ArticlePDF Available

Developmental Sequelae and Neurophysiologic Substrates of Sensory Seeking in Infant Siblings of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

It has been proposed that early differences in sensory responsiveness arise from atypical neural function and produce cascading effects on development across domains. This longitudinal study prospectively followed infants at heightened risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) based on their status as younger siblings of children diagnosed with ASD (Sibs-ASD) and infants at relatively lower risk for ASD (siblings of typically developing children; Sibs-TD) to examine the developmental sequelae and possible neurophysiological substrates of a specific sensory response pattern: unusually intense interest in nonsocial sensory stimuli or “sensory seeking.” At 18 months, sensory seeking and social orienting were measured with the Sensory Processing Assessment, and a potential neural signature for sensory seeking (i.e., frontal alpha asymmetry) was measured via resting state electroencephalography. At 36 months, infants’ social symptomatology was assessed in a comprehensive diagnostic evaluation. Sibs-ASD showed elevated sensory seeking relative to Sibs-TD, and increased sensory seeking was concurrently associated with reduced social orienting across groups and resting frontal asymmetry in Sibs-ASD. Sensory seeking also predicted later social symptomatology. Findings suggest that sensory seeking may produce cascading effects on social development in infants at risk for ASD and that atypical frontal asymmetry may underlie this atypical pattern of sensory responsiveness.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Accepted Manuscript
Title: Developmental Sequelae and Neurophysiologic
Substrates of Sensory Seeking in Infant Siblings of Children
with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Authors: Cara R. Damiano-Goodwin, Tiffany G. Woynaroski,
David M. Simon, Lisa V. Iba˜
nez, Michael Murias, Anne
Kirby, Cassandra R. Newsom, Mark T. Wallace, Wendy L.
Stone, Carissa J. Cascio
PII: S1878-9293(16)30147-5
DOI: http://dx.doi.org/doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2017.08.005
Reference: DCN 480
To appear in:
Received date: 9-8-2016
Revised date: 6-7-2017
Accepted date: 9-8-2017
Please cite this article as: Damiano-Goodwin, Cara R., Woynaroski, Tiffany G.,
Simon, David M., Iba˜
nez, Lisa V., Murias, Michael, Kirby, Anne, Newsom,
Cassandra R., Wallace, Mark T., Stone, Wendy L., Cascio, Carissa J., Developmental
Sequelae and Neurophysiologic Substrates of Sensory Seeking in Infant Siblings
of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder.Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience
http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2017.08.005
This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication.
As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript.
The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof
before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process
errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that
apply to the journal pertain.
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 1
Developmental Sequelae and Neurophysiologic Substrates of
Sensory Seeking in Infant Siblings of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder
Cara R. Damiano-Goodwin1*, Tiffany G. Woynaroski2 3*, David M. Simon4 5, Lisa V. Ibañez6,
Michael Murias7, Anne Kirby8, Cassandra R. Newsom3 9, Mark T. Wallace 2 3 5 10 11, Wendy L.
Stone6, Carissa J. Cascio3 5 11
1 Duke Center for Autism and Brain Development, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA
2 Department of Hearing and Speech Sciences, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville,
TN, USA
3 Vanderbilt Kennedy Center, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, USA
4 Neuroscience Graduate Program, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Vanderbilt University,
Nashville, TN, USA
5 Vanderbilt Brain Institute, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Vanderbilt University,
Nashville, TN, USA
6 Department of Psychology, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, USA
7 Duke Institute for Brain Sciences, Duke University, Durham, NC, USA
8 Department of Occupational Therapy, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT, USA
9 Department of Pediatrics, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN, USA
10 Department of Psychological Sciences, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN, USA
11 Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Vanderbilt University Medical Center,
Nashville, TN, USA
*Drs. Damiano-Goodwin and Woynaroski made equal contributions to this work.
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 2
Corresponding Author:
Carissa J Cascio
1601 23rd Ave, S, Suite 3057
Nashville, TN 37232.
Email: carissa.cascio@vanderbilt.edu
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 3
Graphical Abstract
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 4
Highlights
Infant siblings of children with autism (Sibs-ASD) display elevated sensory seeking.
Sibs-ASD who will be diagnosed with ASD show the highest levels of sensory seeking.
Sensory seeking at 18 months predicts future social symptomatology in Sibs-ASD.
The effect of seeking on social symptomatology is explained by reduced social orienting.
Atypical frontal asymmetry may underlie early differences in sensory seeking.
Abstract
It has been proposed that early differences in sensory responsiveness arise from atypical neural
function and produce cascading effects on development across domains. This longitudinal study
prospectively followed infants at heightened risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) based on
their status as younger siblings of children diagnosed with ASD (Sibs-ASD) and infants at
relatively lower risk for ASD (siblings of typically developing children; Sibs-TD) to examine the
developmental sequelae and possible neurophysiological substrates of a specific sensory
response pattern: unusually intense interest in nonsocial sensory stimuli or “sensory seeking.”
At 18 months, sensory seeking and social orienting were measured with the Sensory
Processing Assessment, and a potential neural signature for sensory seeking (i.e., frontal alpha
asymmetry) was measured via resting state electroencephalography. At 36 months, infants’
social symptomatology was assessed in a comprehensive diagnostic evaluation. Sibs-ASD
showed elevated sensory seeking relative to Sibs-TD, and increased sensory seeking was
concurrently associated with reduced social orienting across groups and resting frontal
asymmetry in Sibs-ASD. Sensory seeking also predicted later social symptomatology. Findings
suggest that sensory seeking may produce cascading effects on social development in infants
at risk for ASD and that atypical frontal asymmetry may underlie this atypical pattern of sensory
responsiveness.
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 5
Keywords: sensory, autism, infant siblings, longitudinal, frontal asymmetry, EEG
1. Introduction
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is characterized by social and communication deficits
accompanied by a pattern of repetitive behaviors, restricted interests, and unusual responses to
sensory stimuli. Atypical sensory responsiveness has been observed or reported in many
individuals with ASD from infancy to adulthood (Baranek, David, Poe, Stone, & Watson, 2006;
Crane, Goddard, & Pring, 2009; Dawson & Watling, 2000). Previous research indicates that
atypical sensory responsiveness emerges early in life (i.e., as early as 2-6 months of age;
Bryson et al., 2007; Dawson, Osterling, Meltzoff, & Kuhl, 2000), possibly before some of the
social and communicative impairments typically associated with ASD (Baranek, 1999; Dawson
et al., 2000; Mulligan & White, 2012). This evidence is consistent with the developmental
primacy of basic sensory neural pathways, many of which are in place prenatally (Anderson &
Thomason, 2013). Recent work also suggests that sensory responsiveness is related to other
core symptoms of ASD (Foss-Feig, Heacock, & Cascio, 2012; Gabriels et al., 2008; Kern et al.,
2007; Stevenson, Segers, Ferber, Barense, & Wallace, 2015). Together, this evidence suggests
that atypical sensory behaviors may serve as early markers of ASD risk. Furthermore, since the
early sensory environment likely influences the development of social and communicative skills
associated with ASD, a better understanding of the cascading effects of basic sensory
disturbances may elucidate the developmental trajectory of later-developing social and
communication deficits in ASD. However, to better understand this process, prospective
longitudinal studies are needed to examine the predictive value of sensory behaviors on social
and communicative development (Cascio, Woynaroski, Baranek, & Wallace, 2016).
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 6
Given the early development of atypical sensory responsiveness, it is possible that
behavioral response patterns to sensory stimuli may serve as an early biomarker or
endophenotype of ASD. An endophenotype is a heritable intermediate phenotype that is
expressed in both affected and non-affected family members. The identification of
endophenotypes provides clues into the biological underpinnings of a disorder (Gottesman &
Gould, 2003), and may ultimately help to discriminate between inherited behavioral features of
the disorder and features that are secondary or epiphenomenal. Infant siblings of individuals
with ASD are an ideal population in which to study candidate endophenotypes. This line of
research compares infant siblings of children diagnosed with ASD (Sibs-ASD) to infant siblings
of typically developing children (Sibs-TD), in order to better understand the characteristics
associated with a genetic risk for ASD. Sibs-ASD are at heightened risk for ASD and, even
when they are not later diagnosed with ASD, often show subclinical symptoms associated with
the disorder (Georgiades et al., 2013; Messinger et al., 2013; Ozonoff et al., 2014; Stone,
McMahon, Yoder, & Walden, 2007). Despite some preliminary studies suggesting that atypical
sensory responsiveness may be an early characteristic of the broader ASD phenotype (Brian et
al., 2008; Loh et al., 2007; Ozonoff et al., 2008), the developmental trajectory of atypical
sensory responsiveness in Sibs-ASD and the neural mechanisms driving these atypicalities
remain poorly understood.
Previous research suggests that atypical sensory responsiveness in ASD can be
characterized as three separate empirically-derived constructs: hyper-responsivity, hypo-
responsivity, and sensory seeking (BenSasson et al., 2008; Boyd et al., 2010). Most previous
research has focused on hypo- or hyper-responsivity (i.e., reduced or exaggerated behavioral
responses to sensory stimuli, respectively) in ASD. There is little research focusing specifically
on sensory seeking (Ben-Sasson et al., 2009), defined here as behaviors with the goal of
enhancing or prolonging a nonsocial sensory experience (e.g., visual examination, repetitive
touching, banging, or licking an object). In understanding the developmental trajectory of ASD
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 7
symptoms, sensory seeking is particularly of interest because these behaviors are likely to
divert attention away from social learning opportunities. In addition, though sensory seeking
may be directed towards other individuals (e.g., hair stroking), the odd nature of these behaviors
may actually obstruct the development of typical social relationships. Supporting this, sensory
seeking has been linked to increased severity of concurrent social and communication deficits in
children with ASD (Hilton, Graver, & LaVesser, 2007; Liss, Saulnier, Fein, & Kinsbourne, 2006;
Watson et al., 2011).
The extent to which early sensory seeking behaviors and early sensory experiences
more broadly relate to later ASD symptomatology remains unclear, however. In addition,
because sensory atypicalities are present so early in infancy and emerge before many of the
social and communicative impairments associated with ASD, the neural networks that support
these atypical sensory behaviors may also be critically involved in the emergence of the core
diagnostic features of ASD. Yet, very little is currently known about the neural correlates of
sensory responsiveness in typically developing infants, and virtually nothing is known about
these processes in Sibs-ASD. To address these questions, the present study examines sensory
seeking behaviors in Sibs-ASD and the neural mechanisms that may be driving these
behaviors. The present study also investigates the extent to which sensory seeking behaviors
might be related to later ASD symptomatology, and thus their potential utility as early markers or
endophenotypes for ASD.
As a potential neural basis for sensory seeking in infants at risk for ASD, the current
study measured asymmetry in resting state alpha band oscillations (approximately 6-9 Hz in
infants) at frontal electrode sites using EEG. Frontal asymmetry as measured by resting state
EEG (i.e., the relative difference in power between hemispheres in the alpha band) is a reliable
and stable measure that has been used to index risk for psychopathology across the lifespan
(Coan & Allen, 2004). Frontal asymmetry is believed to reflect individual differences in the
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 8
lateralization of brain activity which may be attributable to differences in thalamic inhibition of
cortical processing across hemispheres (Jensen & Mazaheri, 2010). It is partly heritable
(Anokhin, Heath, & Myers, 2006) and has been studied as a potential endophenotype in other
clinical conditions, such as depression (Allen & Cohen, 2010). Frontal asymmetry may also
serve as a marker of cortical development, as hemispheric differences in alpha power tend to
change over the course of the first two years of life, and may contribute to changes in
exploratory behavior over this developmental period (Fox, Calkins, & Bell, 1994; Fox,
Henderson, Rubin, Calkins, & Schmidt, 2001).
