ArticlePDF AvailableLiterature Review
PSYCHIATRY
OPINION ARTICLE
published: 07 April 2015
doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00048
Mental imagery and food consumption
Benjamin Missbach1*, Arnd Florack 2and Jürgen König1
1Department of Nutritional Sciences, University of Vienna,Vienna, Austria
2Department of Psychology, University of Vienna,Vienna, Austria
*Correspondence: benjamin.missbach@univie.ac.at
Edited by:
David G. Pearson, University of Aberdeen, UK
Reviewed by:
Eva Kemps, Flinders University, Australia
Keywords: mental imagery, habituation, satiation, depletion, mindfulness
INTRODUCTION
One enigmatic capacity of human expe-
rience is the ability to travel back and
forth in time by using mental simula-
tions. By imagining shapes, forms, and
scenes, humans can relive the past and
visualize future events (1,2). Histori-
cally, this memory-based mechanism has
been discussed in scientific and non-
scientific fields. As described by Marcel
Proust in his epic work, In Search of
Lost Time (3), the sensory experience of
gustatory cues, in this case, a sponge-
cake called Madeleine, seems to be pow-
erful enough to trigger a cascade of vivid
intrusions associated with a particular
set of memories (4). Besides anecdotal
references, visual mental images are of
great interest in the domain of eating
behavior because research has shown that
(involuntary) mental simulations of intru-
sions and (voluntary) repetitive mental
imagery influence eating behavior. Unrav-
eling the basic mechanisms that under-
lie mental imagery in the food domain
has the potential to provide new insights
into the perception and consumption
of food.
In this opinion article, we briefly
report on the role of mental imagery
simulations in eating behavior and its
associated pathologies and illustrate how
research on mental imaginary has con-
tributed to the current understanding of
the cognitive aspects of food intake reg-
ulation and satiation processes. Further-
more, we discuss whether guided mental
imagery intervention strategies can be inte-
grated into the successful self-regulation
of eating behavior and provide a per-
spective for future research on mental
imagery.
MENTAL SIMULATION OF FOOD
CONSUMPTION AND EFFECTS ON
JUDGMENTS AND BEHAVIOR
Many individuals in modern society live
in environments that promote excessive
weight gain because of the omnipresence
of food (5). The exposure to food automat-
ically prepares individuals for food intake
and often evokes thoughts about food, a
simulation of food intake, and a strong
motivation to consume food (6). Indeed,
even the exposure to words related to
tempting food can activate a cascade of
associations with food (7) and the sim-
ulation or reenactment of prior eating
occasions (8). Although internal cues of
hunger or craving are relevant to the for-
mation of such intrusive thoughts to a
large degree, it is clear that thoughts about
food consumption are also triggered by
consumption-related cues in the environ-
ment. Shop design and product presenta-
tion in marketing have tapped into these
mechanisms by using sensory, textural,
and emotional triggers to evoke intrusions,
facilitate the simulation of consumption,
and increase actual purchases and con-
sumption (9,10). Recently, research has
shown that such marketing practices are
not just a creative idea put forth by mar-
keting managers but are supported by con-
crete evidence for the link between men-
tal simulations and behavior. Elder and
Krishna (11) presented participants with
a picture of smooth vanilla yogurt in a
bowl. They varied whether a spoon was
on the right or left side of the bowl. In
support of the hypothesis that participants
simulate their eating of the yogurt prior to
their actual intake in order to forecast the
taste of the yogurt, the researchers found
that right-handed participants indicated
higher purchase intentions when the spoon
was on the right side of the bowl com-
pared with when it was on the left side.
