary (McBride-Chang et al., 2005;
Sparks and Deacon, 2015), and word and pseudoword reading
(Deacon and Kirby, 2004;Kirby et al., 2012). The contribution
of morphological awareness to spelling is robust to a multitude of
control variables (Deacon et al., 2009) and includes both inflected
and derived forms (Deacon et al., 2010) beyond the spelling of
specific morphemes (Casalis et al., 2011).
Deacon and Kirby (2004) examined the role of both
phonological and morphological awareness in learning to read
for English-speaking Canadian children. They investigated the
longitudinal prediction of Grades 3, 4, and 5 pseudoword
reading, single word reading, and reading comprehension from
Grade 2 phonological and morphological awareness. They
found that morphological awareness made a small but unique
contribution to all aspects of reading development mainly
pseudoword reading and reading comprehension during the
3 years of middle elementary school, over and beyond the
effect of phonological awareness. They argued that morphological
awareness might have accounted for more variance in the reading
variables if multiple measures of various formats and tapping
a broader range of derivations and inflections had been used.
The present study addressed this methodological limitation by
assessing children in an elaborate and systematic battery of
phonological and morphological awareness tasks.
In another study of English-speaking Canadian children,
Deacon et al. (2009) examined the predictive value of
morphological awareness, assessed in the early school years, for
the prediction of spelling, assessed in middle elementary grades.
They reported that Grade 2 morphological awareness accounted
for approximately 8% of the variance in Grade 4 general spelling
skills, beyond the effect of verbal and non-verbal intelligence,
phonological awareness, verbal short-term memory, and rapid-
automatized naming (RAN).
Few studies have studied the contribution of morphological
awareness assessed before the onset of formal reading instruction.
Casalis and Louis-Alexandre (2000) studied the longitudinal
contribution of phonological and morphological awareness to
decoding and reading comprehension. They assessed French-
speaking kindergarten children in a variety of morphological
awareness tasks measuring both inflectional and derivational
morphology. Their findings showed strong correlations between
phonological and morphological awareness tasks, as well as
unique contributions of both skills to Grade 2 decoding skills
and reading comprehension. However, they only analyzed the
correlations for individual tasks and did not examine the overall
effects of morphological and phonological awareness skills by
considering all the corresponding tasks together. Therefore,
the total magnitude of the longitudinal relationship remained
unknown.
More recently, using latent variable modeling in Chinese, Pan
et al. (2016) found that pre-literate syllable and morphological
awareness predicted character reading, reading fluency, reading
comprehension, and writing at the age of 11 years, beyond any
effects of phonological awareness, but only indirectly, that is,
through post-literate morphological awareness assessed at the
ages of 7–10 years.
The longitudinal relation between early morphological
awareness and reading and spelling skills has also been studied
in Greek. Manolitsis (2006) found that morphological awareness,
assessed in kindergarten, longitudinally predicted Grade 1 word
reading but its contribution to accuracy was not significant
when kindergarten phonological awareness was controlled for.
Pittas and Nunes (2014) assessed first and third graders in three
morphological awareness tasks: a pseudoword inflection task,
a sentence analogy task, and a morphological relatedness task.
They found a unique contribution of morphological awareness to
reading but not to spelling assessed 8 months later, even after
partialling out the effects of grade, verbal ability, phonological
awareness, and initial reading level.
Grigorakis and Manolitsis (2016) examined the longitudinal
prediction of Greek morphological spelling from morphological
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 2November 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 2039
fpsyg-08-02039 November 23, 2017 Time: 15:59 # 3
Diamanti et al. Preschool Predictors of Early Literacy
awareness measured before and at the beginning of formal
literacy instruction. They assessed 229 kindergarten children
5–6 years old on a variety of morphological awareness tasks
measuring their ability to recognize and manipulate inflections,
derivations, and compound words. Spelling of inflectional
suffixes in words and pseudowords was assessed at Grades 1
and 2. Morphological awareness was a significant longitudinal
predictor of word spelling, surviving control for verbal and
non-verbal intelligence, verbal short-term memory, receptive
and expressive vocabulary, letter sound knowledge, RAN, and
phonological awareness.
Finally, in a cross-linguistic study comparing English, French,
and Greek, Desrochers et al. (2017) found that Greek children’s
morphological awareness skills at the beginning of Grade 2 were
unique predictors of reading comprehension and spelling, but not
of reading accuracy as in English and fluency as in both
English and French at the end of the same grade.
Evidence for the importance of morphological awareness has
also been provided by intervention studies. If morphological
awareness forms a critical substrate for reading development,
then training in morphological awareness, if successful, should
lead to measurable improvements in reading performance. Due
to their experimental rather than correlational nature, studies
of morphological awareness training constitute an empirically
crucial source of evidence regarding the connection between
morphological awareness and literacy. Indeed, instruction
in morphological awareness has been shown to result in
benefits across literacy domains, especially when combined with
phonological awareness training (e.g., Lyster, 2002;Lyster et al.,
2016;Manolitsis, 2017; see meta-analyses and systematic reviews
in Reed, 2008;Bowers et al., 2010;Carlisle, 2010;Goodwin and
Ahn, 2010, 2013).
However, even though dozens of morphological awareness
studies have accumulated to date, as seen in the aforementioned
reviews, a confident conclusion remains unwarranted because
it has been challenging to establish the specificity of training.
The majority of studies have failed to employ an active control
group receiving instruction of similar structure and intensity
but non-morphological in content. Indeed, many studies have
simply compared the experimental group to a passive control
group not receiving any special instruction but following the
regular classroom program. When active control groups are
employed the benefits to literacy from morphological training are
not significantly stronger (e.g., comparing against phonological
awareness training; Lyster, 2002;Lyster et al., 2016).
An additional difficulty with the theoretical interpretation of
the majority of these training studies is that they have relied,
at least in part, on printed materials or strategies potentially
exploiting the orthographic knowledge of participants, thereby
obscuring the origin of the observed effects. That is, although
the focus of the instruction was on the morphological aspects of
words, if training took place using written words then children
may have exhibited literacy gains due to the fact that they
received a form of reading or spelling instruction rather than to
morphological awareness per se.
In sum, despite the recent surge in interest in the relationship
between morphological awareness and reading skill development,
and the strong evidence for its importance, the relevant literature
has not conclusively established the precedence, or necessity,
of morphological awareness for reading development and for
particular reading skills. Many studies have examined concurrent
correlations and most have assessed children in elementary
grades, for which reciprocal effects may have contributed to
the reported findings. That is, if morphological awareness is
assessed after the onset of reading instruction, it is possible
that exposure to the various printed word types may have
contributed to the further development of morphological
awareness. Therefore, a finding of robust correlations may
conceivably be due to an inverse direction of causation than
typically hypothesized.
Although longitudinal studies are one step toward addressing
this shortcoming, it is also critical that the first assessment
of morphological awareness takes place before the onset of
reading instruction, to minimize effects of exposure to print. This
requires the development and validation of appropriate testing
materials for preschoolers that arguably address metalinguistic
morphological skills. In the present study we have thus examined
the longitudinal prediction of early (Grade 1) reading skills by
preschool morphological awareness, controlling for phonological
awareness and vocabulary. To obtain a more nearly complete
picture of the importance of morphological awareness for reading
skill development, we have applied a comprehensive battery of
reading outcomes, including word and pseudoword accuracy,
reading fluency, reading comprehension, as well as spelling.
Development and Assessment of
Morphological Awareness
Typical language development involves unconscious use of
morphology. Very young children produce overgeneralizations,
such as “buyed” (instead of “bought”). The production of
these errors suggests a gradual development in understanding
the rules of inflectional morphology (Berko, 1958;Selby,
1972). Nonetheless, the boundary between tacit knowledge of
morphological processes and conscious morphological awareness
has not been sufficiently investigated. In many cases it is not clear
whether differences in measures of morphological awareness
reflect differences in metalinguistic awareness or in implicit
morphological knowledge (Nagy et al., 2014). Metalinguistic
awareness is thought to be a special kind of linguistic
functioning, beyond language acquisition, which develops in
middle childhood (Tunmer et al., 1984).
The morphological processes of grammatical inflection and
productive derivation seem to follow a similar but non-
simultaneous developmental progression. Evidence shows that
awareness of inflectional morphology is acquired in the first
school years (Diakogiorgi et al., 2005;Kuo and Anderson, 2006),
whereas awareness of derivational morphology develops toward
the fourth year (Anglin, 1993;Carlisle, 2000), and continues to
grow throughout the school years (Berko, 1958;Anglin, 1993;
Berninger et al., 2010). Carlisle (1995) suggested that children’s
awareness of derivational morphology makes a transition from
an implicit to an explicit level at the ages of kindergarten and first
grade.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 3November 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 2039
fpsyg-08-02039 November 23, 2017 Time: 15:59 # 4
Diamanti et al. Preschool Predictors of Early Literacy
Morphological awareness tasks have been classified according
to their cognitive and meta-cognitive requirements, which may
operate at either an implicit or an explicit level (Deacon et al.,
2008). Lexical judgment tasks, which require children to decide
whether two words are related or not, have been widely used
to assess implicit morphological skills (e.g., Mahony et al., 2000;
Duncan et al., 2009), whereas analogy and production tasks have
been used to tap explicit skills (e.g., Berko, 1958;Derwing, 1976;
Nunes et al., 1997;Carlisle, 2000;Kirby et al., 2012). Production
tasks have also been differentiated between implicit and explicit
(Casalis and Louis-Alexandre, 2000).
Diamanti et al. (in press) recently examined the development
of morphological awareness in Greek children 4–7 years old.
They compared the domains of inflectional and derivational
morphology, adopting a distinction between two levels, namely
epilinguistic control and metalinguistic awareness. Epilinguistic
control refers to an intermediate level of elementary awareness
that has been posited to intervene developmentally between
the acquisition of the linguistic skill and the acquisition
of metalinguistic awareness (Gombert, 1992). In contrast,
metalinguistic awareness refers to the individual’s ability to reflect
upon and consciously manipulate morphemes, as well as the
ability to deliberately apply word formation rules. Following
Carlisle (1995), epilinguistic control is evidenced in judgment
tasks, whereas full-blown metalinguistic awareness is evidenced
in production tasks (see Diamanti et al., in press, for further
discussion). In addition to the expected performance increase
with age, Diamanti et al. (in press) found that a single factor
sufficed and accounted for 0.59 of the variance in the four
tasks, consistent with a common developmental path underlying
both domains and both levels of morphological awareness. In
comparison of the developmental growth curves among tasks,
they found that production of derivational morphemes was
more difficult than production of inflectional morphemes and
judgment of derivational morphemes, whereas the differences
between the two inflectional tasks and between the two judgment
tasks were not significant.