Patterns of resting frontal asymmetry may be particularly useful for understanding
sensory seeking in Sibs-ASD, as frontal asymmetry has already been correlated with ASD
symptomatology. More specifically, more severe ASD symptoms and social inhibition have been
observed in children with ASD with relatively greater right frontal asymmetry (Burnette et al.,
2011; Sutton et al., 2005), whereas fewer parent-reported social deficits have been observed in
children with relatively greater left frontal asymmetry. Most relevant to the present study, by the
age of 18 months Sibs-ASD on average demonstrate relatively greater right frontal asymmetry,
whereas Sibs-TD on average display relatively greater left frontal asymmetry (Gabard-Durnam,
Tierney, Vogel-Farley, Tager-Flusberg, & Nelson, 2015).
Frontal asymmetry has also been associated with sensory responsiveness in Sibs-ASD.
Specifically, the atypical pattern of greater right frontal asymmetry has already been observed to
co-occur with higher sensory hyporesponsivity in this high risk group (Simon et al., under
review). This finding is of interest because hyporesponsivity has been theoretically and
empirically linked to sensory seeking. Dunn's (1997) Model of Sensory Processing postulates
that sensory seeking and hyporesponsivity are both associated with a high neurological
threshold, but represent different behavioral response patterns (i.e., active versus passive).
Consistent with this theoretical framework, past studies have demonstrated behavioral
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 9
associations between hyporesponsivity and sensory seeking in children diagnosed with ASD
(Ausderau et al., 2014; Dunn, 1997; Freuler, Baranek, Watson, Boyd, & Bulluck, 2012).
Together, these results suggest that frontal asymmetry may reflect differences in neural
organization and processing that relate to sensory atypicalities in a population at risk for ASD.
The specific aims of the present study are as follows: (a) to determine whether high-risk
Sibs-ASD differ from low-risk Sibs-TD in sensory seeking behaviors at 18 months, (b) to
evaluate whether resting frontal asymmetry may reflect a potential neural mechanism underlying
sensory seeking differences, and (c) to examine whether early sensory seeking is related to
concurrent social orienting and later social deficits associated with ASD at 36 months. We
hypothesized that increased sensory seeking at 18 months diverts attention from important
social cues and thus reduces social orienting, which ultimately has cascading effects on social
development and results in more social deficits at 36 months. We also anticipated that Sibs-
ASD would exhibit more atypical frontal asymmetry patterns (i.e., greater right frontal
asymmetry) as previously observed in Sibs-ASD at 18 months (Gabard-Durnam et al., 2015).
Further, we predicted that right frontal asymmetry would serve as a potential neural correlate of
increased sensory seeking, as right frontal asymmetry has been linked with ASD
symptomatology in children who are diagnosed with or at risk for ASD (Burnette et al., 2011;
Simon et al., under review; Sutton et al., 2005).
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Overview of Study Design
To test our hypotheses, we drew upon data from a multisite, longitudinal study of Sibs-
ASD and Sibs-TD (Edmunds, Ibañez, Warren, Messinger, & Stone, 2016; Key et al., 2015). For
a subset of participants in the aforementioned study, sensory seeking and social orienting
behavior were measured when infants were 18 months old. Frontal asymmetry was measured
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 10
via resting state EEG at this same time point. Social symptomatology was measured when
participants were 36 months old, in the context of a comprehensive diagnostic assessment.
2.2. Participants
The current sample included 20 Sibs-ASD and 20 Sibs-TD from the larger longitudinal
study for whom sensory seeking, social orienting, and/or resting frontal asymmetry was
measured at 18 months (+/- 30 days) and for whom diagnostic outcomes were ascertained.
Infants were excluded from participation in the study if they had (a) severe motor, hearing or
vision impairment according to parent report, (b) identified metabolic, genetic or progressive
neurological disorders, (c) birth weight under 2500 grams, and/or (d) a gestational age of less
than 37 weeks. Sibs-TD were also required to have no history or present concern of
developmental delay and no family history of ASD in first-degree relatives. Proband diagnoses
for older siblings of Sibs-ASD and 36 month outcome diagnoses for Sibs-ASD and Sibs-TD
were confirmed by the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS; Lord et al., 2000),
Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (Lord, Rutter, & Le Couteur, 1994) and clinical judgment of
a licensed psychologist according to criteria from the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of
Mental Disorders 4th Edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 2000). The Mullen
Scales of Early Learning (Mullen, 1995) was administered at 12 months to characterize the
sample. Table 1 further details participant characteristics.
2.3. Measurement of Early Sensory Seeking
Infants’ sensory seeking was measured using the Sensory Processing Assessment
(SPA; Baranek, 1999), a behavioral sampling procedure developed to assess patterns of
sensory responsiveness in young children (with chronological ages approximately 9 months 6
years). During this 15- to 20-minute observational sample, children are presented with a series
of novel toys (e.g., musical dome, bubble blower, vibrating toy) that afford sensory experiences
across a number of modalities (e.g., visual, auditory, tactile). For each novel toy, two aspects of
sensory seeking were coded from video records of SPA administrations (Kirby, Little, Schultz, &
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 11
Baranek, 2015). An overall rating of sensory seeking behavior from 0-2, wherein 0 = no unusual
sensory seeking behavior, 1 = occasional clearly unusual and/or intense seeking interests, and
2 = frequent, intense and/or unusual sensory seeking, was assigned. Additionally, the presence
or absence (Y/N) of discrete sensory seeking behaviors in 12 categories (flap, posture, lick, bite,
smell, sight, touch, proprioceptive, spin, auditory, other repetitive, other) was recorded. From
these codes, we derived two variables for sensory seeking. Seeking intensity was quantified as
the sum of overall ratings of sensory seeking behavior (0-2) across all novel toys. Seeking
inventory was quantified as the total number of discrete sensory seeking behaviors endorsed for
the 12 aforementioned categories across the SPA sample. The z-scores for these two
component variables were averaged to create one aggregate variable that reflected both the
intensity and inventory of sensory seeking behavior. We aggregated these two component
variables because they were conceptually linked and empirically related, and because doing so
increases the stability and thus the potential validity, of the predictor (Rushton, Brainerd, &
Pressley, 1983). Evidence of the empirical relation amongst component variables will be
presented in the Preliminary Analyses section of the Results.
2.4. Measurement of Concurrent Social Orienting and Future Social Symptomatology
Within the context of the SPA, infants are also presented with a number of “presses” for
social orienting. When the infant is engaged with a novel toy, social stimuli (e.g., name call,
shoulder tap, wave) are presented until the child shows a definite behavioral orienting response
or for a maximum of three trials, whichever comes first. No prompting or scaffolding, aside from
the prescribed repetitions (max of three per bid), is provided in response to a failure of the infant
to orient. The infant is simply permitted to continue his or her ongoing activity. Infants
responses are assigned a score from 1-4, wherein 1 = child orients on the 1st trial, 2 = child
does not orient until the 2nd trial, 3 = child does not orient until the 3rd trial, or 4 = child does not
orient across the three trials for that stimulus. Social orienting was operationalized as the sum
of scores (1-4) assigned across all social orienting items. Thus, a higher score for social
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 12
orienting reflects reduced orienting/the need for increased cueing in order to elicit an orienting
response.
The ADOS Module 2 (Lord et al., 2000) was utilized to measure social symptomatology
at 36 months. This assessment was administered as part of comprehensive diagnostic
evaluation by a licensed psychologist who was research-reliable in ADOS administration and
experienced in evaluating young children with ASD. The Social Interaction domain score was
utilized as the metric of social symptomatology in analyses.
2.5. Measurement of Resting Frontal Asymmetry
Eyes open resting EEG was recorded while infants sat quietly on their parents’ laps in a
sound- and light-attenuated psychophysiology laboratory. Parents were instructed to help their
child sit as still as possible and watch a muted video that involved simple moving shapes (‘Baby
Einstein’, Kids II, Inc.). EEG data were collected from 124 (four eye channels excluded) or 128
electrodes using a NetAmp 200 or 400 amplifier and Geodesic Sensor Net (Electrical Geodesics
Inc.). Data were acquired at a sampling rate of 250 or 500 Hz, referenced to the vertex (Cz),
and online filtered from 0.1 to 100 Hz. Data were then exported and further processed using
EEGLAB (Delorme & Makeig, 2004), down sampled to 250 Hz and band-pass filtered with a
zero phase finite impulse response filter between 1-50 Hz.
Epochs 2 seconds long with 50% overlap were extracted, baseline corrected to the
mean, and rigorously visually inspected for artifacts and bad channels. We manually inspected
all channels for EOG, EMG, and movement artifacts and rejected epochs and channels
containing artifacts. Residual artifacts were corrected with independent component analysis.
Data were then re-referenced to the average, and removed channels were interpolated.
Peripheral electrodes (26 total) were excluded from all analyses due to high levels of artifact
contamination and interpolation. A total of 602 (± 118) seconds of data were recorded per
subject (range 232 803 seconds) and 279 (± 106) epochs were retained per subject (range 85
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 13
475). An average of 9.46 (± 3.23) channels were interpolated per subject, and an average of
2.96 (± 1.06) artifact related independent components were removed.
EEG epochs were transformed using a zero padded fast Fourier transform (0.061 Hz
resolution) after application of a Hann window. We selected two electrode groupings (six
electrodes each) centered on the F3/F4 selections used in previous studies of frontal alpha
power in children diagnosed with and infants at risk for ASD (Gabard-Durnam et al., 2015;
Sutton et al., 2005). The left frontal grouping consisted of electrode 25 (F3) and its five
neighbors (20, 21, 24, 28, and 29). The right frontal grouping consisted of electrode 124 (F4)
and its five neighbors (3, 4, 118, 119, and 123). Amplitude values were averaged across these
electrodes, squared to power, and natural log transformed. Frontal asymmetry was calculated
by subtracting log left power from log right power (log R log L). The alpha band was defined
as 6-9 Hz based on previous literature in children this age and confirmed by a notable peak in
this range in the average power spectrum.
2.6. Analytic Plan
An independent-samples t-test was utilized to test between-group differences in sensory
seeking and resting frontal asymmetry at 18 months. A series of multiple regression analyses
was then utilized to test (a) whether early sensory seeking was related to concurrent social
orienting and future social symptomatology and (b) whether early sensory seeking was related
to resting frontal asymmetry. For each of these models, variables were added in step-wise
fashion. The predictor of interest (sensory seeking or frontal asymmetry, respectively) was
entered first to examine whether it accounted for a significant amount of variance in the
dependent variable of interest (social indices and sensory seeking, respectively) across groups.
Risk group (Sibs-ASD versus Sibs-TD) and the relevant product (risk group × predictor of
interest) terms were then added to determine if risk group moderated the effects of the predictor
of interest on the dependent variable. Throughout regression analyses, Cook’s D was used to
determine whether any individual data points were unduly influencing regression coefficients
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 14
(Cook’s D greater than 1 was the criterion value applied for determining undue influence on the
regression line across all analyses).
2.7. Preparation of Data for Analysis
The chosen analysis method assumed multivariate normality, and multivariate normality
is more likely when univariate distributions do not grossly depart from the normal distribution
(Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). Thus, all variables were evaluated for normality. Variables showing
univariate skewness > |1.0| or kurtosis > |3.0| were transformed prior to imputation and analysis.