Hence, the characteristics of the presenta-
tion that facilitated the simulation of eat-
ing increased the motivation to eat the
yogurt. The facilitation effect is not lim-
ited to visual perception. For instance,
Mitchell and Kahn (12) reported that the
ambient odor of a room had a con-
siderable effect on the extent to which
individuals thought about the presented
products. When the odor was congruent
with a product (e.g., chocolate), consumers
were more likely to process information
about the product. Similarly,Seo and Roidl
(13) found that congruent odor enhanced
visual attention to odor-congruent food
items. In their study, 60 participants
were presented with four odors (orange,
lavender, coffee, and licorice) prior to
and during the presentation of foods
via photographic slides. Participants who
received olfactory cues looked more fre-
quently and for longer at the correspond-
ing foods than participants in a control
condition with no odor presentation. The
presentation of food also has an effect
on taste assessments. For example, sub-
tle changes of the natural color of an
orange juice drink (darker orange hue)
decreases liking scores substantially (14). In
accordance, manipulating expected brand
pronunciation (incongruent brand label-
ing) was shown to reduce hedonic lik-
ing of yoghurts (15). Taken as a whole,
the results of the above-mentioned stud-
ies correspond with the assumption that
conditions that facilitate the simulation
of the consumption of attractive food
increase interest in the food (e.g., visual
attention or processing of information
www.frontiersin.org April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 48 | 1
Missbach et al. Mental imagery and food consumption behavior
about the food) and the desire for food
consumption.
Researchers have recently referred to
theories on grounded cognition (16,17)
to explain the effects of perceptions on
thoughts and behavior (10). The basic idea
of these theories is that the cognitive repre-
sentation of concepts is grounded in related
modal systems. Previous theories on cog-
nition had supposed that cognitions were
amodal and, for example, not based on
modal representations of motor behavior.
These theories had implied that individ-
uals can think about eating in the same
way whether or not they can move their
mouth and tongue at the current moment.
By contrast, the more recent approaches of
grounded cognition suppose that thinking
about an object is related to perception and
motor behavior. This means that humans
cannot think about food without simulta-
neously activating related perceptions and
that they simultaneously simulate motor
behavior that is related to the stimulus.
Hence, the mode of thinking is supposed
to be strongly related to the mode of per-
ception and the mode of action. Accord-
ing to the grounded cognition approach,
thinking about food or just reading food-
related words should evoke mental simula-
tions of eating (7). Recently, such assump-
tions have received support from neuro-
science research. Researchers found that
solely the perception of pictures of palat-
able food led to activations in brain areas
associated with gustatory experiences (18)
and elevated ghrelin levels (orexigenic hor-
mone) in healthy volunteers (19). Likefoo d
cues, ghrelin is one major mediator of food
anticipation (20) and recruits the same
neuronal circuitry in the dorsomedial and
ventromedial hypothalamus (21).
An example of a particularly strong
influence of thoughts on behavior is food
craving. Food craving is regarded as a
strong motivational state that urges indi-
viduals to seek and consume a particular
kind of food (22), often containing high
amounts of sugar or fat (23). When indi-
viduals crave food, their thoughts about
food are often so intrusive that they have
trouble pursuing other goals or focusing on
different thoughts. For craving, the causal
direction of the influence is bidirectional
in the sense that craving has an influ-
ence on thought and vice versa. However,
it is important to take into account that
the mental simulation of food consump-
tion triggered by external cues as described
above can result in craving.
REPEATED MENTAL SIMULATION AND
MINDFULNESS
EFFECTS OF REPEATED MENTAL SIMULATION
ON FOOD CONSUMPTION
An interesting aspect of mental simula-
tions during food perception is that men-
tal simulations do not necessarily lead
to increased consumption, but rather,
under certain circumstances, they can lead
to decreased consumption. Interestingly,
repeatedly thinking about food consump-
tion can lead to habituation and a reduced
motivation to consume a specific food,
just like real consumption (24). Habitu-
ation to food is understood as a process
that leads to a decrease in both the phys-
iological and behavioral responses to an
eating episode and a drop in enjoyment
with repeated consumption (24). Several
studies (2527) have found evidence that
habituation can take place when individu-
als repetitively judge food or imagine eating
food. For instance, Morewedge et al. (26)
asked participants to think either 3 or 30
times about eating M&M’s. Later, partici-
pants were allowed to eat M&M’s. Partici-
pants who thought about eating the candy
30 times ate less of the product than those
who thought about it 3 times and those in
a control condition (who imagined throw-
ing coins into a laundry machine). Like
other habituation effects, this effect was
shown to be sensory-specific and was only
present when the imagined food was con-
gruent with the consumed food. The effect
did not show when the imagined food (e.g.,
M&M’s) was not the same as the consumed
food (e.g., cheese cubes) [(26); Experi-
ment 4]. Similar sensory-specific charac-
teristics between imagined and consumed
foods are therefore fundamental for food
habituation to occur. In our recent study,
we found that habituation after imagined
eating needs mental resources as habitua-
tion during actual eating does (28). Sev-
eral studies have indicated that habituation
is based on a memory process that needs
cognitive capacity to occur. Hence, when
individuals are distracted during eating, for
example, when they watch TV during eat-
ing (29,30), they habituate less to food
and eat more. We found that a depleting
task had a similar effect on habituation
after imagined food consumption. In our
study, we used a mathematical counting
task (31) to deplete participants prior to
simulating the eating of 18 walnuts in one
condition. We showed that participants in
this depletion condition did not habituate
to the mental simulation, and habituation
was blocked, whereas they habituated in a
condition without depletion.