Given these findings, Diamanti et al. (in press) suggested that
at these ages epilinguistic control is similarly effective for the two
morphological domains whereas full metalinguistic awareness
of derivational morphology trails behind that of inflectional
morphology, at least as measured by these specific tasks. Thus, on
the one hand, this study highlighted the need for early tracking
and distinctions among levels and domains of morphological
awareness. On the other hand, it demonstrated the reliability
and validity of the materials used and the potential of this
combination of subscales to form a reliable and coherent scale
for overall wide-range assessment of morphological awareness
in the preschool and early elementary school age range. The
present study is a follow-up of a subset of the children in that
study, who attended preschool at the time and were assessed
again 1 year later, in Grade 1, on reading-related outcome
variables.
Relevant Properties of Greek
This subsection is reproduced from Diamanti et al. (in press).
Greek is a language with rich inflectional and derivational
morphology (see Ralli, 2003) and relatively consistent
orthography (Protopapas and Vlahou, 2009). Nouns and
adjectives are obligatorily inflected for gender, number, and case
via fusional suffixation. For example, the noun χoρóς(/xoros/
“dance”) is composed of the stem χoρ (/xor/ expressing the
core semantics) and the inflectional suffix oς(/os/ signifying
masculine singular nominative case). Verb forms also include
a stem and an obligatory inflectional ending, both of which
may be simple or complex. Verbs are inflected for voice, aspect,
tense, number, and person [Ralli, 2003; see Klairis and Babiniotis
(2004) and Holton et al. (2012) for comprehensive descriptions].
For example, the verb χoρε´υω (/xorevo/ “I dance”) is composed
of the same stem χoρ (/xor/), the derivational affix ε´υ (/ev/
forming a verb from a noun), and the inflectional suffix ω(/o/
signifying first person singular).
Distinct inflectional classes are recognized for both
nouns/adjectives and verbs, each with its own set of suffixation
and stem alternation rules (Ralli, 2003, 2005;Holton et al., 2012).
Word formation in Greek also includes systematic derivational
processes, especially for nouns (based on verb stems) and
adjectives (based on verb and noun stems). Compounding is also
highly productive, as new adjectives, nouns, and verbs can be
created from existing stems and words [see Ralli (2003, 2005) for
more information].
Morphology has extensive orthographic consequences
in Greek, insofar as derivational and grammatical suffixes
are associated with specific spellings, which also serve to
disambiguate homonyms. Knowledge of the inflectional type
is often required for correct spelling of adjective, noun, and
verb suffixes [see Protopapas (2017) for more information and
references]. Therefore, it seems reasonable to hypothesize that
an understanding of morphological processes will be especially
beneficial in learning to spell, and particularly useful in spelling
the inflectional suffixes (Grigorakis and Manolitsis, 2016).
This is important in light of the fact that Greek morphological
spelling is known to be challenging, including both inflectional
and especially derivational suffixes (Protopapas et al., 2013a;
Diamanti et al., 2014).
A small amount of instructional activity related to
morphological awareness takes place informally in the
Kindergarten curriculum as part of vocabulary instruction,
in the context of shared book reading and retelling, including
discussion about word types such as diminutive derivation and
number inflection, along with phonological awareness activities
such as letter–sound association and identification. Systematic
decoding is taught in Grade 1, so that most children are able to
read by mid-grade, after which point some instruction related
to morphological awareness appears, for example teaching
the distinct spellings of noun and verb vowel endings (i.e.,
inflectional suffixes).
Most Greek children have mastered the inflectional paradigms
of the language to a large extent by the age of entering
elementary education, at least as far as the suffixes with
orthographic consequences are concerned (i.e., case, gender, and
number, for adjectives and nouns, and person and number,
for verbs). Normally developing kindergarten children approach
ceiling performance in the production of verb past tense
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 4November 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 2039
fpsyg-08-02039 November 23, 2017 Time: 15:59 # 5
Diamanti et al. Preschool Predictors of Early Literacy
and noun gender, number, and case (Mastropavlou, 2006)
although persistent difficulties with verb aspectual formation
and noun gender are observed in certain word classes with
unusual properties (Stavrakaki and Clahsen, 2009;Varlokosta
and Nerantzini, 2013, 2015). Thus, morphological acquisition is
largely but not entirely completed by Grade 1.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Participants
The study sample consisted of 104 children (54 girls and 50
boys) assessed at the middle of kindergarten (February–March;
age M=67.3 months; SD =3.6) and again at the end of
Grade 1 (April–May; about 14 months later). They were native
speakers of Greek and did not have any diagnosed developmental
delay or emotional disorder prohibiting them from enrollment
in typical (general) education settings. They were recruited from
schools in rural (17%), semi-urban (19%), and urban (63%)
areas of four geographically dispersed provinces of Greece,
including a variety of socioeconomic and ethnic backgrounds.
Sample demographics represent a close approximation to the
Greek population (77% urban and 23% rural) based on the 2011
census.
Permission to conduct the study in these public schools
was granted by the Ministry of Education following formal
review and approval of the study plan by the Research Office of
the Educational Policy Institute. Parental and school approval,
as well as the child’s oral assent, were obtained prior to test
administration. Participants were not specifically selected; rather,
consent forms were distributed to entire classrooms and children
who returned the signed parental consent were included in the
study.
Materials
Time 1 (predictor) measures included receptive and expressive
vocabulary, and phonological and morphological awareness.
These tasks were administered through an Android application
(app) for mobile devices (tablets) featuring automatic application
of ceiling rules. Time 2 (outcome) measures included word
and pseudoword reading accuracy, text reading fluency and
comprehension, and spelling. The four reading outcome
measures were from 1A1A, a standardized reading test by
Padeliadu et al. (in press).
Receptive Vocabulary
Four different images were displayed while a recorded spoken
word was played out by the app, and the child was asked
to choose the image that best represented the word that was
heard. The three other images corresponded to a word from
the same semantic category, a phonologically similar word,
and an unrelated word. Words were appropriate for children
in preschool and early elementary grades (including animals,
objects, actions, adjectives, abstract concepts, etc.) and were
presented in order of increasing difficulty (determined by Rasch
analysis of pilot data from 237 children on 65 original items).
Scoring was recorded automatically, amounting to the number
of correct responses. The number of items was N=30 and
the reliability of the scale (Cronbach’s coefficient of internal
consistency) was α=0.88.
Expressive Vocabulary
This was a word definition task, in which each child was
asked to give a brief definition of a series of words. Words
were selected to cover a range of abilities for children in
preschool and early elementary grades, including a variety of
semantic and grammatical categories (animals, food, professions,
objects, actions, abstract concepts, etc.), based on the results
of a pilot study (parallel to that for receptive vocabulary,
with 50 original items). Manual off-line scoring matched other
similar tasks (i.e., WISC vocabulary), such that a proper word
definition received two points, whereas examples of word
use or descriptions were scored with 1 or 0, depending on
word understanding and richness of expression (N=28;
α=0.91).
Phonological Awareness
This was a composite score corresponding to the total number of
items correctly responded to in a series of eight tasks assessing
initial syllable matching (n=7 items; Cronbach’s α=0.84),
initial phoneme matching (n=7; α=0.84), syllable blending
(n=5; α=0.89), phoneme blending (n=7; α=0.93), syllable
segmentation (n=6; α=0.95), phoneme segmentation (n=7;
α=0.95), syllable deletion (n=7; α=0.94), and phoneme
deletion (n=7; α=0.92). For the total scale, as entered in the
analyses, N=53, α=0.97.
In the initial syllable (or phoneme) matching tasks, children
heard the label of a displayed target image and the labels of three
other simultaneously displayed images and had to choose which
of the three images began with the same syllable (phoneme)
as the target image. In the blending tasks, children had to
compose words from a series of syllables (phonemes) that were
heard individually. In the syllable (phoneme) segmentation tasks
children heard a word and were then asked to pronounce
the individual syllables (phonemes) it comprised. Finally, in
the syllable (phoneme) deletion, children were asked to listen
carefully to a word and then to repeat it omitting a specific syllable
(phoneme).
Morphological Awareness
This was a composite score corresponding to the total number of
items correctly responded to in a series of three tasks assessing
judgment (n=8 items; α=0.80) and production (n=11 items;
α=0.73) of inflectional suffixes and production of derivational
suffixes (n=16 items; α=0.94). For the total scale, as entered in
the analyses, N=35, α=0.93. The following description of the
tasks is based on Diamanti et al. (in press).
Inflectional morphemes judgment task
Children saw a picture displaying either one or two turtles
performing an action while listening to two sentences spoken by
two penguin figures displayed next to the action picture. Children
had to choose the sentence matching the picture by pointing at
one of the two penguins after hearing the sentences. Each pair
of sentences contained one pseudoword differing in inflectional
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 5November 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 2039
fpsyg-08-02039 November 23, 2017 Time: 15:59 # 6
Diamanti et al. Preschool Predictors of Early Literacy
suffix, which was either singular or plural. For example, given
a picture of two turtles taking photographs, the two sentences
were “the turtles skeni3rd.sg photos” and “the turtles skenoun3rd.pl
photos.” The correct sentence is the second one because the
inflectional suffix of the pseudoverb denotes the plural form and
agrees with the subject, thus matching the picture. Given a picture
of a turtle holding two rulers, the two sentences were “the turtle
is holding theacc.sg serapaacc.sg and “the turtle is holding theacc.pl
serapesacc.pl (the critical pseudoword is denoted by italics). The
correct sentence is the second one because the inflectional suffix
of the pseudonoun denotes the plural form and matches the
picture.
Inflectional morphemes production task
Children saw a pair of pictures, illustrating actions performed
by turtles differing in the number of agents or patients of
the depicted action, while listening to a verbal description
including a pseudoword (a pseudoverb in eight sentences,
for the action, and a pseudo-noun in three sentences, for
the object). Children were then provided with the beginning
of a second sentence, matching the second picture, up to
the subject of the verb, and were asked to change the
pseudoword number (from singular to plural or from plural
to singular) accordingly. For example, given a picture of
two turtles with sunglasses and a picture of one turtle with
sunglasses, the sentence and prompt would be “The turtles
menane3rd.pl glasses. The turtle. . . and the child should say
menai3rd.sg glasses”; given a picture of a turtle waving at a
monkey and a picture of a turtle waving at two monkeys, the
sentence and prompt would be “The turtle is greeting theacc.sg
reipouacc.sg. The turtle is greeting theacc.pl and the child should
say reipoudesacc.pl (the critical pseudoword is denoted by
italics).
Derivational morphemes production task
Children saw a picture while listening to a sentence with a critical
word (a different one for each sentence) and the beginning of
a second sentence that was syntactically altered and required
manipulation of a derivational morpheme on the critical word
to be completed correctly (e.g., “The sea deepens. The sea is. . .
requiring “deep”; “Miriam always teases her friends. Miriam is
a. . . requiring “teaser” /piraxtiri/, derived from /pirazo/). The
task targeted a variety of derivational morphemes, denoting
property, profession, establishment/institution, material,
collection, comparatives, action, device, nationality/origin,
etc.