Missing data points (ranging from 5 - 32.5% across variables) were then imputed using
stochastic regression imputation, which generates plausible values for missing scores according
to the association of variables with missing data to variables with observed scores. This method
is preferable to traditional methods for dealing with missing data (e.g., listwise deletion, mean
imputation, last observation carried forward) in longitudinal data sets because it prevents loss of
information related to missing data, reduces bias, improves parameter estimates, and preserves
statistical power to detect effects of interest (Enders, 2010). This is particularly true when
auxiliary variables (which are not utilized in the primary analyses but provide extra information
about missing values) are considered in the imputation process (Enders, 2010), as was afforded
by the rich dataset from the multisite, longitudinal study of Sibs-ASD and Sibs-TD on which we
drew (Edmunds et al., 2016; Key et al., 2015). The data used in the present imputation model
included all variables involved in primary analyses as well as auxiliary variables from the
aforementioned larger dataset (e.g., Vineland Socialization scores) that were theoretically and
empirically (r values > .40 per Enders, 2010) related to, and thus possible of informing the
imputation of, one or more of the variables involved in primary analyses. Note that all analyses
reported in the results, by virtue of the stochastic regression imputation, are based on the
complete sample of 20 Sibs-ASD and 20 Sibs-TD, unless otherwise indicated (i.e., with the
exception of instances wherein undue influence was detected). Further information regarding
missing data, as well as effect sizes in observed versus imputed data for group differences and
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 15
associations contributing to the results we report here, are provided in accompanying
Supplemental Material (Tables S.1-S.3).
3. Results
3.1. Preliminary Analyses
3.1.1. Interobserver reliability for coded variables. Interobserver reliability of the
sensory seeking component variables, and the social orienting variable from the SPA, was
estimated for a randomly selected 20% of samples using absolute agreement intra-class
correlation coefficients (ICCs). The ICCs for seeking inventory and intensity were .78 and .80,
respectively. The ICC for social orienting was .99. Thus, inter-rater reliability was good-excellent
for coded variables.
3.1.2. Justification for aggregation of sensory seeking component variables. The
intercorrelation among the sensory seeking inventory and intensity component variables
exceeded our criterion (r .4) for aggregation (r = .86, p < .001).
3.1.3. Transformation of variables. ADOS Social Interaction domain scores at 36
months were positively skewed, but were corrected with a square-root transformation.
3.2. Primary Analyses
3.2.1. Group differences in sensory seeking and frontal asymmetry. Analyses
confirmed that Sibs-ASD and Sibs-TD groups significantly differed in early sensory seeking, t(38)
= -2.26, p = .029. Sibs-ASD (M = .24, SD = 1.09) showed significantly higher seeking relative to
Sibs-TD (M = -.44, SD = .79). This effect was moderate in magnitude (d = .72). Figure A.1
illustrates this result. Sibs-ASD also differed from Sibs-TD in frontal asymmetry at 18 months,
t(38) = 2.311, p = .026. Sibs-ASD (M = -.19, SD = .31) showed significantly greater right
asymmetry relative to Sibs-TD (M = .13, SD = .53). This effect was also moderate in magnitude
(d = .73). Figure A.2 depicts this result. Levene’s tests indicated Sibs-ASD and Sibs-TD were
non-significantly different in variance for early sensory seeking and/or resting frontal asymmetry,
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 16
and thus that the assumption of equal variance inherent to the t-test was not violated in either of
the aforementioned analyses.
3.2.2. Associations between frontal asymmetry and sensory seeking. The relation
between frontal asymmetry and sensory seeking at 18 months varied according to risk group (p
value for the frontal asymmetry x risk group product term in the moderation model < .001). One
participant unduly influenced the regression line within the Sibs-TD group (Cook’s D = 4.43).
Even with this participant removed from analyses, however, this moderated effect remained
significant (p value for the frontal asymmetry x risk group product term in the moderation model
= .002). The frontal asymmetry score was negatively correlated with sensory seeking (zero-
order correlation = -.528, p = .017) within the Sibs-ASD group, but positively correlated with
sensory seeking (zero-order correlation = .486, p = .035) within the Sibs-TD group. Thus,
increased sensory seeking was associated with relatively greater right asymmetry (reflected by
more negative scores for frontal asymmetry) in Sibs-ASD. Conversely, sensory seeking was
associated with relatively greater left asymmetry (reflected by more positive scores for frontal
asymmetry) in Sibs-TD. See Table A.2 for the results of regression analyses and Figure A.3 for
a depiction of this result. Note that the relation between frontal asymmetry and social orienting
at 18 months also varied according to risk group. Further information on this moderated relation
is provided in accompanying Supplemental Material (Result S.1).
3.2.3. Associations between sensory seeking and concurrent social orienting and
later social symptomatology. Sensory seeking at 18 months was related to concurrent social
orienting across groups (p value for the effect of seeking on social orienting, not controlling for
any additional factors < .001), and this effect did not significantly vary according to risk group (p
value for the sensory seeking x risk group product term in the moderation model = .24). One
infant was found to unduly influence the regression line within the Sibs-TD group (Cook’s D =
1.76). Yet, even with this participant removed from the analyses, this association remained
significant (p value for the effect of seeking on social orienting, not controlling for any additional
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 17
factors < .001), and the moderated effect did not reach statistical significance (p value for the
sensory seeking x risk group product term in the moderation model = .11). Within group
analyses excluding this participant, however, suggested that this effect was driven by the Sibs-
ASD group, who showed an extended range of seeking relative to the Sibs-TD group
(magnitude of the relation between sensory seeking and concurrent social orienting, excluding
the statistical outlier within the Sibs-TD group = .76 and .10 for Sibs-ASD and Sibs-TD,
respectively). Thus, increased sensory seeking was associated with reduced social orienting (as
a higher social orienting score reflects delayed or absent social orienting), but this effect is
largely limited to Sibs-ASD. See Table A.3 for the results of regression analyses and Figure A.4
for a depiction of this result.
Early sensory seeking additionally predicted future social symptomatology across groups
(p value for the effect of seeking on future social symptomatology, not controlling for other
factors = .008), but this effect significantly varied according to risk group (p value for the sensory
seeking x risk group product term in the moderation model = .008). Sensory seeking was
positively correlated with later social symptomatology in Sibs-ASD (r = .54, p = .014). Thus,
higher seeking at 18 months predicted increased social symptoms of ASD (i.e., reduced
reciprocal social interaction) at 36 months in Sibs-ASD. The relationship between sensory
seeking and social impairment trended in the opposite direction in Sibs-TD, but did not reach
statistical significance (r = -.34, p = .15). There was no evidence of undue influence within or
across groups in these analyses. See Table A.4 for the results of regression analyses and
Figure A.5 for a depiction of this result.
Early sensory seeking additionally predicted future social symptomatology across groups
(p value for the effect of seeking on future social symptomatology, not controlling for other
factors = .008), but this effect significantly varied according to risk group (p value for the sensory
seeking x risk group product term in the moderation model = .008). Sensory seeking was
positively correlated with later social symptomatology in Sibs-ASD (r = .54, p = .014). Thus,
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 18
higher seeking at 18 months predicted increased social symptoms of ASD (i.e., reduced
reciprocal social interaction) at 36 months in Sibs-ASD. The relationship between sensory
seeking and social impairment trended in the opposite direction in Sibs-TD, but did not reach
statistical significance (r = -.34, p = .15). There was no evidence of undue influence within or
across groups in these analyses. See Table A.4 for the results of regression analyses and
Figure A.5 for a depiction of this result.
3.3. Exploratory Analyses
Based on prior work, we anticipated that early social orienting would also be predictive of
future social symptomatology, at least in high risk infants (e.g., Baranek, 1999; Dawson et al.,
2004; Zwaigenbaum et al., 2005). Social orienting at 18 months did predict future social
symptomatology across risk groups (zero-order correlation for the effect of orienting on future
social symptomatology, not controlling for other factors = .38, p value = .017), but this effect
significantly varied according to risk group (p value for the social orienting x risk group product
term in the moderation model predicting future social symptomatology = .015). Social orienting
was positively correlated with later social symptomatology in Sibs-ASD (r = .68, p = .001), but
not in Sibs-TD (r = -.05, p = .84).
We subsequently examined whether the relation between early sensory seeking and
later social symptomatology could be explained, at least in part, by social orienting (i.e., whether
early sensory seeking indirectly impacted later reciprocal social interaction by reducing social
orienting in infancy). This possibility can be statistically tested using modern mediation analyses
that test the indirect effect of early sensory seeking on future social symptomatology through
social orienting (Hayes, 2009; See Figure A.6A). Two pathways comprise this indirect effect.
The first pathway, referred to as the “a path,” represents the relation between early sensory
seeking and social orienting. The second pathway, referred to as the “b path,” represents the
relation between social orienting and future social symptomatology, controlling for early sensory
seeking. An indirect effect is statistically significant when the confidence interval for the product
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 19
of the unstandardized coefficients for these two paths does not include zero. Because the
relations for early sensory seeking with social orienting and future social symptomatology were
only significant for Sibs-ASD in the analyses reported above, we suspected that this indirect
effect would only apply to the infants at heightened risk for ASD. That is, we thought that this
indirect effect would be conditional on risk group status. We therefore further evaluated whether
risk group moderated the mediation relation of interest.
The moderated mediation analysis depicted in Figure A.6A was tested using the
PROCESS macro in SPSS (Hayes, 2013). Bias-corrected confidence intervals for effects of
interest were generated using 1000 bootstrap samples with the confidence level set at 95%.
Results indicated that this proposed mediation relation was moderated by risk group, 95% CI for
the conditional indirect effect [.0001, .9148]. The indirect effect (a x b) was significant for the
Sibs-ASD group, 95% CI [.05, .84], but was not significant for the Sibs-TD group, 95% CI [-.10,
.30]. The significant indirect effect within the Sibs-ASD group confirms that social orienting
mediates the relation between early sensory seeking and later social symptomatology for the
high-risk group. This mediation is considered complete because the direct effect of early
sensory seeking on future social symptomatology, called the “c’ path,” becomes non-significant
when controlling for social orienting in Sibs-ASD, 95% CI [-.38, .45]. The indirect effect for the
Sibs-ASD group is depicted in Figure A.6B.
As sensory seeking and social orienting were measured concurrently, it is logical that
one may question whether the observed mediation relation would hold if the present predictor
(i.e., sensory seeking) and mediator (i.e., social orienting) were interchanged in the model. The
indirect effect of social orienting on future social symptomatology through sensory seeking was
not statistically significant, however, for either Sibs-ASD, 95% CI [-.33, .38] or Sibs-TD, 95% CI
[-.23, .33]. Thus, although this alternative explanation was an intriguing possibility, sensory
seeking does not appear to mediate the relation between social orienting and future social
symptomatology in either group.
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 20
In post hoc analyses, we additionally examined whether sensory seeking varied not only
according to risk group, but also between the following outcome groups: (a) Sibs-TD, all of
whom had non-ASD outcomes (n = 20), (b) Sibs-ASD who did not receive a diagnosis (Sibs-
ASD-No ASD; n = 14), and (c) Sibs-ASD who did receive a diagnosis of ASD (Sibs-ASD-Dx
ASD; n = 6). To explore this possibility, we carried out a one-way ANOVA with Least Significant
Difference (LSD) comparisons. Results confirmed that outcome groups did differ in early
sensory seeking, F(2, 37) = 4.87, p = .013 (Figure A.7). Post hoc comparisons revealed that Sibs-
ASD-Dx ASD showed increased sensory seeking relative to both Sibs-ASD-No ASD (p = .048)
and Sibs-TD (p = .003). The between-group difference for Sibs-ASD-No ASD and Sibs-TD did
not reach statistical significance (p = .21).