EFFECTS OF MINDFULNESS ON FOOD CHOICE
As reported above, repeatedly imagining
food consumption can reduce the desire
to consume the food (26). But repeti-
tive thoughts about food consumption are
not the only way to reduce the desire to
consume a particular kind of food. Also
important is how individuals think about
the food. While a single vivid image of
the consumption increases the influence
of impulsive responses on food choice,
the influence of impulsive responses on
food choice decreases when consumers
think about the reasons for their food
choice (32). Similarly, mindful attention
to thoughts about food consumption can
reduce choices for unhealthy food items
that are impulsively preferred by con-
sumers. For example, Papies and colleagues
(6) instructed participants to regard their
thoughts from a metacognitive perspec-
tive as temporary constructions that appear
and disappear. When this mindful atten-
tion was applied to food consumption, it
reduced impulsive tendencies to approach
food (6) and also promoted choices of
healthy food (33).
REPEATED MENTAL SIMULATION AND
MINDFULNESS AS INTERVENTIONS
It is important to note that the effects
of mindfulness are conceptually different
from effects of repeated mental simula-
tion. First, repeated mental simulation,but
not mindfulness, needs repetition to show
effects. Second, repeated mental simula-
tion shows specific effects on a particular
kind of food (26) but this is not the case
for mindful attention (6). At present, it
can be assumed that the habituation effects
of repeated mental simulation reflect a
memory process that leads to inhibition
after some length of imagining exposure to
food, whereas mindfulness might be more
likely to block the tempting simulation of
food consumption. Both processes are of
interest for practice. Mindfulness could be
Frontiers in Psychiatry | Affective Disorders and Psychosomatic Research April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 48 | 2
Missbach et al. Mental imagery and food consumption behavior
used as a method for reducing unhealthy
food intake. By contrast, repeatedly imag-
ining food consumption might reduce
food intake when consumption has already
begun. For example, to increase habitu-
ation to a particular kind of food, indi-
viduals could think about consuming an
unhealthy food that they usually prefer
to eat (e.g., chocolate) repeatedly across
consecutive days. However, while such
long-term effects were demonstrated with
exposure to real food (34), it is still a task
for future research to study the long-term
effects of imagined food consumption. But
there is no doubt that the relevance of
habituation processes is obvious if we con-
sider recent research that has shown that
overweight children habituate more slowly
during consumption than non-overweight
children (35). Against this background,
research on repeated mental simulations
could provide a promising starting point
from which to advance interventions in
food consumption.
CONCLUSION
Current research on food intake behav-
ior regulation is driven by a vital
need to understand the successful and
unsuccessful self-regulation of food con-
sumption. Integrating the cognitive mech-
anisms of mental imagery and mindfulness
that guide eating behaviors may be one
important milestone for enhancing current
models of food intake regulation. As pro-
posed by Redden (36), a general model
should include reflective (memory recall
inferences, metacognitions) and perceptual
(adaptation, habituation) components of
satiation. He argues that satiation is par-
tially constructed in the moment on the
basis of external cues that interact with each
other in a specific eating situation. Research
on mental imagery and mindfulness can
help us understand eating behavior and
to design individual-level interventions. It
can also help us understand why some indi-
viduals are more successful self-regulators
than others (37).
REFERENCES
1. Pearson J, Kosslyn SM. Mental imagery. Front Psy-
chol (2013) 4:198. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00198
2. Pylyshyn ZW. Mental imagery: in search of a the-
ory. Behav Brain Sci (2002) 25(2):157–82. doi:10.