Word Reading Accuracy
The word decoding test of the 1A1A decoding subscale was
used, which consists of 57 words two to seven syllables long,
with gradually increasing number of syllables and semantic
complexity and decreasing frequency of occurrence, printed
vertically. Words were nouns, adjectives, passive participles,
and verbs. A stopping criterion of five consecutive errors
was applied. The number of words read correctly was
noted. The internal consistency of the entire “decoding”
factor of 1A1A (which also includes pseudoword decoding,
word/pseudoword discrimination, and word identification) as
reported for elementary grades is high (ω=0.90, H=0.91).
Pseudoword Reading Accuracy
The pseudoword decoding subtest of the 1A1A decoding
subscale was used, which consists of 40 non-words two to six
syllables long, with gradually increasing number of syllables and
phonological complexity, printed vertically. A stopping criterion
of five consecutive errors was applied. The number of non-words
read correctly was noted.
Reading Fluency
A grade-appropriate 247-word passage with an ancient Greek
mythological theme from the reading fluency subscale of 1A1A
was used. Children were asked to read the passage as quickly and
as accurately as they could. The score of the test was the number
of words read correctly within 1 min.
Reading Comprehension
The first three passages from the reading comprehension subscale
of 1A1A were used, which were short and appropriate for the
age of the participants, with gradually increasing semantic and
syntactic difficulty. The first and second passages were narratives,
while the third one was expository. Children had to answer
seven multiple-choice questions for each passage while having
the texts available. The questions required meaning abstraction
based on vocabulary knowledge, as well as literal and inferencing
skills. The score was the total number of questions answered
correctly for all three passages (out of a total of 21 questions).
The internal consistency of the entire “comprehension” factor
of 1A1A (which includes three more passages, for a total of
six) as reported for elementary grades is satisfactory (ω=0.89,
H=0.64).
Spelling
Spelling ability was assessed using a standardized spelling-
to-dictation test (Mouzaki et al., 2010), which includes 60
words dictated in isolation and in a sentence at a child-
determined pace. A stopping criterion of six consecutive errors
was applied. Each word was scored with one point for accurate
spelling.
Procedure
All measures were administered individually by specially trained
research assistants, following a common procedure, in a quiet
room at the children’s kindergarten (Time 1) or school (Time
2). Time 1 (predictor) measures were administered in two to
three sessions of 40–45 min within 2 weeks (in the context of
a variety of other tasks not reported here) using a tablet app
custom made for this purpose. All visual and auditory stimuli
were provided by the app as images and pre-recorded utterances.
Scoring was automated when possible (i.e., evaluation of selection
accuracy), or entered manually after administration when human
judgment was necessary (i.e., evaluation of spoken responses).
Time 2 (outcome) measures were administered individually in
one 40–45-min-long session in the traditional (paper and pencil)
format.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 6November 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 2039
fpsyg-08-02039 November 23, 2017 Time: 15:59 # 7
Diamanti et al. Preschool Predictors of Early Literacy
RESULTS
There were no missing data for this group of participants
(N=104 in all analyses). Visual examination of univariate
quantile–quantile plots and bivariate scatterplots revealed six
extreme outliers (two low values in receptive vocabulary, one low
and one high in fluency, and two high in spelling), which were
replaced by winsorized values at the appropriate percentile (1/N
for single values and 2/Nfor two values) in order to retain a
full data set. Table 1 displays descriptive statistics following this
minor cleanup. Despite some mild deviations from normality,
no extreme values of skew or kurtosis were observed. Table 2
displays the intercorrelations among all variables. Age was not
significantly correlated with morphological awareness (r=0.033,
p=0.740) or with any of the outcome variables (all p>0.11),
probably due to the restricted age range in this sample. Therefore,
age was not entered as a predictor in the regression models.
For each outcome variable, a hierarchical regression analysis
was conducted in three steps: receptive and expressive vocabulary
were entered at the first step, as proxies for language development
and verbal ability in general; phonological awareness was
entered at the second step, and morphological awareness at
the third and final step. This was done in order to quantify
the specific contribution of metalinguistic skills beyond general
language skills, and in particular the specific contribution of
morphological awareness beyond the already well known
effect of phonological awareness, which in this way also acts as a
proxy for general metalinguistic skill. Table 3 displays the results
of these analyses, including the total and additional variance
accounted for at each step (rightmost columns), the coefficients
in the final multiple regression models for each outcome variable
including all predictors (leftmost columns), and the proportions
of shared and unique variance accounted for by each predictor in
the final models (commonality analysis; middle columns).
Residual diagnostics are shown in Figure 1, indicating no
severe deviations from normality and no overly influential
data points. There was a significant unique contribution of
morphological awareness, beyond vocabulary and phonological
TABLE 1 | Descriptive statistics for predictor and dependent variables.
Shapiro–Wilks
Variable M M% mdn SD Min Max W p Skewness Kurtosis
Preschool (predictor) variables
Age (months) 67.3 67.0 3.6 56 74 0.965 0.007 0.22 0.44
Receptive vocabulary 23.7 79.0 25.0 4.6 10 30 0.902 <0.001 1.11 0.85
Expressive vocabulary 25.2 90.0 26.5 8.6 3 44 0.986 0.340 0.33 0.28
Phonological awareness 26.5 50.0 24.0 10.3 2 51 0.976 0.053 0.34 0.18
Morphological awareness 20.1 57.4 21.0 7.9 5 34 0.946 <0.001 0.28 1.13
Grade 1 (outcome) variables
Word accuracy 38.8 68.1 43.0 13.4 5 57 0.890 <0.001 0.95 0.15
Pseudoword accuracy 28.1 70.3 30.0 8.3 5 40 0.910 <0.001 1.04 0.70
Reading fluency 41.7 39.0 16.3 9 93 0.958 0.002 0.77 1.08
Reading comprehension 14.4 68.5 15.0 3.8 2 21 0.954 0.001 0.73 0.53
Spelling 15.2 25.3 15.0 5.2 4 29 0.973 0.029 0.44 0.50
M% =mean percent correct; mdn =median; Min and Max refer to the lowest and highest observed value, respectively, after winsorization of outliers (see text).
Shapiro–Wilks test of normality. For all measures, number of participants N =104.
TABLE 2 | Intercorrelations among all variables.
Variable 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10
Preschool (predictor) variables
(1) Age (months) 0.272 0.179 0.229 0.033 0.031 0.057 0.082 0.148 0.155
(2) Receptive vocabulary 0.420 0.270 0.307 0.160 0.211 0.230 0.440 0.206
(3) Expressive vocabulary 0.251 0.229 0.042 0.103 0.049 0.318 0.039
(4) Phonological awareness 0.472 0.316 0.363 0.409 0.298 0.431
(5) Morphological awareness 0.482 0.473 0.284 0.470 0.392
Grade 1 (outcome) variables
(6) Word accuracy 0.773 0.525 0.392 0.452
(7) Pseudoword accuracy 0.459 0.376 0.423
(8) Reading fluency 0.332 0.693
(9) Reading comprehension 0.360
(10) Spelling
Pearson’s r correlation coefficients; N =104; correlations of 0.193 or greater are significant at p <0.05; and greater than 0.273 at p <0.005.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 7November 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 2039
fpsyg-08-02039 November 23, 2017 Time: 15:59 # 8
Diamanti et al. Preschool Predictors of Early Literacy
awareness, to every outcome variable except fluency, for
which only phonological awareness made a significant unique
contribution. The unique contribution of morphological
awareness was sizeable (9–14% of variance, depending on
outcome measure) and was accompanied by additional,
comparable proportions of variance (9–15%) shared with the
other measures, bringing up the total longitudinal predicted
variance from morphological awareness to more than 20% of
reading (and spelling) outcomes (except fluency).
DISCUSSION
In this longitudinal study we have investigated the prediction
of reading and spelling outcomes near the end of Grade 1
by language and metalinguistic skills assessed in preschool
14 months earlier. Morphological awareness had a significant
unique contribution to all outcome variables except reading
fluency. This finding confirms the important role of
morphological awareness for reading development and extends
it to a younger age than usually studied.
Our results are consistent with the findings of Casalis and
Louis-Alexandre (2000), who studied early reading performance
longitudinally predicted by preschool phonological and
morphological awareness in French, and found both a strong
correlation between phonological and morphological awareness
at these ages as well as longitudinal relationships between both
of them and early reading. Our results are also compatible
with those of Grigorakis and Manolitsis (2016), who examined
the prediction of Grade 1 inflectional spelling by preschool
phonological and morphological awareness in Greek, and
found a significant longitudinal contribution of morphological
awareness beyond phonological awareness and other control
variables.
In particular with respect to spelling, one might expect an
especially important role of morphological awareness in Greek
(Grigorakis and Manolitsis, 2016), because, as noted above, many
inflectional and derivational affixes are associated with specific
spellings (and, indeed, some of them are homophonous and
can only be disambiguated by spelling). This hypothesis cannot
be evaluated in the current study because our strong result
(1R2=0.085, p<0.001) emerged using a standardized spelling
test including many words with difficult stems and not giving
particular weight to grammatical (i.e., inflectional suffix) spelling.
This might be taken to imply that the relationship between
morphological awareness and spelling is not specific to suffixes.
However, our results do not speak to the issue of a suffix-specific
relationship: It may well be the case that morphological awareness
is especially necessary or beneficial for spelling inflectional
suffixes, and this could only be discerned in comparison with
TABLE 3 | Results of regression analyses for the longitudinal prediction of Grade 1 reading skills.
Multiple regression Commonality (variance) Hierarchical regression
Preschool predictor βpUnique Common Total Step R21R2p
Word accuracy
Receptive vocabulary 0.018 0.951 <0.001 0.017 0.017 1 0.018 0.410
Expressive vocabulary 0.148 0.336 0.007 0.005 0.002
Phonological awareness 0.169 0.198 0.013 0.087 0.100 2 0.106 0.089 0.002
Morphological awareness 0.748 <0.001 0.144 0.088 0.232 3 0.250 0.144 <0.001
Pseudoword accuracy
Receptive vocabulary 0.047 0.794 0.001 0.029 0.030 1 0.031 0.207
Expressive vocabulary 0.042 0.659 0.002 0.009 0.011
Phonological awareness 0.147 0.071 0.025 0.107 0.132 2 0.138 0.108 0.001
Morphological awareness 0.409 <0.001 0.112 0.112 0.224 3 0.251 0.112 <0.001
Fluency
Receptive vocabulary 0.663 0.065 0.027 0.055 0.081 1 0.081 0.014
Expressive vocabulary 0.174 0.354 0.007 0.005 0.012
Phonological awareness 0.527 0.001 0.084 0.108 0.192 2 0.227 0.146 <0.001
Morphological awareness 0.333 0.115 0.019 0.106 0.125 3 0.247 0.019 0.115
Comprehension
Receptive vocabulary 0.215 0.007 0.052 0.127 0.179 1 0.202 <0.001
Expressive vocabulary 0.052 0.208 0.011 0.090 0.101
Phonological awareness 0.012 0.731 0.001 0.088 0.089 2 0.231 0.029 0.056
Morphological awareness 0.168 <0.001 0.091 0.130 0.221 3 0.322 0.091 <0.001
Spelling
Receptive vocabulary 0.170 0.115 0.017 0.057 0.074 1 0.075 0.019
Expressive vocabulary 0.079 0.163 0.014 0.007 0.007
Phonological awareness 0.143 0.003 0.061 0.144 0.206 2 0.240 0.165 <0.001
Morphological awareness 0.223 0.001 0.085 0.153 0.238 3 0.325 0.085 0.001
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 8November 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 2039
fpsyg-08-02039 November 23, 2017 Time: 15:59 # 9
Diamanti et al. Preschool Predictors of Early Literacy
appropriately designed spelling tests assessing performance on
particular kinds of suffixes. Such studies should be performed
with older children, because suffix-specific spelling knowledge is
taught after Grade 1. At any rate, our findings suggest that there is
also a more general sense in which early metalinguistic awareness
supports the development of spelling skill. Whether this relates
to language or cognitive skills required for metalinguistic task
performance is not known. Future studies must use appropriate
latent constructs to examine whether these observed longitudinal
relationships are direct or mediated by other, more general,
constructs.