4. Discussion
The present study examined the developmental sequelae and neurophysiological
substrates of an understudied pattern of atypical sensory responsiveness - sensory seeking - in
a group of infants who were at high risk for ASD based on their status as infant siblings of
children who are diagnosed with ASD relative to a control group of infants at relatively lower risk
for ASD. The overarching aim of this work was to investigate sensory seeking as a potential
early marker for ASD risk, or endophenotype, as well as the mechanisms by which sensory
seeking might impact later social development. Towards this aim, we examined how sensory
seeking relates to concurrent social orienting at 18 months, as well as social symptomatology at
36 months, in Sibs-ASD versus Sibs-TD. We also explored a potential neural signature for
sensory seeking resting frontal asymmetry.
4.1. Sensory Seeking is Elevated and Related to Later Social Symptomatology in Sibs-
ASD
Past research has linked high levels of sensory seeking to social impairment in children
and adults who are diagnosed with ASD (Hilton et al., 2007; Liss et al., 2006; Watson et al.,
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 21
2011). We extend the aforementioned work to show that sensory seeking is elevated at 18
months and predictive of later social symptomatology in infants at high-risk for ASD, providing
further evidence that early differences in sensory function may produce cascading effects on
development across other domains, such as social skill (Cascio et al., 2016). Seeking appears
to be highest for those infant siblings who will go on to receive a diagnosis of ASD. These
findings suggest that sensory seeking behaviors at 18 months may serve as an endophenotype
of ASD or an early marker for social deficits in this high-risk population.
4.2. Social Orienting Mediates the Relation between Sensory Seeking and Social Deficits
Our findings indicate that high sensory seeking may impact social development via its
association with reduced social orienting. In our sample of Sibs-ASD, reduced social orienting
accounts for the significant association between sensory seeking and future social
symptomatology. This result suggests that infants at risk for ASD who engage in sensory
seeking behaviors may be too distracted or preoccupied with enhancing sensory experiences to
attend to social cues in their environment. By failing to orient to these social cues, these “high
seekers” are likely missing out on essential social learning opportunities, which may translate to
social deficits at 36 months. This finding has implications for clinical practice, as it suggests that
sensory seeking may be an important target of early intervention. Although, to our knowledge,
no treatment studies to date have attempted to target sensory seeking in infants at risk for ASD,
the current results suggest that social development might be facilitated by interventions that aim
to reduce the frequency of sensory seeking behaviors and/or to increase social orienting in
Sibs-ASD who are seeking out enhanced or prolonged non-social sensory experiences.
4.3. Atypical Resting Frontal Asymmetry May Underlie Sensory Seeking Behavior
As a potential neural marker for sensory seeking in this high-risk population, the present
study also examined whether frontal asymmetry, as indexed by the relative distribution of frontal
alpha oscillations, was (a) different in Sibs-ASD versus Sibs-TD and (b) associated with sensory
seeking. Alpha oscillations are believed to tap localized cortical inhibition from both cortical and
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 22
subcortical inputs, and this transient inhibition is believed to play a crucial role in the gating of
cortical processing (Jensen & Mazaheri, 2010). Although further research is warranted in order
to better understand the psychological constructs associated with frontal asymmetry, differences
in the relative balance of these inhibitory oscillations are believed to influence the degree to
which individual hemispheres contribute to top down regulatory functions critical for both ‘bottom
up’ processing of sensory inputs and regulation of complex behaviors such as attentional
orienting (e.g., Mazaheri, Nieuwenhuis, van Dijk, & Jensen, 2009).
Typically, infants gradually shift in the laterality of frontal asymmetry between 6 and 18
months of age, exhibiting relative left frontal asymmetry (i.e., relatively reduced alpha power in
the left versus right hemisphere) by 14 to 18 months of age (Fox et al., 1994; Fox et al., 2001;
Gabard-Durnam et al., 2015). Because alpha oscillations are inversely related to cortical
activation, this pattern can be interpreted as reflecting greater left hemisphere activation at rest
in typically developing infants by around 18 months. Recent research suggests that Sibs-ASD
show the opposite developmental trajectory, progressing from a frontal asymmetry pattern
suggestive of relative left hemisphere activation to relative right hemisphere activation over the
same 6 to 18 month window (Gabard-Durnam et al., 2015). This atypical pattern of frontal
asymmetry has additionally been documented, and linked with characteristics of ASD, in older
children who are diagnosed with autism (Burnette et al., 2011; Sutton et al., 2005), further
supporting the notion that a disruption in rhythmic neural processes is related to ASD
symptomatology.
As hypothesized, the Sibs-ASD group showed greater right frontal asymmetry when
compared to the Sibs-TD group at 18 months. This result is consistent with previous research
(Gabard-Durnam et al., 2015). In addition, within the Sibs-ASD group, increased sensory
seeking was associated with this atypical pattern of greater right frontal asymmetry (i.e.,
relatively reduced alpha power in the right versus left hemisphere) at 18 months of age. This
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 23
finding suggests that increased sensory seeking in Sibs-ASD may be associated with atypical
organization and maturation of oscillatory processes. It is also possible that greater right frontal
asymmetry may develop as a compensatory neural function due to reduced left cortical activity
in this population, as some previous research suggests that differences in the lateralization of
brain function may occur over development when one hemisphere compensates for reduced
functioning in the other hemisphere (Dehaene-Lambertz, Pena, Christophe, & Landrieu, 2004).
Further research is needed along these lines.
Interestingly, Sibs-TD demonstrated the opposite pattern, with relatively greater left
frontal asymmetry being associated with increased sensory seeking. This result is somewhat
difficult to interpret. We note, however, that there was a fairly limited range of sensory seeking
behavior observed across Sibs-TD. The moderated results that we obtained indicate that
increased seeking, to the extent that it was observed within the TD group, not only tended to co-
occur with a more left lateralized or “typical” resting brain state, but also did not predict future
social deficits. On the whole, these results may suggest that some degree of sensory seeking
behavior may be adaptive. Thus, there is a need for future research to determine at what point,
and in what populations, sensory seeking has implications for social development.
Past work linking frontal asymmetry to hyporesponsiveness in Sibs-ASD (Simon et al.,
under review) and to other symptoms of ASD within children who are diagnosed (Burnette et al.,
2011; Sutton et al., 2005), suggests that atypical frontal asymmetry is not specific to sensory
seeking, but is associated with sensory responsiveness and ASD symptomatology more
broadly. As such, further work is also warranted to elucidate how these early-emerging
oscillatory patterns contribute to atypical development in ASD.
4.4. Limitations and Future Directions
The results of the present study have important implications for understanding the
developmental course of social deficits associated with ASD and the broader autism phenotype,
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 24
but are limited. Findings are subject, of course, to the limitations of the longitudinal correlational
design. We cannot rule out alternative explanations for any of the associations we have
observed or conclude that any of the links that we have observed are causal in nature. Along
these lines, one possibility is that an unmeasured variable has contributed to one or more of the
relations of interest in this report. For example, it is possible that anxiety in the Sibs-ASD group
contributed to both reduced social orienting and increased sensory seeking during the sensory
assessment. Anxiety is of particular interest because it has previously been linked to ASD
symptomatology, including atypical responses to sensory stimuli (e.g., Mazurek et al., 2013). In
addition, anxiety has been associated with greater right frontal asymmetry (Davidson, Marshall,
Tomarken, & Henriques, 2000; Wiedemann et al., 1999). Future studies should examine the
potential role of anxiety in the relations among sensory seeking, social orienting, and frontal
asymmetry.
Additionally, measurement of sensory seeking and social orienting at a single time point
precludes us from establishing temporal precedence for sensory seeking. The same is true for
the concurrent measurement of frontal asymmetry and seeking behavior. Future studies, with
measurement at additional time points, planned a priori to establish the presence of atypical
frontal asymmetry prior to the emergence of sensory seeking, and to ascertain the presence of
sensory seeking prior to diminished social orienting, would increase our confidence in the
direction of effects suggested here. Further research is also needed to determine whether these
results generalize to other groups at risk for autism spectrum disorder (e.g., premature infants,
infants who are showing red flags but have no family history of ASD), or whether the present
pattern of results is specific to infants who are genetically predisposed to ASD. Finally,
subsequent work should seek to determine how we might best intervene upon atypical sensory
responsiveness in Sibs-ASD, and whether effects of treatment on sensory responses translate
to more optimal developmental outcomes in children who are diagnosed with, or at high risk for,
ASD. Large-scale studies that evaluate which of the variables of interest here (frontal alpha
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 25
asymmetry, sensory seeking, social orienting), in combination with other brain and behavioral
factors previously observed to covary with social symptoms, show incremental validity or have
“added-value” in predicting future social symptomatology in Sibs-ASD would be helpful for
focusing our efforts at early identification and intervention in infants at heightened risk for ASD.
5. Conclusions
The present study found that sensory seeking behaviors may serve as an early marker
for social deficits, or an endophenotype, in ASD. Infant siblings of children with ASD
demonstrate increased sensory seeking at 18 months, and sensory seeking is related to later
social symptomatology at 36 months. In addition, results provide insight into the mechanisms by
which sensory seeking might impact later social development by showing that the predictive
relationship between sensory seeking and social deficits at 36 months is mediated by social
orienting. Finally, findings suggest that resting frontal asymmetry is a potential neural marker of
atypical sensory seeking in this high-risk population. Further research is needed to increase our
confidence in the associations suggested here, to determine whether findings generalize to
other populations at high-risk for ASD, and to guide clinical practice with infants who present
with atypical patterns of sensory responsiveness.
Acknowledgments
The work described was supported by NIH U54 HD083211 (PI: Dykens), NICHD R01
HD057284 (PI: Stone), the Marino Autism Research Institute, the Wallace Foundation, the
Simons Foundation Autism Research Initiative, and by CTSA award No. KL2TR000446 from the
National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences. Its contents are solely the responsibility
of the authors and do not necessarily represent the official views of the National Center for
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 26
Advancing Translational Sciences, or the National Institutes of Health. The authors would like to
thank the laboratory of Dr. Grace Baranek for guidance in using the SPA and Warren Lambert
for his statistical support. The authors declare no conflicting interests.
Baranek, G. (1999). Autism during infancy: A retrospective video analysis of sensory-motor and
social behaviors at 9-12 months of age. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
29(3), 213-224.
Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early
social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to
distress. Developmental Psychology, 40(2), 271-282.
Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new
millennium. Communication Monographs, 76(4), 408-420. doi:
10.1080/03637750903310360
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A
regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005).
Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of
Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 143-152. doi: S0736574804000553
[pii]10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.05.001
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 27
References
Allen, J., & Cohen, M. (2010). Deconstructing the “resting” state: Exploring the temporal
dynamics of frontal alpha asymmetry as an endophenotype for depression. Frontiers in
Human Neuroscience, 4, 121-134.
American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders-IV-TR. Washington, DC: APA.
Anderson, A. L., & Thomason, M. E. (2013). Functional plasticity before the cradle: a review of
neural functional imaging in the human fetus. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews,
37(9), 2220-2232.
Anokhin, A., Heath, A., & Myers, E. (2006). Genetic and environmental influences on frontal
EEG asymmetry: A twin study. Biological Psychology, 71(3), 289-295.
Ausderau, K., Sideris, J., Furlong, M., Little, L. M., Bulluck, J., & Baranek, G. T. (2014). National
survey of sensory features in children with ASD: Factor structure of the sensory
experience questionnaire (3.0). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(4),
915-925.