1017/S0140525X02000043
3. Proust M, Davis L. Swann’s Way: In Search of Lost
Time. Penguin Classics Deluxeed. (Vol. 1). Ruther-
ford, NJ: Penguin Group US (2004).
4. Shepherd G, Shepherd-Barr K. Proust effect.
In: Binder M, Hirokawa N, Windhorst U, edi-
tors. Encyclopedia of Neuroscience. Berlin: Springer
(2009). p. 3333–5.
5. Swinburn B, Egger G, Raza F. Dissecting obeso-
genic environments: the development and applica-
tion of a framework for identifying and prioritiz-
ing environmental interventions for obesity. Prev
Med (1999) 29(6 Pt 1):563–70. doi:10.1006/pmed.
1999.0585
6. Papies EK, Barsalou LW, Custers R. Mindful
attention prevents mindless impulses. Soc Psy-
chol Pers Sci (2012) 3(3):291–9. doi:10.1177/
1948550611419031
7. Papies EK. Tempting food words activate eating
simulations. Front Psychol (2013) 4:838. doi:10.
3389/fpsyg.2013.00838
8. Hesslow G. The current status of the simulation
theory of cognition. Brain Res (2012) 1428:71–9.
doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.06.026
9. Krishna A, Schwarz N. Sensory marketing,
embodiment, and grounded cognition: a review
and introduction. J Consum Psychol (2014)
24(2):159–68. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2013.12.006
10. Krishna A. An integrative review of sensory
marketing: engaging the senses to affect per-
ception, judgment and behavior. J Consum
Psychol (2012) 22(3):332–51. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.
2011.08.003
11. Elder RS, Krishna A. The “visual depiction effect”
in advertising: facilitating embodied mental simu-
lation through product orientation. J Consum Res
(2012) 38(6):988–1003. doi:10.1086/661531
12. Mitchell DJ, Kahn BE, Knasko SC. There’s some-
thing in the air: effects of congruent or incongru-
ent ambient odor on consumer decision making.
J Consum Res (1995) 22(2):229–38. doi:10.1086/
209447
13. Seo HS, Roidl E, Muller F, Negoias S. Odors
enhance visual attention to congruent objects.
Appetite (2010) 54(3):544–9. doi:10.1016/j.appet.
2010.02.011
14. Hoegg J, Alba JW. Taste perception: more
than meets the tongue. J Consum Res (2007)
33(4):490–8. doi:10.1086/510222
15. Leclerc F, Schmitt BH, Dube L. Foreign branding
and its effects on product perceptions and atti-
tudes. J Mark Res (1994) 31(2):263–70. doi:10.
2307/3152198
16. Barsalou LW. Grounded cognition. Annu Rev
Psychol (2008) 59:617–45. doi:10.1146/annurev.
psych.59.103006.093639
17. Niedenthal PM, Barsalou LW, Winkielman P,
Krauth-Gruber S, Ric F. Embodiment in atti-
tudes, social perception, and emotion. Pers Soc
Psychol Rev (2005) 9(3):184–211. doi:10.1207/
s15327957pspr0903_1
18. Simmons WK, Martin A, Barsalou LW. Pic-
tures of appetizing foods activate gustatory cor-
tices for taste and reward. Cereb Cortex (2005)
15(10):1602–8. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi038
19. Schussler P, Kluge M, Yassouridis A, Dresler M,
Uhr M, Steiger A. Ghrelin levels increase after pic-
tures showing food. Obesity (Silver Spring) (2012)
20(6):1212–7. doi:10.1038/oby.2011.385
20. Uchida A, Zigman JM, Perello M. Ghrelin and
eating behavior: evidence and insights from
genetically-modified mouse models. Front Neu-
rosci (2013) 7:121. doi:10.3389/fnins.2013.00121
21. van der Plasse G, Merkestein M, Luijendijk MC,
van der Roest M,Westenberg HG, MulderAB, et al.
Food cues and ghrelin recruit the same neuronal
circuitry. Int J Obes (Lond) (2013) 37(7):1012–9.
doi:10.1038/ijo.2012.174
22. KempsE, Tiggemann M. A role for mental imagery
in the experience and reduction of food cravings.
Front Psychiatry (2014) 5:193. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.