Our findings seem to be somewhat at odds with those of
Manolitsis (2006), who found that preschool morphological
awareness longitudinally predicted Grade 1 single word reading
speed, but not accuracy, after controlling for phonological
awareness. We have not measured single word reading speed, so
this finding is not directly comparable to our measure of text
reading fluency. The difference in the longitudinal prediction
of word reading accuracy is difficult to explain conclusively
without more information; it may be attributable to differences
in the task content or task reliabilities. In particular, two of the
morphological awareness tests used by Manolitsis had internal
reliabilities less than 0.70, whereas the third one was a compound
inversion task, unlike the ones we used here. Despite these
differences, Manolitsis also found largely shared longitudinal
contributions from preschool phonological and morphological
awareness to Grade 1 word reading. In other words his general
pattern of findings was not inconsistent with ours.
Vocabulary made a significant unique contribution only in
the prediction of reading comprehension, and this was largely
accounted for by the receptive (picture selection) rather than the
expressive (verbal definitions) measure. This finding is consistent
with the role of vocabulary in the development of reading
comprehension that has been revealed in middle elementary
grades in Greek (Protopapas et al., 2007, 2013b). Vocabulary
was not related to Grade 1 reading accuracy performance, even
when entered in the first step of the regression. In contrast,
its significant Step 1 contribution to fluency and spelling was
eventually trumped by morphological awareness due to shared
variance related to these outcomes. This suggests that these
morphological awareness tests capture language skills variance
that is relevant for reading development at this age (cf. Hjetland
et al., submitted).
It has long been known that phonological and morphological
awareness share much of their variance at this age (e.g., Carlisle
and Nomanbhoy, 1993) and thus it is no surprise that their
contribution to reading performance is largely shared (e.g.,
Manolitsis, 2006). In our study, phonological awareness made
a significant contribution to all reading outcomes (marginal
for comprehension) when entered after vocabulary, as expected.
However, this was only significant for fluency and spelling, in
which it included a substantial unique contribution (6–8%). In
contrast, the contribution of phonological awareness to word and
pseudoword accuracy and reading comprehension was largely
shared with morphological awareness, ending up non-significant
in the final multiple regression models. In particular, the unique
contribution of phonological awareness to word and pseudoword
reading accuracy, in the presence of morphological awareness,
was less than 3% of the variance. One way to interpret this, going
beyond any shared content between materials in phonological
and morphological awareness tasks, is to consider the extent
to which these morphological awareness tests may also capture
more general metalinguistic skill variance that is relevant for
learning to read.
This finding raises the interesting possibility that the
predictive power of phonological awareness for reading
development may not be entirely due to its phonological nature
but perhaps in part because it concerns metalinguistic skill,
which, in turn, depends on earlier language skill development.
It will be necessary to examine whether this finding holds up in
follow-up research, in Greek and other languages, and in a wider
range of ages. One reason it has not been found in the few studies
that have examined the longitudinal prediction of early reading
outcomes by preschool skills may have to do with psychometric
issues. Specifically, tests of morphological awareness tend to be
of lower reliability than tests of phonological awareness, and
FIGURE 1 | Multiple regression diagnostics. (Top) Quantile–quantile plots of standardized residuals for the longitudinal prediction of each outcome variable.
(Bottom) Corresponding leverage-residual plots with overlaid smooth trend, also displaying Cook’s distance curves at values of 0.2 and 0.3.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 9November 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 2039
fpsyg-08-02039 November 23, 2017 Time: 15:59 # 10
Diamanti et al. Preschool Predictors of Early Literacy
therefore may not pick up all the variance that can properly be
attributed to a well-defined morphological awareness construct
due to measurement noise. Our study stands out for the very high
reliability of both the phonological and morphological awareness
measures, allowing the regression models to capture substantial
proportions of the reliable variance in the dependent variables.
Ideally, future studies should include multiple highly reliable
tasks as indices of corresponding latent constructs in order to
examine the relative contribution of different metalinguistic skills
to early reading outcomes as free from measurement noise as
possible.
In this work we have treated phonological and morphological
awareness as unitary constructs, by combining responses from
multiple subtasks examining specific aspects of these domains.
This methodological choice is supported by the very high
reliability of the aggregated tasks. It is also supported by
strong evidence in favor of phonological awareness being a
unidimensional construct (e.g., Schatschneider et al., 1999;
Anthony and Lonigan, 2004; also in Greek: Papadopoulos et al.,
2009, 2012). Similarly, with respect to the morphological tasks,
covering both inflectional and derivational morphology, and
both judgment and production tasks, Diamanti et al. (in press)
found that a single factor sufficed and accounted for 59% of the
total variance, consistent with a unidimensional construct for
morphological awareness as well [Muse (2005) as cited in Tighe
and Schatschneider (2015)].
Our study joins the long list of studies, mentioned in
the introduction, in suggesting that an explicit understanding
of linguistic structure is substantially predictive of future
reading performance. It provides an important confirmation
of the importance of morphological awareness for reading
development, by testing preliterate children, for whom a
reverse effect (of reading experience on the development of
morphological awareness) is unlikely, and by employing highly
reliable tests covering different aspects of the target construct,
such as a variety of suffixes and functions and tasks of different
formats and demands. In addition, our findings bring out
differences in the relevance of phonological and morphological
awareness for the prediction of different reading (and spelling)
tasks, at least for the age tested, that is, beginner readers.
Finally, the present study raises the intriguing possibility that
the general cognitive demands of metalinguistic tasks may be of
utmost importance for the prediction of reading development,
whereas the linguistic content of the tasks may be of secondary
importance or critical for specific associations with particular
reading skills. Given the increasing prominence of morphological
awareness study in the reading literature, we expect that this issue
will be further investigated and clarified in future comprehensive
studies.
ETHICS STATEMENT
This study was carried out in accordance with the guidelines of
the American Psychological Association for treatment of human
participants, with written informed consent obtained from the
parents (or legal guardians) of all children participants, approval
by the school authorities, and oral assent of the children prior
to test administration. The protocol was approved by the Greek
Ministry of Education, following positive recommendation of the
Institute of Educational Policy.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
VD conceptualized this study. VD, AM, AR, FA, and SP
contributed to the design and implementation of data collection.
AP conducted the statistical analysis of the data. VD and
AP drafted the manuscript. All authors have contributed to
the writing and revising of the manuscript and agree to be
accountable for the content of the work.
FUNDING
This research was supported in part by a postdoctoral
research fellowship to VD in the context of research program
“The Foundation of Reading and Writing in a Transparent
Orthography: Oral Language Development and Early Literacy
Skills” funded by the University of Crete Special Account, PI: AM.
REFERENCES
Aidinis, A., and Nunes, T. (2001). The role of different levels of phonological
awareness in the development of reading and spelling in Greek. Read. Writ.
14, 145–177. doi: 10.1023/A:1008107312006
Anglin, J. M. (1993). Vocabulary Development: A Morphological Analysis.
(Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58). Chicago, IL:
University of Chicago Press.
Anthony, J. L., and Lonigan, C. J. (2004). The nature of phonological awareness:
converging evidence from four studies of preschool and early grade school
children. J. Educ. Psychol. 96, 43–55. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.43
Berko, J. (1958). The child’s learning of English morphology. Word 14, 150–177.
doi: 10.1080/00437956.1958.11659661
Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Nagy, W., and Carlisle, J. (2010). Growth
in phonological, orthographic, and morphological awareness in grades
1 to 6. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 39, 141–163. doi: 10.1007/s10936-009-
9130-6
Bowers, P. N., Kirby, J. R., and Deacon, S. H. (2010). The effects of morphological
instruction on literacy skills: a systematic review of the literature. Rev. Educ. Res.
80, 144–179. doi: 10.3102/0034654309359353
Byrne, B. (1996). The learnability of the alphabetic principle: children’s initial
hypothesis about how print represents spoken language. Appl. Psycholinguist.
17, 401–426. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400008171
Byrne, B., and Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1991). Evaluation of a program to teach
phonemic awareness to young children. J. Educ. Psychol. 83, 451–455.
doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.83.4.451
Byrne, B., and Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1993). Evaluation of a program to teach
phonemic awareness to young children: a 1-year follow-up. J. Educ. Psychol.
85, 104–111. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.85.1.104
Caravolas, M., Hulme, C., and Snowling, M. J. (2001). The foundations of spelling
ability: evidence from a 3-year longitudinal study. J. Mem. Lang. 45, 751–774.
doi: 10.1006/jmla.2000.2785
Caravolas, M., Volín, J., and Hulme, C. (2005). Phoneme awareness is a
key component of alphabetic literacy skills in consistent and inconsistent
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 10 November 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 2039
fpsyg-08-02039 November 23, 2017 Time: 15:59 # 11
Diamanti et al. Preschool Predictors of Early Literacy
orthographies: evidence from Czech and English children. J. Exp. Child Psychol.
92, 107–139. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.04.003
Cardoso-Martins, C., and Pennington, B. F. (2004). The relationship between
phoneme awareness and rapid serial naming skills and literacy acquisition: the
role of developmental period and reading ability. Sci. Stud. Read. 8, 27–52.
doi: 10.1207/s1532799xssr0801_3
Carlisle, J. F. (1995). “Morphological awareness and early reading achievement, in
Morphological Aspects of Language Processing, ed. L. B. Feldman (Hillsdale, NJ:
Erlbaum), 189–209.
Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically
complex words: impact on reading. Read. Writ. 12, 169–190. doi: 10.1023/A:
1008131926604
Carlisle, J. F. (2003). Morphology matters in learning to read: a commentary. Read.