Baranek, G. (1999). Sensory processing assessment for young children (SPA). Unpublished
manuscript, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill.
Baranek, G., David, F. J., Poe, M. D., Stone, W. L., & Watson, L. R. (2006). Sensory
experiences questionnaire: Discriminating sensory features in young children with
autism, developmental delays, and typical development. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry, 47(6), 591-601. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01546.x
Ben-Sasson, A., Hen, L., Fluss, R., Cermak, S. A., Engel-Yeger, B., & Gal, E. (2009). A meta-
analysis of sensory modulation symptoms in individuals with autism spectrum disorders.
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(1), 1-11. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-
0593-3
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 28
BenSasson, A., Cermak, S., Orsmond, G., TagerFlusberg, H., Kadlec, M., & Carter, A. (2008).
Sensory clusters of toddlers with autism spectrum disorders: Differences in affective
symptoms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(8), 817-825.
Boyd, B. A., Baranek, G. T., Sideris, J., Poe, M. D., Watson, L. R., Patten, E., & Miller, H.
(2010). Sensory features and repetitive behaviors in children with autism and
developmental delays. Autism Research, 3(2), 78-87.
Brian, J., Bryson, S., Garon, N., Roberts, W., Smith, I. M., Szatmari, P., & Zwaigenbaum, L.
(2008). Clinical assessment of autism in high-risk 18-month-olds. Autism, 12(5), 433-
456.
Bryson, S. E., Zwaigenbaum, L., Brian, J., Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., Rombough, V., &
McDermott, C. (2007). A prospective case series of high-risk infants who developed
autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(1), 12-24.
Burnette, C. P., Henderson, H. A., Inge, A. P., Zahka, N. E., Schwartz, C. B., & Mundy, P. C.
(2011). Anterior EEG asymmetry and the modifier model of autism. Journal of Autism
and Developmental Disorders, 41(8), 1113-1124. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1138-0
Cascio, C. J., Woynaroski, T., Baranek, G. T., & Wallace, M. T. (2016). Toward an
interdisciplinary approach to understanding sensory function in autism spectrum
disorder. Autism Research.
Coan, J. A., & Allen, J. (2003). Frontal EEG asymmetry and the behavioral activation and
inhibition systems. Psychophysiology, 40(1), 106-114.
Coan, J. A., & Allen, J. J. (2004). Frontal EEG asymmetry as a moderator and mediator of
emotion. Biological Psychology, 67(1), 7-50.
Crane, L., Goddard, L., & Pring, L. (2009). Sensory processing in adults with autism spectrum
disorders. Autism, 13(3), 215-228.
Davidson, R. (1993). Cerebral asymmetry and emotion: Conceptual and methodological
conundrums. Cognition & Emotion, 7(1), 115-138.
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 29
Davidson, R. (1998). Anterior electrophysiological asymmetries, emotion, and depression:
Conceptual and methodological conundrums. Psychophysiology, 35(5), 607-614.
Davidson, R., Marshall, J. R., Tomarken, A. J., & Henriques, J. B. (2000). While a phobic waits:
Regional brain electrical and autonomic activity in social phobics during anticipation of
public speaking. Biological Psychiatry, 47(2), 85-95.
Dawson, G., Osterling, J., Meltzoff, A. N., & Kuhl, P. (2000). Case study of the development of
an infant with autism from birth to two years of age. Journal of Applied Developmental
Psychology, 21(3), 299-313.
Dawson, G., & Watling, R. (2000). Interventions to facilitate auditory, visual, and motor
integration in autism: A review of the evidence. Journal of Autism and Developmental
disorders, 30(5), 415-421.
Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Pena, M., Christophe, A., & Landrieu, P. (2004). Phoneme perception in
a neonate with a left sylvian infarct. Brain and Language, 88(1), 26-38.
Delorme, A., & Makeig, S. (2004). EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial
EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. Journal of Neuroscience
Methods, 134(1), 9-21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009
Dunn, W. (1997). The impact of sensory processing abilities on the daily lives of young children
and their families: A conceptual model. Infants & Young Children, 9(4), 23-35.
Edmunds, S. R., Ibañez, L. V., Warren, Z., Messinger, D. S., & Stone, W. L. (2016). Longitudinal
prediction of language emergence in infants at high and low risk for autism spectrum
disorder. Development and Psychopathology. doi: 10.1017/SO954579416000146
Enders, C. (2010). Applied missing data analysis: Guilford Press.
Foss-Feig, J. H., Heacock, J. L., & Cascio, C. J. (2012). Tactile responsiveness patterns and
their association with core features in autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism
Spectrum Disorders, 6(1), 337-344.
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 30
Fox, N. A. (1991). If it's not left, it's right: Electroencephalograph asymmetry and the
development of emotion. American Psychologist, 46(8), 863-872.
Fox, N. A. (1994). Dynamic cerebral processes underlying emotion regulation. Monographs of
the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(23), 152-166.
Fox, N. A., Calkins, S. D., & Bell, M. A. (1994). Neural plasticity and development in the first two
years of life: Evidence from cognitive and socioemotional domains of research.
Development and Psychopathology, 6(04), 677-696.
Fox, N. A., Henderson, H. A., Rubin, K. H., Calkins, S. D., & Schmidt, L. A. (2001). Continuity
and discontinuity of behavioral inhibition and exuberance: Psychophysiological and
behavioral influences across the first four years of life. Child Development, 72(1), 1-21.
Freuler, A., Baranek, G. T., Watson, L. R., Boyd, B. A., & Bulluck, J. C. (2012). Precursors and
trajectories of sensory features: qualitative analysis of infant home videos. American
Journal of Occupational Therapy, 66(5), e81-e84.
Gabard-Durnam, L., Tierney, A., Vogel-Farley, V., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Nelson, C. A. (2015).
Alpha asymmetry in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and
Developmental Disorders, 45(2), 473-480.
Gabriels, R. L., Agnew, J. A., Miller, L. J., Gralla, J., Pan, Z., Goldson, E., . . . Hooks, E. (2008).
Is there a relationship between restricted, repetitive, stereotyped behaviors and interests
and abnormal sensory response in children with autism spectrum disorders? Research
in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(4), 660-670.
Georgiades, S., Szatmari, P., Boyle, M., Hanna, S., Duku, E., Zwaigenbaum, L., . . . Mirenda, P.
(2013). Investigating phenotypic heterogeneity in children with autism spectrum disorder:
a factor mixture modeling approach. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(2),
206-215.
Gottesman, I. I., & Gould, T. D. (2003). The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology
and strategic intentions. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160(4), 636-645.
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 31
Hagemann, D., Naumann, E., Thayer, J. F., & Bartussek, D. (2002). Does resting
electroencephalograph asymmetry reflect a trait? An application of latent state-trait
theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(4), 619.
Hayes, A. F. (2009). Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new
millennium. Communication Monographs, 76(4), 408-420. doi:
10.1080/03637750903310360
Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A
regression-based approach. New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Hilton, C., Graver, K., & LaVesser, P. (2007). Relationship between social competence and
sensory processing in children with high functioning autism spectrum disorders.
Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 1(2), 164-173.
Jensen, O., & Mazaheri, A. (2010). Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity:
Gating by inhibition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 186.
Kern, J. K., Trivedi, M. H., Grannemann, B. D., Garver, C. R., Johnson, D. G., Andrews, A. A., .
. . Schroeder, J. L. (2007). Sensory correlations in autism. Autism, 11(2), 123-134.
Key, A., Ibanez, L. I., Henderson, H. A., Warren, Z., Messinger, D. S., & Stone, W. L. (2015).
Positive affect processing and joint attention in infants at high risk for autism: An
exploratory study Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45, 4051-4062. doi:
10.1007/s10803-014-2191-x
Kirby, A. V., Little, L. M., Schultz, B., & Baranek, G. T. (2015). Observational characterization of
sensory interests, repetitions, and seeking behaviors. American Journal of Occupational
Therapy, 69(3), 6903220010p6903220011-6903220010p6903220019. doi:
10.5014/ajot.2015.015081
Liss, M., Saulnier, C., Fein, D., & Kinsbourne, M. (2006). Sensory and attention abnormalities in
autistic spectrum disorders. Autism, 10(2), 155-172.
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 32
Loh, A., Soman, T., Brian, J., Bryson, S. E., Roberts, W., Szatmari, P., . . . Zwaigenbaum, L.
(2007). Stereotyped motor behaviors associated with autism in high-risk infants: A pilot
videotape analysis of a sibling sample. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,
37(1), 25-36.
Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Jr., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., . . . Rutter,
M. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of
social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of
Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205-223.
Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Le Couteur, A. (1994). Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: A revised
version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive
developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24(5), 659-
685.
Mazaheri, A., Nieuwenhuis, I. L., van Dijk, H., & Jensen, O. (2009). Prestimulus alpha and mu
activity predicts failure to inhibit motor responses. Human Brain Mapping, 30(6), 1791-
1800.
Mazurek, M. O., Vasa, R. A., Kalb, L. G., Kanne, S. M., Rosenberg, D., Keefer, A., . . . Lowery,
L. A. (2013). Anxiety, sensory over-responsivity, and gastrointestinal problems in
children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 41(1),
165-176.
Messinger, D., Young, G. S., Ozonoff, S., Dobkins, K., Carter, A., Zwaigenbaum, L., . . .
Constantino, J. N. (2013). Beyond autism: a baby siblings research consortium study of
high-risk children at three years of age. Journal of the American Academy of Child &
Adolescent Psychiatry, 52(3), 300-308. e301.
Mullen, E. M. (1995). Mullen scales of early learning: Pearson.
Mulligan, S., & White, B. P. (2012). Sensory and motor behaviors of infant siblings of children
with and without autism. American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 66(5), 556-566.
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 33
Ozonoff, S., Macari, S., Young, G. S., Goldring, S., Thompson, M., & Rogers, S. J. (2008).
Atypical object exploration at 12 months of age is associated with autism in a
prospective sample. Autism, 12(5), 457-472.
Ozonoff, S., Young, G. S., Belding, A., Hill, M., Hill, A., Hutman, T., . . . Schwichtenberg, A.
(2014). The broader autism phenotype in infancy: When does it emerge? Journal of the
American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(4), 398-407. e392.
Pizzagalli, D. A., Sherwood, R. J., Henriques, J. B., & Davidson, R. J. (2005). Frontal brain
asymmetry and reward responsiveness a source-localization study. Psychological
Science, 16(10), 805-813.
Rushton, J., Brainerd, C., & Pressley, M. (1983). Behavioral development and construct validity:
The principle of aggregation. Psychological Bulletin, 94, 18-38. doi: 10.1037/0033-
2909.94.1.18
Simon, D., Damiano, C., Woynaroski, T., Ibañez, L., Murias, M., Stone, W. L., . . . Cascio, C.
(under review). Neural correlates of sensory hyporesponsiveness in toddlers at high risk
for autism spectrum disorder.
Smith, E. E., Reznik, S. J., Stewart, J. L., & Allen, J. J. (2016). Assessing and conceptualizing
frontal EEG asymmetry: An updated primer on recording, processing, analyzing, and
interpreting frontal alpha asymmetry. International Journal of Psychophysiology.
Stevenson, R. A., Segers, M., Ferber, S., Barense, M. D., & Wallace, M. T. (2015). The impact
of multisensory integration deficits on speech perception in children with autism
spectrum disorders. Multisensory and sensorimotor interactions in speech perception,
249.