2014.00193
23. Martin CK, McClernon FJ, Chellino A, Correa
JB. Food cravings: a central construct in food
intake behavior, weight loss, and the neurobiol-
ogy of appetitive behavior. In: Preedy VR, Wat-
son RR, Martin CR, editors. Handbook of Behavior,
Food and Nutrition (2011). New York: Springer.
p. 741–55.
24. Epstein LH, Temple JL, Roemmich JN, Bouton
ME. Habituation as a determinant of human food
intake. Psychol Rev (2009) 116(2):384–407. doi:10.
1037/a0015074
25. Missbach B, Florack A, Weissmann L, Konig J.
Mental imagery interventions reduce subsequent
food intake only when self-regulatory resources
are available. Front Psychol (2014) 5:1391. doi:10.
3389/fpsyg.2014.01391
26. Morewedge CK, Huh YE, Vosgerau J. Thought
for food: imagined consumption reduces actual
consumption. Science (2010) 330(6010):1530–3.
doi:10.1126/science.1195701
27. Larson JS, ReddenJP, Elder RS. Satiation fromsen-
sory simulation: evaluating foods decreases enjoy-
ment of similar foods. J Consum Psychol (2014)
24(2):188–94. doi:10.1016/j.jcps.2013.09.001
28. Missbach B, Florack A, Weissmann L, König J.
Mental imagery interventions reduce subsequent
food intake only when self-regulatory resources
are available. Front Psychol (2014) 5:1391. doi:10.
3389/fpsyg.2014.01391
29. Harris JL, Bargh JA, Brownell KD. Priming effects
of television food advertising on eating behavior.
Health Psychol (2009) 28(4):404–13. doi:10.1037/
a0014399
30. Higgs S,Woodward M. Television watching during
lunch increases afternoon snack intake of young
women. Appetite (2009) 52(1):39–43. doi:10.1016/
j.appet.2008.07.007
31. Webb TL, Sheeran P. Can implementation inten-
tions help to overcome ego-depletion? J Exp Soc
Psychol (2003) 39(3):279–86. doi:10.1016/S0022-
1031(02)00527-9
32. Scarabis M, Florack A, Gosejohann S. When con-
sumers follow their feelings: the impact of affec-
tive or cognitive focus on the basis of consumers’
choice. Psychol Market (2006) 23(12):1015–34.
doi:10.1002/mar.20144
33. Papies EK, Pronk TM, Keesman M, Barsalou LW.
The benefits of simply observing: mindful atten-
tion modulates the link between motivation and
behavior. J Pers Soc Psychol (2015) 108(1):148–70.
doi:10.1037/a0038032
34. Epstein LH, Carr KA, Cavanaugh MD, Paluch RA,
Bouton ME. Long-term habituation to food in
obese and nonobese women. AmJ Clin Nutr (2011)
94(2):371–6. doi:10.3945/ajcn.110.009035
www.frontiersin.org April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 48 | 3
Missbach et al. Mental imagery and food consumption behavior
35. Temple JL, Giacomelli AM, Roemmich JN,
Epstein LH. Overweight children habituate slower
than non-overweight children to food. Phys-
iol Behav (2007) 91(2–3):250–4. doi:10.1016/j.
physbeh.2007.03.009
36. Redden JP. Desire over time: the multi-
faceted nature of satiation. In: Hofmann
W, Nordgren L, editors. The Psychology of
Desire. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2014)
(in press).
37. Friese M, Engeler M, Florack A. Self-perceived
successful weight regulators are less affected by
self-regulatory depletion in the domain of eating
behavior. Eat Behav (2015) 16:5–8. doi:10.1016/j.
eatbeh.2014.10.011
Conflict of Interest Statement: The authors declare
that the research was conducted in the absence of any
commercial or financial relationships that could be
construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Received: 28 January 2015; accepted: 23 March 2015;
published online: 07 April 2015.
Citation: Missbach B, Florack A and König J (2015)
Mental imagery and food consumption. Front. Psychia-
try 6:48. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00048
This article was submitted to Affective Disorders and Psy-
chosomatic Research, a section of the journal Frontiers in
Psychiatry.
Copyright © 2015 Missbach, Florack and König . This
is an open-access article distributed under the terms
of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC
BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other
forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or
licensor are credited and that the original publication
in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted
academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduc-
tion is permitted which does not comply with these
terms.