Psychol. 24, 291–322. doi: 10.1080/02702710390227369
Carlisle, J. F. (2010). Effects of instruction in morphological awareness on literacy
achievement: an integrative review. Read. Res. Q. 45, 464–487. doi: 10.1598/
RRQ.45.4.5
Carlisle, J. F., and Nomanbhoy, D. M. (1993). Phonological and morphological
awareness in first graders. Appl. Psycholinguist. 14, 177–195. doi: 10.1017/
S0142716400009541
Casalis, S., Deacon, S. H., and Pacton, S. (2011). How specific is the
connection between morphological awareness and spelling? A study of
French children. Appl. Psycholinguist. 32, 499–511. doi: 10.1017/S01427164110
0018X
Casalis, S., and Louis-Alexandre, M.-F. (2000). Morphological analysis,
phonological analysis and learning to read French: a longitudinal study.
Read. Writ. 12, 303–335. doi: 10.1023/A:1008177205648
Cataldo, S., and Ellis, N. (1988). Interactions in the development of spelling,
reading and phonological skills. J. Res. Read. 11, 86–109. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-
9817.1988.tb00153.x
Deacon, S. H., and Bryant, P. (2005). What young children do and do not
know about the spelling of inflections and derivations. Dev. Sci. 8, 583–594.
doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00449.x
Deacon, S. H., and Bryant, P. (2006). This turnip’s not for turning: children’s
morphological awareness and their use of root morphemes in spelling. Br. J.
Dev. Psychol. 24, 567–575. doi: 10.1348/026151005X50834
Deacon, S. H., Campbell, E., Tamminga, M., and Kirby, J. (2010). Seeing the harm
in harmed and harmful: morphological processing by children in Grades 4, 6,
and 8. Appl. Psycholinguist. 31, 759–775. doi: 10.1017/S0142716410000238
Deacon, S. H., Kieffer, M. J., and Laroche, A. (2014). The relation between
morphological awareness and reading comprehension: evidence from
mediation and longitudinal models. Sci. Stud. Read. 18, 432–451.
doi: 10.1080/10888438.2014.926907
Deacon, S. H., and Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: just “more
phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness
in reading development. Appl. Psycholinguist. 25, 223–238. doi: 10.1017/
S0142716404001110
Deacon, S. H., Kirby, J. R., and Casselman-Bell, M. (2009). How robust is the
contribution of morphological awareness to general spelling outcomes? Read.
Psychol. 30, 301–318.
Deacon, S. H., Parrila, R., and Kirby, J. R. (2008). “A review of the evidence
on morphological processing in dyslexics and poor readers: a strength or
weakness?, in The Sage Handbook of Dyslexia, eds G. Reid, A. J. Fawcett, F.
Manis, and L. S. Siegel (London: Sage), 212–237.
Derwing, B. L. (1976). Morpheme recognition and the learning of rules of
derivational morphology. Rev. Can. Linguist. 21, 38–66.
Desrochers, A., Manolitsis, G., Gaudreau, P., and Georgiou, G. (2017). Early
contribution of morphological awareness to literacy skills across languages
varying in orthographic consistency. Read. Writ. doi: 10.1007/s11145-017-
9772-y
Diakogiorgi, K., Baris, T., and Valmas, T. (2005). Iκανóτητα χρ0
ησης
µoρϕoλoγικ0
ων στρατηγικ0
ηνστην oρθoγραϕηµ0
ενηγραϕ0
η απóµαθητ0
ες
της A0τ0
αξης τoν δηµoτικo0
υ[Ability to use morphological strategies in spelling
by children in the first elementary grade]. Psychology 12, 568–586.
Diamanti, V., Benaki, A., Mouzaki, A., Ralli, A., Antoniou, F., Papaioannou, S.,
et al. (in press). Development of early morphological awareness in
Greek: epilinguistic versus metalinguistic and inflectional versus derivational
awareness. Appl. Psycholinguist. doi: 10.1017/S0142716417000522
Diamanti, V., Goulandris, N., Stuart, M., and Campbell, R. (2014). Spelling of
derivational and inflectional suffixes by Greek-speaking children with and
without dyslexia. Read. Writ. 27, 337–358. doi: 10.1007/s11145-013- 9447-2
Duncan, L. G., Casalis, S., and Colé, P. (2009). Early metalinguistic awareness
of derivational morphology: observations from a comparison of English and
French. Appl. Psycholinguist. 30, 405–440. doi: 10.1017/S0142716409090213
Ellis, N., and Cataldo, S. (1990). The role of spelling in learning to read. Lang. Educ.
4, 1–28. doi: 10.1080/09500789009541270
Furnes, B., and Samuelsson, S. (2010). Predicting reading and spelling difficulties
in transparent and opaque orthographies: a comparison between Scandinavian
and US/Australian children. Dyslexia 16, 119–142. doi: 10.1002/dys.401
Gombert, J. E. (1992). Metalinguistic Development. Chicago, IL: University of
Chicago Press.
Goodwin, A. P., and Ahn, S. (2010). A meta-analysis of morphological
interventions: effects on literacy achievement of children with literacy
difficulties. Ann. Dyslexia 60, 183–208. doi: 10.1007/s11881-010- 0041-x
Goodwin, A. P., and Ahn, S. (2013). A meta-analysis of morphological
interventions in English: effects on literacy outcomes for school-age children.
Sci. Stud. Read. 17, 257–285. doi: 10.1007/s11881-010-0041-x
Grigorakis, I., and Manolitsis, G. (2016). H σνµβoλ0
η της µoρϕoλoγικ0
ης στα
πρiτα στ0
αδια αν0
ςπτυξης της ικανóτητας oρθoγραϕηµ0
eνης γραϕ0
ης [The
contribution of morphological awareness on the early stages of spelling
development]. Presch. Prim. Educ. 4, 128–148. doi: 10.12681/ppej.8581
Holton, D., Mackridge, P., Philippaki-Warburton, I., and Spyropoulos, V. (2012).
Greek: A Comprehensive Grammar, 2nd Edn. London: Routledge.
Kirby, J. R., Deacon, S. H., Bowers, P. N., Izenberg, L., Wade-Woolley, L., and
Parrila, R. (2012). Children’s morphological awareness and reading ability.
Read. Writ. 25, 389–410. doi: 10.1007/s11145-010- 9276-5
Klairis, C., and Babiniotis, G. (2004). 0ραµµατικ0
η της ν0
eαςελληνικ0
ης:
1oµoλειτoυργικ0
η-επικoινωνιακ0
η[Grammar of Modern Greek:
Structural/Functional-Communicative]. Athens: Ellinika Grammata.
Kuo, L. J., and Anderson, R. C. (2006). Morphological awareness and learning to
read: a cross-language perspective. Educ. Psychol. 41, 161–180. doi: 10.1207/
s15326985ep4103_3
Lonigan, C. J., Anthony, J. L., Phillips, B. M., Purpura, D. J., Wilson, S. B.,
and McQueen, J. D. (2009). The nature of preschool phonological processing
abilities and their relations to vocabulary, general cognitive abilities, and print
knowledge. J. Educ. Psychol. 101, 345–358. doi: 10.1037/a0013837
Lyster, S.-A. H. (2002). The effects of morphological versus phonological awareness
training in kindergarten on reading development. Read. Writ. 15, 261–294.
doi: 10.1023/A:1015272516220
Lyster, S.-A. H., Lervåg, A., and Hulme, C. (2016). Preschool morphological
training produces long-term improvements in reading comprehension. Read.
Writ. 29, 1269–1288. doi: 10.1007/s11145-016- 9636-x
Mahony, D., Singson, M., and Mann, V. (2000). Reading ability and sensitivity
to morphological relations. Read. Writ. 12, 191–218. doi: 10.1023/A:
1008136012492
Manolitsis, G. (2006). “H σχ0
εση της µoρϕoλoγικ0
ης επιγνωσης µε την
αναγνωστικ0
η ικανóτητα κατ0
α τα πρ0
ωτα στ0
αδια αν0
απτυξ0
ης της. [The
relation of morphological awareness with early reading ability], in Proceedings
of the 26th Annual Meeting of the Department of Linguistics: Studies in Greek
linguistics, eds D. Koutsogianis, A. Haralabopoulos, and N. Liosis (Thessaloniki:
Aristotle University of Thessaloniki), 282–293.
Manolitsis, G. (2017). “How effective is morphological awareness instruction on
early literacy skills?, in Literacy in the Early Years: Reflections on International
Research and Practice, eds C. J. MacLachlan and A. W. Arrow (Singapore:
Springer), 151–174.
Mastropavlou, M. (2006). The Role of Phonological Salience and Feature
Interpretability in the Grammar of Typically Developing and Language
Impaired Children. Doctoral dissertation, Aristotle University of Thessaloniki,
Thessaloniki.
McBride-Chang, C., Wagner,R., Muse, A., Chow, B. W.-Y., and Shu, H. (2005). The
role of morphological awareness in children’s vocabulary acquisition in English.
Appl. Psycholinguist. 26, 415–435. doi: 10.1017/S014271640505023X
Mouzaki, A., Protopapas, A., Sideridis, G., and Simos, P. (2010). “Mια δoκιµασíα
για την αξιoλóγησητης oρθoγραϕíας [A test for the assessment of spelling],
in Spelling: Learning and Disorders, eds A. Mouzaki and A. Protopapas (Athens:
Gutenberg), 327–347.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 11 November 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 2039
fpsyg-08-02039 November 23, 2017 Time: 15:59 # 12
Diamanti et al. Preschool Predictors of Early Literacy
Muroya, N., Inoue, T., Hosokawa, M., Georgiou, G. K., Maekawa, H., and Parrila, R.
(2017). The role of morphological awareness in word reading skills in Japanese:
a within-language cross-orthographic perspective. Sci. Stud. Read. 21, 449–462.
doi: 10.1080/10888438.2017.1323906
Muse, A. E. (2005). The Nature of Morphological Knowledge. Doctoral dissertation,
Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL.
Nagy, W. E., Carlisle, J. F., and Goodwin, A. P. (2014). Morphological
knowledge and literacy acquisition. J. Learn. Disabil. 47, 3–12. doi: 10.1177/
0022219413509967
National Reading Panel (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching
Children to Read: Reports of the Subgroups (00-4754). Washington, DC: U.S.
Department of Health and Human Services.
Nunes, T., Bryant, P. E., and Bindman, M. (1997). Learning to spell regular and
irregular verbs. Read. Writ. 9, 427–449. doi: 10.1023/A:1007951213624
Padeliadu, S., Antoniou, F., and Sideridis, G. (in press). 1ιερε 0
υνηση
αναγνωστικ 0
ων δεξιoτ0
ητων και αδυναµι 0
ων [Investigation of Reading
Skills and Weaknesses]. Nicosia: Rocketlexia.
Pan, J., Song, S., Su, M., McBride, C., Liu, H., Zhang, Y., et al. (2016). On the
relationship between phonological awareness, morphological awareness and
Chinese literacy skills: evidence from an 8-year longitudinal study. Dev. Sci. 19,
982–991. doi: 10.1111/desc.12356
Papadopoulos, T. C., Kendeou, P., and Spanoudis, G. (2012). Investigating the
factor structure and measurement invariance of phonological abilities in a
sufficiently transparent language. J. Educ. Psychol. 104, 321–336. doi: 10.1037/
a0026446
Papadopoulos, T. C., Spanoudis, G., and Kendeou, P. (2009). The dimensionality
of phonological abilities in Greek. Read. Res. Q. 44, 127–143. doi: 10.1598/RRQ.