Stone, W. L., McMahon, C. R., Yoder, P. J., & Walden, T. A. (2007). Early social-communicative
and cognitive development of younger siblings of children with autism spectrum
disorders. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 161(4), 384-390.
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 34
Sutton, S. K., Burnette, C. P., Mundy, P. C., Meyer, J., Vaughan, A., Sanders, C., & Yale, M.
(2005). Resting cortical brain activity and social behavior in higher functioning children
with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(2), 211-222. doi:
10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00341.x
Sutton, S. K., & Davidson, R. J. (1997). Prefrontal brain asymmetry: A biological substrate of the
behavioral approach and inhibition systems. Psychological Science, 8(3), 204-210.
Tabachnick, B., & Fidell, L. (2001). Using multivariate statistics (4th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn and
Bacon.
Watson, L., Patten, E., Baranek, G. T., Poe, M., Boyd, B. A., Freuler, A., & Lorenzi, J. (2011).
Differential associations between sensory response patterns and language, social, and
communication measures in children with autism or other developmental disabilities.
Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 54(6), 1562-1576.
Wiedemann, G., Pauli, P., Dengler, W., Lutzenberger, W., Birbaumer, N., & Buchkremer, G.
(1999). Frontal brain asymmetry as a biological substrate of emotions in patients with
panic disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 56(1), 78-84.
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 35
Figure A.1. Risk group differences in early sensory seeking. Sibs-ASD = Infant siblings of
children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Sibs-TD = Infant siblings of typically
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 36
developing children. Sensory seeking aggregate = average of z-scores for seeking intensity
(sum of seeking ratings across eight novel toys) and seeking inventory (sum of discrete seeking
behaviors across 12 categories) component variables derived from the Sensory Processing
Assessment (Baranek, 1999). Error bars represent standard error of the mean. * p < .05.
Figure A.2. Risk group differences in resting frontal asymmetry. Sibs-ASD = Infant siblings of
children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Sibs-TD = Infant siblings of typically
developing children. Frontal asymmetry = log right alpha power - log left alpha power (log R
log L). Note that negative scores for frontal asymmetry reflect relatively greater right hemisphere
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 37
activation (i.e., a more atypical pattern at this chronological age), whereas positive scores reflect
relatively greater left hemisphere activation (i.e., a more typical pattern at this chronological
age). Error bars represent standard error of the mean. * p < .05.
Figure A.3. Relation between resting frontal asymmetry and early sensory seeking. Sibs-TD =
Infant siblings of typically developing children. Sibs-ASD = Infant siblings of children diagnosed
with autism spectrum disorder. Sibs-ASD-Dx ASD = Infant siblings of children diagnosed with
autism spectrum disorder who are diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder at 36 months.
Sensory seeking aggregate = average of z-scores for seeking intensity (sum of seeking ratings
across eight novel toys) and seeking inventory (sum of discrete seeking behaviors across 12
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 38
categories) component variables derived from the Sensory Processing Assessment (Baranek,
1999). Note that negative scores for frontal asymmetry reflect relatively greater right hemisphere
activation (i.e., a more atypical pattern at this chronological age), whereas positive scores reflect
relatively greater left hemisphere activation (i.e., a more typical pattern at this chronological
age).
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 39
Figure A.4. Relation between early sensory seeking and social orienting. Sibs-TD = Infant
siblings of typically developing children. Sibs-ASD = Infant siblings of children diagnosed with
autism spectrum disorder. Sibs-ASD-Dx ASD = Infant siblings of children diagnosed with autism
spectrum disorder who are diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder at 36 months. Sensory
seeking aggregate = average of z-scores for seeking intensity (sum of seeking ratings across
eight novel toys) and seeking inventory (sum of discrete seeking behaviors across 12
categories) component variables derived from the Sensory Processing Assessment (Baranek,
1999).
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 40
Figure A.5. Relation between early sensory seeking and future social symptomatology. Sibs-TD
= Infant siblings of typically developing children. Sibs-ASD = Infant siblings of children
diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Sibs-ASD-Dx ASD = Infant siblings of children
diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder who are diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder at
36 months. Sensory seeking aggregate = average of z-scores for seeking intensity (sum of
seeking ratings across eight novel toys) and seeking inventory (sum of discrete seeking
behaviors across 12 categories) component variables derived from the Sensory Processing
Assessment (Baranek, 1999).
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 41
Figure A.6. Conditional indirect effect between early sensory seeking and future social
symptomatology through social orienting, according to risk group. Figure A depicts the
conceptual model for the moderated mediation relation. Figure B represents the significant
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 42
mediation relation within the Sibs-ASD group, product of a * b paths reflecting the indirect effect
= .41; 95% CI [.05, .84]. Note that both the a path, representing the association between
sensory seeking and social orienting, and the b path, representing the association between
social orienting and future social symptomatology when controlling for sensory seeking, are
statistically significant. The c’ path, representing the association between sensory seeking and
future social symptomatology when controlling for social orienting, is non-significant. Thus,
reduced social orienting accounts for the observed association between sensory seeking and
future social symptomatology in Sibs-ASD. Sibs-ASD = Infant siblings of children diagnosed
with autism spectrum disorder. Sibs-TD = Infant siblings of typically developing children. Values
in Figure B are standardized coefficients. * p < .05. ** p < .001. ns = non-significant result.
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 43
Figure A.7. Outcome group differences in early sensory seeking. Sibs-TD = Infant siblings of
typically developing children. Sibs-ASD-No ASD = Infant siblings of children diagnosed with
autism spectrum disorder who do not receive a diagnosis of ASD. Sibs-ASD-Dx ASD = Infant
siblings of children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder who do receive a diagnosis of ASD
at 36 months. Sensory seeking aggregate = average of z-scores for seeking intensity (sum of
seeking ratings across eight novel toys) and seeking inventory (sum of discrete seeking
behaviors across 12 categories) component variables derived from the Sensory Processing
Assessment (Baranek, 1999). * p < .05. ** p < .005. ns = non-significant result. Error bars
represent standard error of the mean.
-0.6
-0.4
-0.2
0.0
0.2
0.4
0.6
0.8
1.0
1.2
1.4
Sibs-TD Sibs-ASD-No ASD Sibs-ASD-Dx ASD
Sensory Seeking Aggregate Score at 18 mos
ns
*
**
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 44
6. Appendices
Table A.1
Sample Characteristics at Time 1
Characteristic
Sibs-ASD
Sibs-TD
Chronological Age In Months
18.24 (.42)
18.34 (.29)
Sex (Male : Female)
8 : 12
10 : 10
Mullen Early Learning
Composite
103.05 (12.69)
110.42 (7.46)*
Mullen Receptive Language
43.40 (6.76)
47.11 (4.76)
Mullen Expressive Language
50.50 (8.69)
54.58 (7.62)
Note. * p < .05. Mullen = Mullen Scales of Early Learning (Mullen, 1995). Early Learning
Composite is a standard score. Receptive and Expressive language scores are T-scores. Sibs-
ASD = Infant siblings of children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Sibs-TD = Infant
siblings of children with typical developmental histories.
Mullen Scales of Early Learning were collected when infants were 12 months old as part of the
larger study of social-emotional development.
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 45
Table A.2
Results of Regression Analyses for Effect of Frontal Asymmetry on Sensory Seeking by Risk
Group
Standardized
Model
B
SE
Beta
t
Significance
1
Constant
-.231
.147
-1.563
.126
Seeking
-.811
.414
-.307
-1.959
.058
2
Constant
-.610
.170
-3.585
.001
Seeking
.610
.482
.230
1.266
.214
Risk Group
.490
.259
.267
1.891
.067
Seeking x
Risk Group
-2.458
.727
-.636
-3.380
.002*
Note.* p value for effect of interest < .05. Models exclude Sib-TD infant who unduly influenced
analyses.
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 46
Table A.3
Results of Regression Analyses for the Effect of Sensory Seeking on Social Orienting by Risk
Group
Standardized
Model
B
SE
Beta
t
Significance
1
Constant
2.279
.119
19.089
.000
Seeking
.570
.128
.590
4.448
.000*
2
Constant
2.219
.265
8.366
.000
Seeking
.136
.361
.140
.376
.709
Risk Group
-.175
.309
-.099
-.566
.575
Seeking x
Risk Group
.642
.389
.557
1.649
.108
Note.* p value for effect of interest < .05. Models exclude Sib-TD infant who unduly influenced
analyses.
SENSORY SEEKING IN INFANT SIBLINGS 47
Table A.4
Results of Regression Analyses for the Effect of Sensory Seeking on Social Symptomatology by
Risk Group
Standardized
Model
B
SE
Beta
t
Significance
1
Constant
2.020
.134
15.032
.000
Seeking
.376
.136
.411
2.777
.008*
2
Constant
1.337
.171
7.824
.000
Seeking
-.229
.193
-.249
-1.186
.243
Risk Group
1.083
.229
.599
4.734
.000
Seeking x
Risk Group
.672
.238
.564
2.822
.008*
Note.* p value for effect of interest < .05. There was no evidence of undue influence on
analyses.
... Less is known about sensory responsivity in infants, particularly when sensory differences emerge. Sensory responsivity differences between infants with high autism likelihood who received a later diagnosis, compared to those who did not, were reported for sensory seeking behavior at 18 months of age (Damiano-Goodwin et al., 2018). Differences in sensory responsivity between infants with high autism likelihood who received a later diagnosis compared to those who did not were more limited across the timespan of 12-18 months of age (Feldman et al., 2021). ...
... Differences in sensory processing at the neurological level are theorized to have cascading effects not only on sensory responsivity at the behavioral level but also on higher level orders of development such as, but not limited to, adaptive, cognitive, social, and linguistic skills (Cascio et al., 2016). In particular, empirical evidence supports that increased sensory seeking in infants with high autism likelihood who are later diagnosed with autism, yields cascading effects on social development at 36 months of age Damiano-Goodwin et al., 2018). There is also empirical support for the cascading effects of hyporesponsivity on later communication skills in infants with high autism likelihood who are later diagnosed with autism (Grzadzinski et al., 2021). ...
... The specific aims of the study were: (a) to identify to what extent sensory responsivity in infancy covaries with later adaptive behavior outcomes, specifically socialization and communication, in toddlers with high autism likelihood; and (b) to explore relations between sensory responsivity and later motor and daily living skills. The primary hypotheses in this study are as follows: (a) elevated hyper-, hyporesponsivity, and sensory seeking scores at 12 months of age will each covary with lower socialization scores at 36 months of age (Baranek et al., 2006;Damiano-Goodwin et al., 2018;Feldman et al., 2020;Liss et al., 2006;Williams et al., 2018;Wolff et al., 2019) and lastly (b) elevated sensory seeking scores at 12 months of age will covary with lower communication scores at 36 months of age (Feldman et al., 2021;Liss et al., 2006;Wolff et al., 2019). While there is some empirical support for associations between sensory responsivity and motor and daily living domains of adaptive behavior among older children on the autism spectrum (Jasmin et al., 2008;Liu, 2013;Tomchek & Dunn, 2007;Travers et al., 2022), we did not have any specific hypotheses related to these domains given no theoretical basis and consider these analyses exploratory. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated the extent to which sensory responsivity in infancy contributes to adaptive behavior development among toddlers at high-familial likelihood for autism. Prospective, longitudinal data were analyzed for 218 children, 58 of whom received an autism diagnosis. Results indicated that sensory profiles at age one year (hyperresponsivity, sensory seeking) were negatively associated with later adaptive behavior, particularly for socialization, at age 3 years regardless of diagnostic status. These results suggest that early differences in sensory responsivity may have downstream developmental consequences related to social development among young children with high-familial likelihood for autism.