Frontiers in Psychiatry | Affective Disorders and Psychosomatic Research April 2015 | Volume 6 | Article 48 | 4
... In light of these intriguing findings, the next step is to utilize the repeated imagined-consumption paradigm as an intervention that can be applied to decrease the consumption of specific food types (Missbach et al., 2015). Unlike memory inhibition skills that are useful from preventing cues in the environment to retrieve thoughts about eating desires (Davidson et al., 2005), the technique of imagined consumption might potentially become a weight management tool that could be applied as an effective strategy when already being confronted with a cue-elicited eating desire (e.g., Jansen et al., 2003). ...
... Mental simulation of food consumption has been called upon as yet another prospective basis for developing interventions to effectively reduce food intake (Morewedge et al., 2010;Missbach et al., 2015). The main objective of this study was to investigate conditions under which the repeated imaginedconsumption paradigm could be applied more efficiently. ...
Article
Full-text available
Imagined food consumption is a method of elaborately imagining oneself eating a specific food that, when repeated 30 times, has been shown to decrease subsequent intake of the same food. The technique relies on a memory-based habituation process when behavioral and motivational responses to a stimulus decrease after its repeated presentation. Thus, repeatedly imagining food consumption leads to food-specific habituation effects. Large numbers of imagined consumption repetitions are effortful and time consuming and can be problematic when applied in interventions with the goal of reducing food intake. In the present study, we assessed the efficacy of the technique at smaller numbers of repetitions while testing motor simulation as a potential facilitator of the habituation-based consumption-reduction effect. 147 participants imagined eating chocolate pudding 15 or 3 consecutive times and simultaneously performed either facilitating or not-facilitating eating movements. Results showed that participants who imagined eating the chocolate pudding 15 times (M15 = 178.20, SD15 = 68.08) ate more of the pudding than those who imagined consuming it 3 times (M3 = 150.73, SD3 = 73.31). The nature of the motor movements that were performed did not impact this effect. The data suggest that the imagined food consumption technique can result in an unexpected increase in food consumption, when smaller numbers of imagination repetitions are performed.
... Healthy eating is important for physical (e.g., Hu, 2002) and psychological health (e.g., Akbaraly, et al., 2009;Jacka, Mykletun, Berk, Bjelland, & Tell, 2011). Some interventions have tried to promote healthy eating by changing the way people think about food (for reviews see Missbach, Florack, & König, 2015;and Tapper, 2017). One such method is based on mindfulness (Papies, Barsalou, & Custers, 2012;Papies, Pronk, Keesman, & Barsalou, 2015). ...
Article
Many people would like to reduce indulging in unhealthy foods, but find it difficult to do so. Previous research shows that individuals eat smaller portions of unhealthy hedonic food if they first imagine the sensory properties of tempting food (sensory imagery; Cornil & Chandon, 2016). Similarly, they show less preference for such food if they think about food in a detached way (decentering; Papies, Barsalou, & Custers, 2012; Papies, Pronk, Keesman, & Barsalou, 2015). Given that these two mindsets are seemingly at odds with each other, we compared them across two studies to examine their effects on the preference for (Experiment 1) and consumption of (Experiment 2) hedonic healthy and unhealthy food. Although sensory imagery and decentering had largely different effects for preferences towards healthy and unhealthy foods, they had comparable effects on the consumption of both types of foods, serving to reduce the effects of consumption in participants affected by hunger and emotional eating. These results suggest that while sensory imagery and decentering are based on different mechanisms, they produce similar results when it comes to the consumption of hedonic food, regardless of how healthy the food is.
Article
Desire is commonly understood as a mental state in relation to which we are passive. Since it seems to arise in us spontaneously, without antecedent deliberation, it also seems to constitute a paradigmatic type of mental state which is not up to us. In this paper, I will contest this idea. I will defend a view according to which we can actively shape our desires by controlling the way in which we imagine their contents. This view is supported both by behavioral and neural data which indicate that imagining can either strengthen or weaken our existing desires. Arguably, this influence is made possible by our capacity to imaginatively elaborate on the content of our desires. This gives a reason to think that what we desire is partially under our control. It is under our control only partially because we can influence our desires insofar as their content appears appealing to us in imagination.