44.2.2
Pittas, E., and Nunes, T. (2014). The relation between morphological awareness and
reading and spelling in Greek: a longitudinal study. Read. Writ. 27, 1507–1527.
doi: 10.1007/s11145-014- 9503-6
Porpodas, C. D. (1999). Patterns of phonological and memory processing in
beginning readers and spellers of Greek. J. Learn. Disabil. 32, 406–416. doi:
10.1177/002221949903200506
Protopapas, A. (2017). “Learning to read Greek, in Learning to Read
across Languages and Writing Systems, eds L. T. W. Verhoeven
and C. A. Perfetti (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press),
155–180.
Protopapas, A., Fakou, A., Drakopoulou, S., Skaloumbakas, C., and Mouzaki, A.
(2013a). What do spelling errors tell us? Classification and analysis of errors
made by Greek schoolchildren with and without dyslexia. Read. Writ. 26,
615–646. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00818
Protopapas, A., Mouzaki, A., Sideridis, G. D., Kotsolakou, A., and Simos,
P. G. (2013b). The role of vocabulary in the context of the simple
view of reading. Read. Writ. Q. 29, 168–202. doi: 10.1080/10573569.2013.
758569
Protopapas, A., Sideridis, G. D., Simos, P. G., and Mouzaki, A. (2007).
Development of lexical mediation in the relation between reading
comprehension and word reading skills in Greek. Sci. Stud. Read. 11,
165–197. doi: 10.1080/10888430701344322
Protopapas, A., and Vlahou, E. L. (2009). A comparative quantitative analysis
of Greek orthographic transparency. Behav. Res. Methods 41, 991–1008.
doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.991
Ralli, A. (2003). Morphology in Greek linguistics: the state of the art. J. Greek
Linguist. 4, 77–129. doi: 10.1075/jgl.4.09ral
Ralli, A. (2005). Moρϕoλoγ´ια [Morphology]. Athens: Patakis.
Reed, D. K. (2008). A synthesis of morphology interventions and effects on reading
outcomes for students in Grades K-12. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 23, 36–49.
doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5826.2007.00261.x
Rothou, K. M., and Padeliadu, S. (2015). Inflectional morphological awareness and
word reading and reading comprehension in Greek. Appl. Psycholinguist. 36,
1007–1027. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01793
Schatschneider, C., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B. R., Fletcher, J. M., and Mehta, P.
(1999). The dimensionality of phonological awareness: an application of item
response theory. J. Educ. Psychol. 91, 439–449. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.439
Selby, S. (1972). The development of morphological rules in children. Br. J. Educ.
Psychol. 42, 293–299. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8279.1972.tb00722.x
Sparks, E., and Deacon, S. H. (2015). Morphological awareness and
vocabulary acquisition: a longitudinal examination of their relationship
in English-speaking children. Appl. Psycholinguist. 36, 299–321.
doi: 10.1017/S0142716413000246
Stavrakaki, S., and Clahsen, H. (2009). The perfective past tense in Greek child
language. J. Child Lang. 36, 113–142. doi: 10.1017/S0305000908008866
Tighe, E. L., and Schatschneider, C. (2015). Exploring the dimensionality of
morphological awareness and its relations to vocabulary knowledge in adult
basic education students. Read. Res. Q. 50, 293–311. doi: 10.1002/rrq.102
Tong, X., Deacon, S. H., Kirby, J. R., Cain, K., and Parrila, R. (2011). Morphological
awareness: a key to understanding poor reading comprehension in English.
J. Educ. Psychol. 103, 523–534. doi: 10.1037/a0023495
Tunmer, W. E., Pratt, C., and Herriman, M. (1984). Metalinguistic Awareness in
Children: Theory, Research and Implications. New York, NY: Springer.
Vaknin-Nusbaum, V., Sarid, M., Raveh, M., and Nevo, E. (2016a). The contribution
of morphological awareness to reading comprehension in early stages of
reading. Read. Writ. 29, 1915–1934. doi: 10.1007/s11145-016-9658-4
Vaknin-Nusbaum, V., Sarid, M., and Shimron, J. (2016b). Morphological
awareness and reading in second and fifth grade: evidence from Hebrew. Read.
Writ. 29, 229–244. doi: 10.1007/s11145-015- 9587-7
Varlokosta, S., and Nerantzini, M. (2013). Grammatical gender in specific language
impairment: evidence from determiner-noun contexts in Greek. Psychology 20,
338–357.
Varlokosta, S., and Nerantzini, M. (2015). “The acquisition of past tense by
Greek-speaking children with specific language impairment: the role of
phonological saliency, regularity, and frequency, in Advances in Research on
Specific Language Impairment, ed. S. Stavrakaki (Amsterdam: John Benjamins),
253–286.
Wei, T. Q., Bi, H. Y., Chen, B. G., Liu, Y., Weng, X. C., and Wydell, T. N.
(2014). Developmental changes in the role of different metalinguistic awareness
skills in Chinese reading acquisition from preschool to third grade. PLOS ONE
9:e96240. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0096240
Conflict of Interest Statement: The preschool measures reported here form part
of a commercially available screening battery (Logometro, produced by InteLearn
Multimedia Educational Applications) designed by the authors, who receive part
of the proceeds from its use.
Copyright © 2017 Diamanti, Mouzaki, Ralli, Antoniou, Papaioannou and
Protopapas. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in
other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited
and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted
academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not
comply with these terms.
Frontiers in Psychology | www.frontiersin.org 12 November 2017 | Volume 8 | Article 2039
... The strongest and most consistently reported phonological predictor of literacy skills is phoneme awareness (i.e., the ability to identify and manipulate individual sounds) (phonemes) in spoken words. Numerous empirical studies underscore the robust relationship between phoneme awareness and literacy skills (Cataldo and Ellis, 1988;Landerl and Wimmer, 2008;Diamanti et al., 2017;Schmitterer and Schroeder, 2018), especially in the early phases of development (Babayigit and Stainthorp, 2010;Nielsen and Juul, 2016). The significance of phoneme awareness as a predictor of literacy skills transcends linguistic and writing system boundaries (Furnes and Samuelsson, 2009). ...
... In line with previous research (Cataldo Landerl and Wimmer, 2008;Diamanti et al., 2017;Schmitterer and Schroeder, 2018), the results revealed that kindergarten proficiency in phonological processing skills significantly predicted performance in spelling and reading during Grade 1, even after adding the respective autoregressor. A novel finding of the current study was that when identifying predictors that are specifically related to either reading or spelling by controlling for the respective other literacy skill (step 4), phonological processing turned out to be specifically related to spelling and to the variance shared between reading and spelling, but not to the variance related to reading only. ...
Article
Full-text available
Theoretical background While reading and spelling skills often are interconnected in models of literacy development, recent research suggests that the two skills can dissociate and that reading and spelling are associated with at least partly different cognitive predictors. However, previous research on dissociations between reading and spelling skills focused on children who have already mastered the first phases of literacy development. These findings suggest that dissociations are due to distinct deficits in orthographic processing (i.e., unprecise orthographic representations vs. inefficient serial processing). It is therefore unclear whether dissociations already become apparent during the initial stages, or rather emerge later in development. This study aims to enhance the understanding of the predictors of early spelling and reading skills, investigating potential variations, by considering various cognitive factors beyond well-established ones. Methods Data were collected at two time points: cognitive predictors and early reading and spelling skills were assessed at the end of kindergarten (T1) before formal literacy instruction started, and reading and spelling skills were again assessed in Grade 1 (T2). The data analysis included 353 first-grade participants. Linear regression analyses assessed predictive patterns, while logistic regression analyses explained children's likelihood of belonging to different proficiency groups (at-risk or typical skills). Results Results revealed phonological processing, letter knowledge, and intelligence, as significant predictors for Spelling in grade 1 (T2), even after adding the autoregressor (Spelling in kindergarten at T1) and the respective other literacy skill (Reading T2). For Reading in grade 1 (T2), phonological processing, and rapid automatized naming (RAN) surfaced as significant predictors after adding the autoregressor (Reading T1). However, only RAN surfaced as a significant predictor for Reading T2 after adding the respective other literacy skill (Spelling T2). In line with these findings, logistic regression analyses revealed that phonological processing predicted group allocation for Spelling T2 and RAN predicted group allocation for Reading T2. Conclusions Overall, the study underscores the importance of phonological processing and letter knowledge as early predictors of spelling and reading skills in Grade 1. Moreover, intelligence is identified as a predictor for early spelling, while rapid automatized naming (RAN) emerges as a predictor for early reading.
... En este sentido, tener un adecuado nivel de comprensión permite mejores posibilidades sociales, educativas y laborales (Chávez Delgado et al., 2022;Eadie et al., 2018;Law et al., 2009). Uno de los factores decisores para que el sujeto logre óptimos niveles de lectura es la adquisición propicia de habilidades de lectura temprana (Apel et al., 2019;Clemens et al., 2017;Deacon et al., 2019;Diamanti et al., 2017;Arafat et al., 2017;McIlraith & Language and Reading Research Consortium, 2018;Tibi & Kirby, 2018;Pfost, 2015). En esta línea, se ha constatado que el rendimiento en la adquisición de dichas habilidades varía entre los estudiantes dependiendo de factores como el género (Mullis et al., 2020;O'Dea et al., 2018). ...
... En el primer grupo se encuentran aquellas vinculadas con la conversión grafema-fonema, como por ejemplo la conciencia fonológica (CF) y, en una segunda agrupación, las habilidades relacionadas con el significado, dentro de las cuales está la conciencia de lo impreso (CI; Justice et al., 2007). Diversas investigaciones han establecido que el nivel de desarrollo de dichas habilidades predice el rendimiento lector posterior (Apel et al., 2019;Clemens et al., 2017;Deacon et al., 2019;Diamanti et al., 2017;Arafat et al., 2017;McIlraith & Language and Reading Research Consortium, 2018;Tibi & Kirby, 2018). La evidencia indica que un deficiente aprendizaje de estas habilidades es un factor que intensifica las brechas en los resultados escolares (Gutiérrez et al., 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
Un número importante de investigaciones han constatado la existencia de diferencias en el rendimiento lector entre niños y niñas. Sin embargo, los hallazgos no son convergentes; por otra parte, la mayoría de los estudios se han resuelto en cursos posteriores al preescolar y en sistemas ortográficos opacos. En menor medida se ha indagado el rendimiento inicial lector entre niños y niñas preescolares que asisten a escuelas vulnerables, en período de pospandemia. En este sentido, el objetivo de esta investigación fue determinar si las diferencias de género en el rendimiento lector ocurren desde edades tempranas, a pesar de que los estudiantes no hayan asistido presencialmente a las escuelas. La muestra consistió en 58 alumnos provenientes de 4 colegios públicos. A través de un ANOVA se evaluó la existencia de diferencias de género en el rendimiento lector inicial de los estudiantes. Los resultados mostraron que no existen diferencias en el rendimiento lector global entre niños y niñas, pero sí las hay entre tareas, ya que las niñas se desempeñaron mejor que los niños en tareas de conciencia de lo impreso. Por otra parte, se demostró que tanto niños como niñas obtienen mejores resultados en tareas de conciencia fonológica frente a las tareas de conciencia de lo impreso.