... Sensory seeking scores were heightened in infancy for the autistic group and attenuated slightly before increasing by middle childhood for all groups. The current results showing elevated sensory seeking scores in infancy converge with results from Damiano-Goodwin et al. (2018) who reported that sensory seeking is elevated at 18 months in high-likelihood siblings later diagnosed with ASD. Additionally, the attenuation of sensory seeking scores across time was also reported in previous findings indicating that sensory-seeking behaviors decrease in autistic children as they age (Baranek et al., 2019;Wolff et al., 2019). ...
... Hyper sensory responsivity behaviors tend to increase as autistic children age. Given that differences are apparent in late infancy, early sensory assessment could contribute to screening for ASD and provide targets to intervene on prior to potentially longlasting impacts on adaptive behavior outcomes Damiano-Goodwin et al., 2018;Germani et al., 2014). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Empirical evidence regarding the development of sensory responsivity in young children at high likelihood to develop autism spectrum disorder (ASD) remains relatively limited. It is unclear how sensory responsivity behaviors may change over time and impact later developmental outcomes. The goals of this study were to (a) characterize developmental trajectories of sensory responsivity across infancy (∼12 months), toddlerhood (∼24 months), and school age (6–11 years) in children at high and low familial likelihood for ASD; and (b) determine if sensory responsivity in infancy predicts adaptive and cognitive functioning at school age among children with ASD. Methods Generalized linear mixed effects models were used to examine scores from the Sensory Experiences Questionnaire in three groups of children including high‐likelihood children later diagnosed with ASD (HL‐ASD; n = 30), high‐likelihood children without ASD (HL‐Neg; n = 150), and low‐likelihood control children not meeting ASD diagnostic criteria (LL‐Neg; n = 94). Hierarchical linear regression was then used to examine the association between sensory responsivity scores in infancy and functional adaptive and cognitive outcomes at school age for children at high likelihood of ASD. Results Development of sensory responsivity from infancy to later childhood is best estimated by the effects of chronological age and Group for Sensory Seeking and Hypo responsivity and the additional effect of the interaction of Group and chronological age for Total and Hyper responsivity. Early elevated Hypo responsivity and Sensory Seeking scores are negatively associated with later adaptive behavior but not cognitive level. Conclusion Overall, higher degrees of Sensory Seeking and Hypo sensory responsivity are detectable in autistic children's behavioral repertoires by 12 months of age and associate with reduced adaptive functioning in middle childhood. These results point to the potential importance of early detection and treatment implications of early sensory behaviors.
... Most of the previous literature on the role of early sensory processing on later outcomes has used parent-report data; however, some studies considered different measures to examine sensory processing, including behavioral and brain techniques (Ramappa et al., 2022). For example, differences in parentreport sensory responsiveness were associated with brain development in 6-month-old infant siblings of autistic children later diagnosed with ASD (Wolff et al., 2017), and an observational measure of sensory seeking at 18 months of age is associated with neural responses (frontal asymmetry) and later social skills at 3 years (Damiano-Goodwin et al., 2018). A multifaceted approach, using observational, behavioral, and neurophysiological techniques, may yield more informative and complementary results to understand individual differences in sensory processing (Burns et al., 2017). ...
Article
Full-text available
Early sensory responsiveness may produce cascading effects on later development, but the relation between sensory profiles and autistic diagnosis remains unclear. In a longitudinal sample of toddlers at elevated likelihood for autism, we aimed to characterize sensory subgroups and their association with clinical outcomes later on. Three sensory subgroups were described and early sensory sensitivity plays a significant role in later development and diagnosis. This study supported the importance of examining different levels of sensory patterns to dissect the phenotypic heterogeneity in sensory processing. As sensory differences are associated with later developmental outcomes, these results may be critical when designing intervention needs and support for children at increased likelihood for neurodevelopmental disorders.
... Another study found differences in FAS in children who show atypical sensory-seeking behaviors (Damiano-Goodwin et al., 2018), providing further evidence of the association between frontal asymmetry and sensory processing in children. ...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction Parents often use sensory stimulation during early‐life interactions with infants. These interactions, including gazing, rocking, or singing, scaffold child development. Previous studies have examined infant neural processing during highly controlled sensory stimulus presentation paradigms. Objective In this study, we investigated infant behavioral and neural responsiveness during a mother–child social interaction during which the mother provided infant stimulation with a progressive increase in the number of sensory modalities. Methods We prospectively collected and analyzed video‐coded behavioral interactions and electroencephalogram (EEG) frontal asymmetry (FAS) from infants (n = 60) at 2–4 months born at ≥ 34 weeks gestation. As the number of sensory modalities progressively increased during the interaction, infant behaviors of emotional connection in facial expressiveness, sensitivity to mother, and vocal communication increased significantly. Conversely, infant FAS for the entire cohort did not change significantly. However, when we accounted for infant irritability, both video‐coded behaviors and EEG FAS markers of infant responsiveness increased across the interaction in the non‐irritable infants. The non‐irritable infants (49%) demonstrated positive FAS, indicating readiness to engage with, rather than to withdraw from, multisensory but not unisensory interactions with their mothers. Results These results suggest that multisensory input from mothers is associated with greater infant neural approach state and highlight the importance of infant behavioral state during neural measures of infant responsiveness.
... In humans, altered sensory processing encompasses sensory hyperactivity/ hypoactivity and sensory seeking, and is a core symptom of an ASD diagnosis (Marco et al., 2011;Kirby et al., 2017;Robertson and Baron-Cohen, 2017;Damiano-Goodwin et al., 2018). In SYNGAP1 syndrome specifically, sensory-seeking behaviors include an affinity for contact with flowing water and/or perpetual motion (Wright et al., 2022). ...
Preprint
Full-text available
Background and Aims SYNGAP1 disorder is a prevalent genetic form of Autism Spectrum Disorder and Intellectual Disability (ASD/ID) and is caused by de novo or inherited mutations in one copy of the SYNGAP1 gene. In addition to ASD/ID, SYNGAP1 disorder is associated with comorbid symptoms including treatment-resistant-epilepsy, sleep disturbances, and gastrointestinal distress. Mechanistic links between these diverse symptoms and SYNGAP1 variants remain obscure, therefore, our goal was to generate a zebrafish model in which this range of symptoms can be studied. Methods We used CRISPR/Cas9 to introduce frameshift mutations in the syngap1a and syngap1b zebrafish duplicates ( syngap1ab ) and validated these stable models for Syngap1 loss-of-function. Because SYNGAP1 is extensively spliced, we mapped splice variants to the two zebrafish syngap1a and b genes and identified mammalian-like isoforms. We then quantified locomotory behaviors in zebrafish syngap1ab larvae under three conditions that normally evoke different arousal states in wild type larvae: aversive, high-arousal acoustic, medium-arousal dark, and low-arousal light stimuli. Results We show that CRISPR/Cas9 indels in zebrafish syngap1a and syngap1b produced loss-of-function alleles at RNA and protein levels. Our analyses of zebrafish Syngap1 isoforms showed that, as in mammals, zebrafish Syngap1 N- and C-termini are extensively spliced. We identified a zebrafish syngap1 α1-like variant that maps exclusively to the syngap1b gene. Quantifying locomotor behaviors showed that syngap1ab larvae are hyperactive compared to wild type but to differing degrees depending on the stimulus. Hyperactivity was most pronounced in low arousal settings, with overall movement increasing with the number of mutant syngap1 alleles. Conclusions Our data support mutations in zebrafish syngap1ab as causal for hyperactivity associated with elevated arousal that is especially pronounced in low-arousal environments.
... For example, atypical sensory processing might interfere with social and language learning. Distracting sensory inputs can lead autistic people to exhibit atypical brain activity in social tasks (Green et al., 2018), and altered sensory processing predicts later social and language outcomes in ASD (Baranek et al., 2018;Damiano-Goodwin et al., 2018;Kolesnik et al., 2019). Furthermore, one might expect that autistic children's experiences of sensory distress could lead them to develop anxious vigilance and fears surrounding the possibility these uncomfortable stimuli might recur (Verhulst et al., 2022). ...
Article
Full-text available
Prior studies suggest that habituation of sensory responses is reduced in autism and that diminished habituation could be related to atypical autistic sensory experiences, for example, by causing brain responses to aversive stimuli to remain strong over time instead of being suppressed. While many prior studies exploring habituation in autism have repeatedly presented identical stimuli, other studies suggest group differences can still be observed in habituation to intermittent stimuli. The present study explored habituation of electrophysiological responses to auditory complex tones of varying intensities (50-80 dB SPL), presented passively in an interleaved manner, in a well-characterized sample of 127 autistic (MDQ = 65.41, SD = 20.54) and 79 typically developing (MDQ = 106.02, SD = 11.50) children between 2 and 5 years old. Habituation was quantified as changes in the amplitudes of single-trial responses to tones of each intensity over the course of the experiment. Habituation of the auditory N2 response was substantially reduced in autistic participants as compared to typically developing controls, although diagnostic groups did not clearly differ in habituation of the P1 response. Interestingly, the P1 habituated less to loud 80 dB sounds than softer sounds, whereas the N2 habituated less to soft 50 dB sounds than louder sounds. No associations were found between electrophysiological habituation and cognitive ability or participants' caregiver-reported sound tolerance (Sensory Profile Hyperacusis Index). The results present study results extend prior research suggesting habituation of certain sensory responses is reduced in autism; however, they also suggest that habituation differences observed using this study's paradigm might not be a primary driver of autistic participants' real-world sound intolerance.
... Although some studies have begun to demonstrate the predictive utility of sensory reactivity in autistic children and other populations such as infants at elevated likelihood to develop autism (e.g. [182][183][184][185][186][187]), these studies have largely used supra-modal response pattern scores; therefore, additional large-scale, longitudinal studies are necessary to determine which single-modality sensory subconstructs (or combinations thereof ) can be utilized as clinically-relevant predictors of core and frequently co-occurring features of autism. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Differences in responding to sensory stimuli, including sensory hyperreactivity (HYPER), hyporeactivity (HYPO), and sensory seeking (SEEK) have been observed in autistic individuals across sensory modalities, but few studies have examined the structure of these "supra-modal" traits in the autistic population. Methods: Leveraging a combined sample of 3868 autistic youth drawn from 12 distinct data sources (ages 3-18 years and representing the full range of cognitive ability), the current study used modern psychometric and meta-analytic techniques to interrogate the latent structure and correlates of caregiver-reported HYPER, HYPO, and SEEK within and across sensory modalities. Bifactor statistical indices were used to both evaluate the strength of a "general response pattern" factor for each supra-modal construct and determine the added value of "modality-specific response pattern" scores (e.g., Visual HYPER). Bayesian random-effects integrative data analysis models were used to examine the clinical and demographic correlates of all interpretable HYPER, HYPO, and SEEK (sub)constructs. Results: All modality-specific HYPER subconstructs could be reliably and validly measured, whereas certain modality-specific HYPO and SEEK subconstructs were psychometrically inadequate when measured using existing items. Bifactor analyses supported the validity of a supra-modal HYPER construct (ωH = .800) but not a supra-modal HYPO construct (ωH = .653), and supra-modal SEEK models suggested a more limited version of the construct that excluded some sensory modalities (ωH = .800; 4/7 modalities). Modality-specific subscales demonstrated significant added value for all response patterns. Meta-analytic correlations varied by construct, although sensory features tended to correlate most with other domains of core autism features and co-occurring psychiatric symptoms (with general HYPER and speech HYPO demonstrating the largest numbers of practically significant correlations). Limitations: Conclusions may not be generalizable beyond the specific pool of items used in the current study, which was limited to caregiver report of observable behaviors and excluded multisensory items that reflect many "real-world" sensory experiences. Conclusion: Of the three sensory response patterns, only HYPER demonstrated sufficient evidence for valid interpretation at the supra-modal level, whereas supra-modal HYPO/SEEK constructs demonstrated substantial psychometric limitations. For clinicians and researchers seeking to characterize sensory reactivity in autism, modality-specific response pattern scores may represent viable alternatives that overcome many of these limitations.