Article
Full-text available
Method: Participants were assigned to an Imagery 1, Imagery 2 or Placebo group. For 4 weeks, each group was exposed to two conditions (morning text message vs. no morning text message). In the morning message condition, the Imagery groups received a text message with the instruction to mentally imagine performing an APA and the Placebo group received a placebo message. All participants received an evening text message: "Did you do your cardio today? If yes, what did you do?" 3 days per week. Results: Participants of the Imagery groups reported significantly more weekly minutes of APA in the morning text message condition compared with the no morning message condition. Conclusion: Electronic messages were effective at increasing minutes of APA.
Article
Full-text available
Research has shown that imagining food consumption leads to food-specific habituation effects. In the present research, we replicated these effects and further examined whether the depletion of self-regulatory resources would reduce the habituation effects of imagined food consumption. Since self-regulatory resources have been shown to reduce habituation effects during the perception of emotional stimuli, we expected a reduction in habituation effects from imagined food consumption when self-regulatory resources were depleted. In Study 1, we replicated habituation effects as a response to imagining gummy bear consumption with a high (36) and medium number (18) of repetitions in a camouflaged taste test. Participants imagining gummy bear intake showed decreased food intake compared with participants who imagined putting a coin into a laundry machine. The number of repetitions did not significantly moderate the observed habituation effect. In Study 2, we investigated whether self-regulatory depletion would impede habituation effects evoked by the imagination of walnut consumption. Participants in a depleted state did not show a reduction in food intake after imagining walnut intake compared with participants in a non-depleted state. We discuss directions for future research and processes that might underlie the observed moderating effect of self-regulatory resources.
Article
Full-text available
Mindful attention, a central component of mindfulness meditation, can be conceived as becoming aware of one’s thoughts and experiences and being able to observe them as transient mental events. Here, we present a series of studies demonstrating the effects of applying this metacognitive perspective to one’s spontaneous reward responses when encountering attractive stimuli. Taking a grounded cognition perspective, we argue that reward simulations in response to attractive stimuli contribute to appetitive behavior and that motivational states and traits enhance these simulations. Directing mindful attention at these thoughts and seeing them as mere mental events should break this link, such that motivational states and traits no longer affect reward simulations and appetitive behavior. To test this account, we trained participants to observe their thoughts in reaction to appetitive stimuli as mental events, using a brief procedure designed for nonmeditators. Across 3 experiments, we found that adopting the mindful attention perspective reduced the effects of motivational states and traits on appetitive behavior in 2 domains, in both the laboratory and the field. Specifically, after applying mindful attention, participants’ sexual motivation no longer made opposite-sex others seem more attractive and thus desirable as partners. Similarly, participants’ levels of hunger no longer boosted the attractiveness of unhealthy foods, resulting in healthier eating choices. We discuss these results in the context of mechanisms and applications of mindful attention and explore how mindfulness and mindful attention can be conceptualized in psychological research more generally.
Article
Full-text available
This study shows that tempting food words activate simulations of eating the food, including simulations of the taste and texture of the food, simulations of eating situations, and simulations of hedonic enjoyment. In a feature listing task, participants generated features that are typically true of four tempting foods (e.g., chips) and four neutral foods (e.g., rice). The resulting features were coded as features of eating simulations if they referred to the taste, texture, and temperature of the food (e.g., “crunchy”; “sticky”), to situations of eating the food (e.g., “movie”; “good for Wok dishes”), and to the hedonic experience when eating the food (e.g., “tasty”). Based on the grounded cognition perspective, it was predicted that tempting foods are more likely to be represented in terms of actually eating them, so that participants would list more features referring to eating simulations for tempting than for neutral foods. Confirming this hypothesis, results showed that eating simulation features constituted 53% of the features for tempting food, and 26% of the features for neutral food. Visual features, in contrast, were mentioned more often for neutral foods (45%) than for tempting foods (19%). Exploratory analyses revealed that the proportion of eating simulation features for tempting foods was positively correlated with perceived attractiveness of the foods, and negatively with participants’ dieting concerns, suggesting that eating simulations may depend on individuals’ goals with regard to eating. These findings are discussed with regard to their implications for understanding the processes guiding eating behavior, and for interventions designed to reduce the consumption of attractive, unhealthy food.