... Since the symptoms of dyslexia and the problems experienced in reading skills differ from individual to individual, it is necessary to create individualized learning environments in order to provide individualized instruction to improve reading skills and to intervene by determining the most effective activities and strategies in the shortest time (Doganay-Bilgi, 2017;Frith, 2017;Shaywitz, 2003). When the literature is examined, it is also stated that the skills of individuals with dyslexia such as (Gibbs, 2005;Layes et al., 2022), print awareness (Georgiou et al., 2016;Rothe et al., 2014), morphological awareness (Diamanti et al., 2017;Mather & Wendling, 2011), rapid naming (Albuquerque, 2017;Norton & Wolf, 2012), working memory (Azizi et al., 2020;Swanson & Howell, 2001), reading fluency (Gedik & Akyol, 2022;Satilmis, 2021) and reading comprehension (Erbasan & Saglam, 2020;Stetter & Hughes, 2017) should be frequently supported with designed activities and developed strategies. ...
... Regarding phonological awareness, Diamanti et al. (2017) evaluated 104 preschoolers' early phonological awareness skills and researched its predictive power on reading and spelling outcomes at the end of Grade 1. Those scholars found that phonological awareness played a vital role in assessing the preschoolers' fluency in reading text as well as their comprehension of the text. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Early reading has gained significant attention in the academic community. With the increasing volume of literature on this subject, it has become crucial to assess the current research landscape and identify emerging trends. Methods This study utilized the dynamic topic model to analyze a corpus of 1,638 articles obtained from the Web of Science Core Collection to furnish a lucid understanding of the prevailing research and forecast possible future directions. Results Our in-depth assessment discerned 11 cardinal topics, among which notable ones were interventions' impacts on early reading competencies; foundational elements of early reading: phonological awareness, letters, and, spelling; and early literacy proficiencies in children with autism spectrum disorder. Although most topics have received consistent research attention, there has been a marked increase in some topics' popularity, such as foundational elements of early reading and early literary proficiencies in children with autism spectrum disorder. Conversely, other topics exhibited a downturn. Discussion This analytical endeavor has yielded indispensable insights for scholars, decision-makers, and field practitioners, steering them toward pivotal research interrogatives, focal interest zones, and prospective research avenues. As per our extensive survey, this paper is a pioneering holistic purview of the seminal areas of early reading that highlights expected scholarly directions.
... This finding largely corroborates previous research indicating the important role of morphological awareness in reading fluency and accuracy in various languages, including inflectional morphology (Deacon et al., 2013;Kirby et al., 2012;Mahony et al., 2000), and in the Arabic language in particular (Asadi et al., 2017;Mahfoudhi et al., 2010;Ravid & Schiff, 2009;Saiegh-Haddad & Henkin-Roitfarb, 2014;Shalhoub-Awwad & Leikin, 2016;Vaknin-Nusbaum, 2018;Vaknin-Nusbaum & Saiegh-Haddad, 2020). Similarly, morphological awareness has been found to be a key skill for mastering spelling across orthographies (Bowers et al., 2010;Diamanti et al., 2017;Goodwin & Ahn, 2010). Related to the correlation between Arabic morphology and spelling, Taha and Saiegh-Haddad (2017) revealed the implications of derivational morphology processing in the spelling of Arabic words. ...
Article
Full-text available
Purpose: This study examined the differences in spoken Arabic (SpA) and standard Arabic (StA) in inflectional (gender, number, possessive pronouns, and tense) construction use in Arabic among preschoolers. Moreover, we tested the contribution of the inflectional constructions possessed in kindergarten to reading skills in the first grade and examined whether this morphological contribution differs between SpA and StA. Method: We assessed 261 Arabic-speaking kindergartners for 1 year until the end of first grade for inflectional knowledge in kindergarten and reading skills in first grade (reading accuracy and fluency, spelling, and reading comprehension). Results: The findings revealed that among inflections, prevalence of performance on gender constructions was the highest, followed by number and possessive pronouns, and lowest performance for tense constructions. Although the performance for SpA was higher than for StA in all constructions, similar patterns were observed except similarity between gender and number in StA. Moreover, the results indicate a significant contribution of almost all inflectional constructions (except possessive pronouns) possessed in kindergarten to all reading skills in the first grade. However, tense did not contribute to reading comprehension, and possessive pronouns did not contribute to any of the reading measures. Regarding diglossia, although the claims that linguistic components in StA are not represented in the mental lexicon, StA accounted for an additional significant 2%–3% of the explained variance in Step 2 (which checked the practical significance of statistically significant results) in all reading measures. Conclusions: This study highlights the impact of diglossia-specific morphological differences (prevalence of the use of the morphological construction in Arabic in SpA vs. StA) on reading and literacy measures, especially the contribution of morphological awareness in SpA, which may provide a stronger basis for StA reading skills. The implications of these results are discussed, especially regarding exposing children to the morphological representations of both the SpA and StA forms to promote reading and literacy in Arabic.
... It is possible that learning and attaining high-level skill in reading by children does not engage PA, at either the phoneme or syllable level, as it does in learning of alphabetic writing systems. In the case of alphabetic literacy the evidence is now strong that it does (Diamanti, et al., 2017;Ehri, 2005;Kjeldsen et al., 2014;Saiegh-Haddad & Taha, 2017;Schaars et al., 2017). Take note that we have put aside for now any more discussion of awareness at the syntactic level or metacognition at the discourse/text level. ...
Article
Full-text available
In the research on literacy learning the concept of language awareness has come forward as a unifying framework for understanding the underlying knowledge that supports ability in reading and writing. Consensus is gathering around the idea that language awareness is an essential foundation. If subsequent work in this area confirms it, this factor may turn out to be the key cognitive-domain explanation for successful literacy learning in school (and for academic purposes in general). In this review we examine two cross-cultural comparisons regarding this claim. The comparisons point to the need to examine cases that juxtapose contrasting conditions. Relevant contrasts place side by side examples that appear to be typical and examples that appear to be exceptional. Taking what appear on the surface as sharply diverging cases, how is access to requisite underlying competencies similar, and how different, from one instance to the other?
... We measured MA with three measures, assessing inflectional, derivational, and compounding morphology at a metalinguistic level. According to Diamanti et al. (2017), metalinguistic awareness refers to the individual's ability to reflect upon and consciously manipulate morphemes, as well as the ability to apply word formation rules. All tasks were administered orally. ...
Article
Full-text available
Although relations between morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and vocabulary have been widely observed, questions remain about their precise associations. The purpose of the present study was to explore the relations of morphological awareness with two highly related linguistic skills (phonological awareness and vocabulary) in a transparent orthography with rich morphology. The study sample consisted of 121 (58 males, Mean age = 93.94 months, SD = 3.32) 2nd grade Greek-speaking children. Confirmatory factor analysis revealed that the three-factor model provided the best fit to data, indicating that although morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and vocabulary are highly correlated constructs, they represent distinct linguistic skills. In addition, hierarchical regression analysis was used to examine the potential bidirectional effects among the three linguistic skills. The results indicated that both phonological awareness and vocabulary significantly contributed to morphological awareness, with phonological awareness having a stronger effect on it. Conversely, morphological awareness significantly affected both phonological awareness and vocabulary. The effect size from phonological awareness and vocabulary to morphological awareness was similar to the effect size reported from morphological awareness to phonological awareness and vocabulary. These results imply that morphological awareness is highly associated with phonological awareness and vocabulary, being though a distinct skill and these linguistic skills have bidirectional effects with each other in first grades.
Article
Full-text available
Reading literacy has attracted the attention of a growing number of researchers in recent times. Though it is seen as an essential competency, thorough bibliometric analyses on the subject are lacking within a few African contexts, one of which is reading for comprehension in the isiZulu home language. This study attempted to fill this gap, by selecting 95 articles through the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis Protocol (PRISMA-P) method and leveraging tools such as VOSviewer, CitNetExplorer’s visualization citation network, and counting techniques. The results indicated that South Africa is a central player, experiencing a notable increase in publications over the past decade. Authors such as Probert, Paulsrud B., Tian Z., Toth J., Port R.F., McKinney C., Tyler R. Moodley V., and Wildsmith-Cromarty R. stand out, alongside prolific journals like English-Medium Instruction and Translanguaging, Language Learning, and Language Teaching. Crucial topics identified include Cultural Context, Language Proficiency, Interactive Read-Alouds, and Differentiated Instruction. The results emphasized the pivotal role of reading motivation in formal education, highlighting the complex nature of online activities. The study concluded that the field of research on teaching reading for comprehension in isiZulu has evolved into a well-established and cross-disciplinary field, with South Africa leading in contributions and collaborations, and anticipates continued growth which will solidify isiZulu reading instruction as a promising and enduring subject of study. Keywords: isiZulu, Home Language, Reading Comprehension, CitNetExplorer, PRISMA-P, Bibliometric Review, Community Involvement
Article
Full-text available
It is well established that children’s reading comprehension is driven, at least in part, by their awareness of morphemes, or the smallest units of meaning in language. The question of how it does so is largely open; this mechanistic knowledge would specify theories of reading comprehension and guide effective classroom instruction. We report here on a longitudinal study designed to test two candidate mechanisms by which morphological awareness might support the development of reading comprehension: use of morphemes to read words and to understand words, known as morphological decoding and analysis, respectively. We tracked the development of 221 children on key measures from Grades 3 to 5. These three-wave longitudinal data enabled us to test morphological decoding and analysis as mediators connecting morphological awareness to gains in reading comprehension over time. Structural equation modeling showed that morphological awareness contributed to gains in both morphological decoding and analysis. Critically, only morphological analysis mediated the contribution of morphological awareness to gains in reading comprehension between Grades 3 and 5. These findings elaborate predictions of the morphological pathways framework. Specifically, longitudinal modeling shows how morphological awareness supports children’s developing reading comprehension: by enabling the use of morphemes to read and understand words, with effects on the understanding of words supporting children’s growing skill in understanding texts. Evidence supporting these two specific mechanisms inspires the design of targeted teaching on morphemes toward the development of strong reading comprehension.