... In young autistic children, early sensory dysregulation may hinder the development of joint attention, language, and social play (Baranek et al., 2013;Miller Kuhaneck & Britner, 2013). A growing body of research suggests atypical responses to sensory input may precede differences in social interaction and communication, wherein early neural dysfunction produces cascading effects on social development (Baranek et al., 2018;Damiano-Goodwin et al., 2018). Infants who go on to develop autism show increased sensory seeking compared to those who do not, leading to reduced social orienting and increased social symptom severity (Baranek et al., 2018). ...
Article
Auditory processing differences, including hyper- or hyposensitivity to sound, aversions to sound, and difficulty listening under noisy, real-world conditions, are commonly reported in autistic individuals. However, the developmental course and functional impact of these auditory processing differences are unclear. In this study, we investigate the prevalence, developmental trajectory, and functional impact of auditory processing differences in autistic children throughout childhood using a longitudinal study design. Auditory processing differences were measured using the Short Sensory Profile, a caregiver questionnaire, in addition to adaptive behaviors and disruptive/concerning behaviors at 3, 6, and 9 years of age. Our results showed that auditory processing differences were reported in greater than 70% of the autistic children in our sample at all three timepoints, maintained a high prevalence through 9 years of age, and were associated with increased disruptive/concerning behaviors and difficulty with adaptive behaviors. Furthermore, in our sample of children, auditory processing differences at age 3 years predicted disruptive/concerning behaviors and difficulty with adaptive behaviors at age 9 years. These findings warrant further investigations of the potential benefit of incorporating measures of auditory processing during routine clinical evaluations as well as interventions targeting auditory processing differences in autistic children.
Article
The current study examined whether autistic children’s perception of others’ pain could be modulated by their first-hand pain sensitivity and theory of mind (ToM). We measured the first-hand pain sensitivity, the rating of others’ pain intensity, and the performance in the ToM tasks in 43 5- to 8-year-old autistic and 30 neurotypical children. Our results revealed hyposensitivity to first-hand pain, underestimation of others’ pain intensity, as well as difficulties in the False Belief subtasks of ToM in autistic children. Furthermore, we detected an interaction between children’s first-hand pain sensitivity and ToM in predicting their perception of others’ pain. To be specific, for autistic and NT children with normal or hyper-sensitivity to first-hand pain, better performance on ToM predicted higher ratings for others’ pain intensity; while for autistic and NT children with hyposensitivity to first-hand pain, ToM did not predict ratings for others’ pain intensity. Our study contributes to the understanding of pain perception in young children and provides implications for clinical practices to improve social understanding in autistic children.
Article
Importance Atypical sensory processing is challenging for children and families, yet there is limited understanding of its associated risk factors. Objective To determine the association between early-life digital media exposure and sensory processing outcomes among toddlers. Design, Setting, and Participants This multicenter US study used data that were analyzed from the National Children’s Study (NCS), a cohort study of environmental influences on child health and development, with enrollment from 2011 to 2014. Data analysis was performed in 2023. The study included children enrolled in the NCS at birth whose caregivers completed reports of digital media exposure and sensory processing. Exposures Children’s viewing of television or video at 12 months (yes or no), 18 months, and 24 months of age (hours per day). Main Outcomes and measures Sensory processing was reported at approximately 33 months of age on the Infant/Toddler Sensory Profile. Quadrant scores (low registration, sensation seeking, sensory sensitivity, and sensation avoiding) were categorized into groups representing typical, high, and low sensory-related behaviors, and multinomial regression analyses were performed. Results A total of 1471 children (50% male) were included. Screen exposure at 12 months of age was associated with a 2-fold increased odds of being in the high category of low registration (odds ratio [OR], 2.05; 95% CI, 1.31-3.20), while the odds of being in the low category instead of the typical category decreased for sensation seeking (OR, 0.55; 95% CI, 0.35-0.87), sensation avoiding (OR, 0.69; 95% CI, 0.50-0.94), and low registration (OR, 0.64; 95% CI, 0.44-0.92). At 18 months of age, greater screen exposure was associated with increased risk of high sensation avoiding (OR, 1.23; 95% CI, 1.03-1.46) and low registration (OR, 1.23; 95% CI, 1.04-1.44). At 24 months of age, greater screen exposure was associated with increased risk of high sensation seeking (OR, 1.20; 95% CI, 1.02-1.42), sensory sensitivity (OR, 1.25; 95% CI, 1.05-1.49), and sensation avoiding (OR, 1.21; 95% CI, 1.03-1.42). Conclusions and Relevance In this cohort study, early-life digital media exposure was associated with atypical sensory processing outcomes in multiple domains. These findings suggest that digital media exposure might be a potential risk factor for the development of atypical sensory profiles. Further research is needed to understand the relationship between screen time and specific sensory-related developmental and behavioral outcomes, and whether minimizing early-life exposure can improve subsequent sensory-related outcomes.
Article
Full-text available
Altered patterns of sensory responsiveness are a frequently reported feature of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Younger siblings of individuals with ASD are at a greatly elevated risk of a future diagnosis of ASD, but little is known about the neural basis of sensory responsiveness patterns in this population. Younger siblings (n = 20) of children diagnosed with ASD participated in resting electroencephalography (EEG) at an age of 18 months. Data on toddlers' sensory responsiveness were obtained using the Sensory Experiences Questionnaire. Correlations were present between hyporesponsiveness and patterns of oscillatory power, functional connectivity, and signal complexity. Our findings suggest that neural signal features hold promise for facilitating early identification and targeted remediation in young children at risk for ASD.
Article
Full-text available
Few behavioral indices of risk for autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are present before 12 months, and potential biomarkers remain largely unexamined. This prospective study of infant siblings of children with ASD (n = 16) and low-risk comparison infants (n = 15) examined group differences in event-related potentials (ERPs) indexing processing of facial positive affect (N290/P400, Nc) at 9 months and their relation to joint attention at 15 months. Group differences were most pronounced for subtle facial expressions, in that the low-risk group exhibited relatively longer processing (P400 latency) and greater attention resource allocation (Nc amplitude). Exploratory analyses found associations between ERP responses and later joint attention, suggesting that attention to positive affect cues may support the development of other social competencies.
Article
Full-text available
Speech perception is an inherently multisensory process. When having a face-to-face conversation, a listener not only hears what a speaker is saying, but also sees the articulatory gestures that accompany those sounds. Speech signals in visual and auditory modalities provide complementary information to the listener (Kavanagh and Mattingly, 1974), and when both are perceived in unison, behavioral gains in in speech perception are observed (Sumby and Pollack, 1954). Notably, this benefit is accentuated when speech is perceived in a noisy environment (Sumby and Pollack, 1954). To achieve a behavioral gain from multisensory processing of speech, however, the auditory and visual signals must be perceptually bound into a single, unified percept. The most commonly cited effect that demonstrates perceptual binding in audiovisual speech perception is the McGurk effect (McGurk and MacDonald, 1976), where a listener hears a speaker utter the syllable “ba,” and sees the speaker utter the syllable “ga.” When these two speech signals are perceptually bound, the listener perceives the speaker as having said “da” or “tha,” syllables that are not contained in either of the unisensory signals, resulting in a perceptual binding, or integration, of the speech signals (Calvert and Thesen, 2004).
Article
Frontal electroencephalographic (EEG) alpha asymmetry is widely researched in studies of emotion, motivation, and psychopathology, yet it is a metric that has been quantified and analyzed using diverse procedures, and diversity in procedures muddles cross-study interpretation. The aim of this article is to provide an updated tutorial for EEG alpha asymmetry recording, processing, analysis, and interpretation, with an eye towards improving consistency of results across studies. First, a brief background in alpha asymmetry findings is provided. Then, some guidelines for recording, processing, and analyzing alpha asymmetry are presented with an emphasis on the creation of asymmetry scores, referencing choices, and artifact removal. Processing steps are explained in detail, and references to MATLAB-based toolboxes that are helpful for creating and investigating alpha asymmetry are noted. Then, conceptual challenges and interpretative issues are reviewed, including a discussion of alpha asymmetry as a mediator/moderator of emotion and psychopathology. Finally, the effects of two automated component-based artifact correction algorithms-MARA and ADJUST-on frontal alpha asymmetry are evaluated.
Article
Heightened interest in sensory function in persons with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) presents an unprecedented opportunity for impactful, interdisciplinary work between neuroscientists and clinical practitioners for whom sensory processing is a focus. In spite of this promise, and a number of overlapping perspectives on sensory function in persons with ASD, neuroscientists and clinical practitioners are faced with significant practical barriers to transcending disciplinary silos. These barriers include divergent goals, values, and approaches that shape each discipline, as well as different lexical conventions. This commentary is itself an interdisciplinary effort to describe the shared perspectives, and to conceptualize a framework that may guide future investigation in this area. We summarize progress to date and issue a call for clinical practitioners and neuroscientists to expand cross-disciplinary dialogue and to capitalize on the complementary strengths of each field to unveil the links between neural and behavioral manifestations of sensory differences in persons with ASD. Joining forces to face these challenges in a truly interdisciplinary way will lead to more clinically informed neuroscientific investigation of sensory function, and better translation of those findings to clinical practice. Likewise, a more coordinated effort may shed light not only on how current approaches to treating sensory processing differences affect brain and behavioral responses to sensory stimuli in individuals with ASD, but also on whether such approaches translate to gains in broader characteristics associated with ASD. It is our hope that such interdisciplinary undertakings will ultimately converge to improve assessment and interventions for persons with ASD. Autism Res 2016,. © 2016 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
Article
This study used a prospective longitudinal design to examine the early developmental pathways that underlie language growth in infants at high risk ( n = 50) and low risk ( n = 34) for autism spectrum disorder in the first 18 months of life. While motor imitation and responding to joint attention (RJA) have both been found to predict expressive language in children with autism spectrum disorder and those with typical development, the longitudinal relation between these capacities has not yet been identified. As hypothesized, results revealed that 15-month RJA mediated the association between 12-month motor imitation and 18-month expressive vocabulary, even after controlling for earlier levels of RJA and vocabulary. These results provide new information about the developmental sequencing of skills relevant to language growth that may inform future intervention efforts for children at risk for language delay or other developmental challenges.
Article
Sensory interests, repetitions, and seeking behaviors (SIRS) are common among children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and other developmental disabilities (DD) and involve unusual actions that intensify or reinforce a sensory experience. Researchers and practitioners typically use parent-report measures or informal clinical observations to understand the presence and nature of SIRS. In this study, we used a scoring supplement to the Sensory Processing Assessment for Young Children, an observational measure, to characterize SIRS across three groups of children-those with ASD (n = 40), DD (n = 37), and typical development (n = 39). Group differences were identified in frequency and intensity of overall SIRS, complexity of SIRS, and incidence of particular types of SIRS (i.e., posturing, sighting, proprioceptive seeking, spinning). Facial affect was also explored and found to be primarily neutral during engagement in SIRS across groups. Implications for practice and future research are discussed. Copyright © 2015 by the American Occupational Therapy Association, Inc.