Article
There has been a recent swell of interest in marketing as well as psychology pertaining to the role of sensory experiences in judgment and decision making. Within marketing, the field of sensory marketing has developed which explores the role of the senses in consumer behavior. In psychology, the dominant computer metaphor of information processing has been challenged by researchers demonstrating various manners in which mental activity is grounded in sensory experience. These findings are arduous to explain using the amodal model of the human mind. In this introduction, we first delineate key assumptions of the information processing paradigm and then discuss some of the key conceptual challenges posed by the research generally appearing under the titles of embodiment, grounded cognition, or sensory marketing. We then address the use of bodily feelings as a source of information; next, we turn to the role of context sensitive perception, imagery, and simulation in consumer behavior, and finally discuss the role of metaphors. Through this discourse, we note the contributions to the present special issue as applicable.
Article
With three experiments, the authors examine the notion that foreign branding-the strategy of pronouncing or spelling a brand name in a foreign language-triggers cultural stereotypes and influences product perceptions and attitudes. Choosing French brands as one specific case, Experiment 1 shows that the French pronunciation of a brand name affects the perceived hedonism of the products, attitudes toward the brand, and attitudes toward the brand name. Experiment 2 shows that congruent country-of-origin information, added to French branding, does not result in more hedonic perceptions; incongruence, however, diminishes the effect. In Experiment 3, an actual product taste test is performed. Despite the presence of direct sensory experience, consumer perceptions of a product change as a result of French branding.
Chapter
Food cravings are discrete phenomena involving a strong desire to consume a specific food that is hard to resist. Food cravings are associated with hunger, but food cravings are only alleviated by consumption of a specific type of food, while hunger can be alleviated by eating any number of foods. Food cravings are common and are associated with food intake and body mass, and neuroimaging studies indicate that exposure to imagined or actual craved foods increases activation in brain regions subserving reward, motivation, and memory. A number of self-report inventories with good psychometric properties are available that measure food cravings, including inventories to measure cravings for specific types of foods, such as chocolate and carbohydrates. Although food cravings were once hypothesized to result from food restriction or nutrient deficiencies, there is little empirical support for this etiological model. For example, food restriction during dieting and weight loss decreases food cravings, and more restrictive diets are associated with larger craving decreases. An alternative etiological model with empirical support involves conditioning, where food cravings result from pairing food intake with stimuli such as hunger, emotional states, or environmental stimuli. Randomized controlled trials are warranted to evaluate the efficacy of interventions to manage food cravings, and further research is needed to evaluate changes in food cravings in response to: (1) short- and long-term restriction of specific foods, with and without energy restriction/weight loss and (2) restriction of specific types of foods during diets that vary in intensity/weight loss. Results from such studies will improve our understanding of the etiology of food cravings and provide information on effective methods to manage food cravings.
Article
We demonstrate in two studies that people get more satiated on a food after repeatedly rating or choosing among similar foods shown in pictures. Repeated evaluations of food apparently have an effect similar to actual consumption—decreased enjoyment of foods that share a similar taste characteristic (i.e., sensory-specific satiety). We provide mediation evidence to show that satiation manifests because considering a food engenders spontaneous simulations of the taste of that food item, which by itself is enough to produce satiation. These findings establish sensory simulations as an important mechanism underlying satiation, and provide behavioral evidence that simple evaluations can produce sensory-specific satiety.
Article
I define “sensory marketing” as “marketing that engages the consumers' senses and affects their perception, judgment and behavior.” From a managerial perspective, sensory marketing can be used to create subconscious triggers that characterize consumer perceptions of abstract notions of the product (e.g., its sophistication or quality). Given the gamut of explicit marketing appeals made to consumers every day, subconscious triggers which appeal to the basic senses may be a more efficient way to engage consumers. Also, these sensory triggers may result in consumers' self-generation of (desirable) brand attributes, rather than those verbally provided by the advertiser. The understanding of these sensory triggers implies an understanding of sensation and perception as it applies to consumer behavior—this is the research perspective of sensory marketing. This review article presents an overview of research on sensory perception. The review also points out areas where little research has been done, so that each additional paper has a greater chance of making a bigger difference and sparking further research. It is quite apparent from the review that there still remains tremendous need for research within the domain of sensory marketing—research that can be very impactful.