Article
Full-text available
This study aims to explore the relations of phonological awareness and rapid naming with efficient word reading. Our work builds on the strong evidence base of associations between phonological awareness, rapid naming, orthographic knowledge, and efficient word reading. Specifically, we tested a pathway linking phonological awareness to orthographic knowledge and on to efficient word reading and a pathway linking rapid naming to orthographic knowledge and on to efficient word reading, following on the self-teaching (Share in Cognition 55(2):151-218, 1995. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1016/ 0010-0277(94) 00645-2) and rapid naming hypotheses (Wolf & Bow-ers in J Educ Psychol 91(3):415-438, 1999. https:// doi. org/ 10. 1037/ 0022-0663. 91.3. 415), respectively. Based on these two classic theories of orthographic knowledge development, we conducted a multiple-path analysis to test the mediating role of orthographic knowledge in the relation of phonological awareness and rapid naming with efficient word reading. The sample consisted of 231 monolingual Greek-speaking children; 121 (58 males) Grade 2 children (M age = 7.82 years, SD = 3.32) and 110 (51 males) Grade 5 children (M age = 10.84, years, SD = 3.54) participated in the study. After establishing the significant contribution of orthographic knowledge to efficient word reading, we found two direct pathways: one from phonological awareness in Grade 5 and one from rapid naming in Grades 2 and 5 and two indirect pathways in which phonological awareness and rapid naming contribute to efficient word reading via orthographic knowledge in both Grades. Then, we assessed whether there is a shift over one of the two theories over time, using a multi-group analysis with data from Grades 2 and 5. Indeed, we found differences between two Grades in pathways from sub-lexical orthographic knowledge to efficient word reading, from phonological awareness and rapid naming to efficient word reading, and from rapid naming to sub-lexical orthographic knowledge. Our findings revealed that both pathways, representing the main self-teaching and rapid naming hypotheses, are active and work in both Grades, reflecting the parallel support of the two classic theories of orthographic knowledge development.
Article
Full-text available
Τhe present study investigates whether Greek-speaking children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI) face difficulties in the acquisition of gender in determiner-noun contexts, as expressed via agreement οn the determiner. The results of an elicitation task with real and novel nouns showed that children with SLI (a) show difficulties primarily with masculine and feminine gender marking, and do not use prototypicality of the noun suffix, as typically developing children do, to mark the gender on the determiner in conditions with real nouns, and (b) do not use, with the same consistency as typically developing children do, the noun ending as a cue to mark the gender value on the determiner in conditions with novel nouns. It is argued that although grammatical gender is considered an intrinsic lexical property of the noun, it is not learned by children with SLI along with other lexical features of the noun. Moreover, when lexical information is not provided in the nouns, children with SLI cannot process morphology cues, such as the inflectional suffixes on the nouns, as consistently as typically developing children do.
Chapter
Full-text available
Around the world, children embark on learning to read in their home language or writing system. But does their specific language, and how it is written, make a difference to how they learn? How is learning to read English similar to or different from learning in other languages? Is reading alphabetic writing a different challenge from reading syllabic or logographic writing? Learning to Read across Languages and Writing Systems examines these questions across seventeen languages representing the world's different major writing systems. Each chapter highlights the key features of a specific language, exploring research on learning to read, spell, and comprehend it, and on implications for education. The editors' introduction describes the global spread of reading and provides a theoretical framework, including operating principles for learning to read. The editors' final chapter draws conclusions about cross-linguistic universal trends, and the challenges posed by specific languages and writing systems.
Article
Full-text available
This cross-sectional study examined the development of morphological awareness in Greek children 4–7 years old. A distinction was adopted between epilinguistic control, evidenced in judgment tasks and indicative of elementary levels of awareness, and metalinguistic awareness, evidenced in production tasks and indicative of full-blown conscious awareness. The morphological domains of inflectional and derivational morphology were specifically contrasted to determine whether they follow distinct developmental trajectories. Trial-level performance data from 236 children in four morphological awareness tasks as a function of age were modeled using generalized additive models. Significant performance increase with age was found for all four awareness tasks. The results further indicated that production of derivational morphemes was consistently more difficult than production of inflectional morphemes and judgment of derivational morphemes, whereas the differences between the two inflectional and between the two judgment tasks were not significant. This suggests that at these ages, epilinguistic control is similarly effective for the two morphological domains whereas full metalinguistic awareness of derivational morphology trails behind that of inflectional morphology, at least as measured by these specific tasks. The findings highlight the need for early tracking and finer distinctions within the domain of morphological awareness, to identify and potentially enhance the critical skills related to the development of vocabulary and reading comprehension.
Article
Full-text available
In the present study, we examined the role of morphological awareness in reading and spelling performance across three languages varying in orthographic consistency (English, French, and Greek), after controlling for the effects of phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming (RAN). One hundred fifty-nine English-speaking Canadian, 238 French-speaking Canadian, and 224 Greek children were assessed at the beginning of Grade 2 on measures of morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and RAN. At the end of Grade 2, they were assessed on reading accuracy, reading fluency, reading comprehension, and spelling to dictation. The results indicated that morphological awareness was a unique predictor of reading comprehension and spelling in all three languages, of reading fluency in English and French, and of reading accuracy in English only. Furthermore, the results of multigroup analyses revealed no significant differences in the contribution of morphological awareness to the literacy outcomes across languages. Theoretical and practical implications of these finding are discussed. Full publication in (SN) SharedIt here: http://rdcu.be/vrzS
Article
Full-text available
We examined the relationship between morphological awareness and word reading skills in syllabic Hiragana and morphographic Kanji. Participants were 127 Grade 1 Japanese-speaking children who were followed until Grade 2. The results showed that Grade 1 morphological awareness was uniquely and comparably associated with word reading skills in both Hiragana and Kanji in Grade 1 after controlling for nonverbal and verbal cognitive abilities, phonological awareness, and rapid automatized naming. The effect of Grade 1 morphological awareness on Grade 2 Kanji word recognition was slightly stronger (∆R² = .10) than the effect on Grade 2 Hiragana reading fluency (∆R² = .03). The findings suggest that morphological awareness plays an important role in early word reading skills across the two scripts, and with reading skill development it may become more important for mastering morphographic Kanji characters.
Article
Full-text available
Recent research studies in several alphabetic orthographic systems have shown a significant contribution of morphological awareness in the development of spelling ability. It is assumed that awareness of morphemes facilitates the application of morphophonemic principles on spelling. However, apart from its effect on understanding the conventions of the general spelling system of a language, morphological awareness seems to facilitate the orthographic performance of specific morphemes as well, especially inflectional suffixes, through their morphemic differentiation. The aim of this longitudinal study was to examine the contribution of morphological awareness in Kindergarten and Grade 1, on children’s spelling ability of inflectional suffixes in both Grades 1 and 2. Two hundred and fifteen Greek – speaking children from Kindergarten up to Grade 1 were assessed on measures of: (a) morphological awareness (e.g., word analogy, decomposition of derivative words, reversing compounds), (b) general cognitive skills (nonverbal intelligence, verbal intelligence, short-term memory, vocabulary), and (c) early literacy skills (phonological awareness, rapid automatized naming, letter knowledge). Also, in both Grades 1 and 2 children were assessed on measures of spelling ability of inflectional suffixes in words and pseudowords. The results of the hierarchical regression analyses showed that the morphological awareness of children in both Kindergarten and Grade 1 predicted significantly their spelling of inflectional suffixes only in words, in Grades 1 and 2 respectively, beyond the effects of cognitive and language skills. Morphological awareness skills did not contribute significantly to children’s spelling of inflectional suffixes in pseudowords. Overall, these findings highlight that early morphological awareness skills contribute significantly to the development of spelling ability even at the early primary school years. Therefore, it is suggested that the teaching of spelling inflectional suffixes has to emphasize the semantic and syntactic role of inflectional suffixes through activities of writing rather than memorizing rules for the correct spelling of each inflectional suffix.
Article
Full-text available
The contribution of morphological awareness to reading comprehension in Hebrew was studied in 298 second grade students who practiced two types of inflections, plural and possessive. Reading tasks at the beginning and end of the school year indicated that all improved on all tests in that period. Orthographic word recognition and morphological awareness predicted reading comprehension at the end of year. Students with low (LPD) and high (HPD) phonological decoding skills clearly differed qualitatively in reading comprehension. In the HPD students it was predicted by awareness of possessive inflections; in the LPD students it was predicted by orthographic word recognition ability. The results highlight the importance of examining the different components of morphological awareness in readers with different levels of phonological decoding ability.
Chapter
Full-text available
The performance of 13 Greek-speaking children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI) on past tense formation is compared to the performance of two control groups of typically developing (TD) children. It is shown that children with SLI have difficulties in the production of past tense forms compared to language or age-matched TD controls. It is argued that phonological salience or regularity does not affect the acquisition of past tense in children with SLI and TD controls. In contrast, regularity of aspectual formation and frequency of the verb form has an effect on the acquisition of the perfective past in children with SLI. Τhese results are discussed in relation to one morphosyntactic and one morphophonological account of past tense acquisition in SLI.
Book
In the past fi fteen years there has been a growi ng interest in the development of children's awareness of language as an object in itself -- a phenomenon now generally referred to as metal inguistic awareness. Until the publication of an earlier volume in the Springer Series in Language and Communication, The Chitd's Conoeption oi Language, edited by A. Sinclair, R. J. Jarvella, and W. J. M. Levelt, there had been no systematic treatment of metalinguistic awareness. The major goal of that volume was to map out the field of study by describing the phenomenon of interest and defining major theoretical issues. The aim of the present volume is to present an overview of metalinguistic awareness in children which reflects the current state of research and theory. The volume is divided into three major sections. The first considers various conceptual and methodological issues that have arisen from efforts to study metalinguistic awareness. It addresses such questions as what is metalinguistic awareness, when does it begin to emerge, and what tasks and procedures can be employed to assess its development in young children. The second sect ion cri ti ca 11y revi ews the research that has been conducted i nto the four general types of metalinguistic awareness -- phonologieal, word, syntactic, and pragmatic awareness. In the final section the development of metalinguistic awareness is examined in relation to general cognitive development, reading acquisition, bilingualism, and early childhood education.
Chapter
The present study examined whether morphological awareness instruction in Kindergarten classrooms contributes to the improvement of young children’s early literacy skills (e.g., morphological and phonological awareness, print knowledge, vocabulary). Two quasi-experimental studies were implemented. Study 1 consisted of a treatment and a control group of young children and Study 2 consisted of two treatment groups and a control group. In both studies, the treatment groups received a 5 week intervention scheme with several morphological awareness activities. In Study 2, a second treatment group received an intervention scheme with activities which combined morphological and phonological awareness. Both studies’ findings showed that the treatment groups which received morphological awareness instruction or a blended instruction on morphological and phonological awareness improved the morphological awareness abilities more than the control group. There were small or no transfer effects on the improvement of print knowledge and vocabulary. However, Study 2 showed that phonological awareness abilities improved only in the treatment group which received the blended instruction. According to the present findings, the teaching of morphemes in Kindergarten is beneficial for morphological awareness improvement, but it has to be combined with other early literacy activities in order to have broader effects on young children’s literacy development.