ArticlePDF Available

Implicit conscientiousness predicts academic performance

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Across two studies, we provide the first evidence of a positive causal relationship between implicit con-scientiousness and academic performance. Results showed how both implicit and explicit conscientious-ness predicted the number of examinations that students successfully passed in the semester that followed their participation in the study. The implicit measure of conscientiousness is shown to be incre-mentally valid when compared to two different explicit measures. Participants' gender moderated the effect of implicit, but not explicit, conscientiousness. Lastly, we found that motivating students to manage their impression resulted in an increased self-reported conscientiousness which was not reflected in the implicit measure, nor did this manipulation affect the predictive validity of implicit and explicit consci-entiousness. The main theoretical and practical implications of these findings are discussed.
Content may be subject to copyright.
Implicit conscientiousness predicts academic performance
Michelangelo Vianello
*
, Egidio Robusto, Pasquale Anselmi
Department of General Psychology, University of Padova, v. Venezia 8 35131, Padova, Italy
article info
Article history:
Received 17 June 2009
Received in revised form 10 November 2009
Accepted 12 November 2009
Available online xxxx
Keywords:
IAT
Implicit conscientiousness
Explicit conscientiousness
Academic performance
Gender differences
Faking
abstract
Across two studies, we provide the first evidence of a positive causal relationship between implicit con-
scientiousness and academic performance. Results showed how both implicit and explicit conscientious-
ness predicted the number of examinations that students successfully passed in the semester that
followed their participation in the study. The implicit measure of conscientiousness is shown to be incre-
mentally valid when compared to two different explicit measures. Participants’ gender moderated the
effect of implicit, but not explicit, conscientiousness. Lastly, we found that motivating students to manage
their impression resulted in an increased self-reported conscientiousness which was not reflected in the
implicit measure, nor did this manipulation affect the predictive validity of implicit and explicit consci-
entiousness. The main theoretical and practical implications of these findings are discussed.
Ó2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
1. Introduction
An important topic in the evaluation of personality concerns
participants’ ability to convey an ‘‘augmented” image of their
selves. Any time social desirability and impression management
strategies are an issue, self-reported measures might be inade-
quate because they might lead to wrong decisions (e.g., to hire a
rather negligent person that is quite able to manage how he is per-
ceived). On the other hand, self-reported measures of conscien-
tiousness have been found to be extremely useful to predict
scholastic and job performance. Since the famous meta-analysis
by Barrick and Mount (1991), personality measures have increas-
ingly and effectively been used in scholastic and organizational
contexts. More recent work has demonstrated that conscientious-
ness is the best personality predictor of job performance, training
performance and academic achievements, and that it has incre-
mental validity beyond cognitive ability (Di Fabio & Busoni,
2007; Higgins, Peterson, Pihl, & Lee, 2007; Schmidt & Hunter,
1998, 2004). Chamorro-Premuzic and Furnham (2004) explained
the relationship between conscientiousness and academic perfor-
mance (typically ranging from r=.2tor= .5) in terms of the per-
sisting, self-disciplined, and achievement-oriented nature of
conscientious students.
Yet, Komar, Brown, Komar, and Robie (2008) showed that faking
has strong effects on the criterion-related validity of conscientious-
ness, and Holden (2008) suggested that the influence of faking
might have been seriously underestimated.
On the contrary, the predictive validity of implicit measures is
not influenced by social desirability (Greenwald, Poehlman, Uhl-
mann, & Banaji, 2009), as implicit measures are generally more
resistant to faking (see, e.g., Blair, 2002), especially when respon-
dents are new to the technique (Fiedler & Blümke, 2005) and when
they are not instructed on how to fake it (Do Yeong, 2003). As far as
conscientiousness is concerned, Steffens (2004) showed that in-
structed participants were very well able to fake the NEO-Five Fac-
tor Inventory (NEO-FFI, Borkenau & Ostendorf, 1993) but not the
implicit measure of conscientiousness. In this study, the authors
measured implicit conscientiousness by means of an Implicit Asso-
ciation Test (IAT, Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwartz, 1998), which is
the most widely used implicit technique and is applied in very dif-
ferent domains (see Lane, Banaji, Nosek, & Greenwald, 2007 for a
review). Since it was first presented in 1998, the IAT was followed
by many other techniques (e.g. Bar-Anan, Nosek, & Vianello, 2009;
Payne, Cheng, Govorun, & Stewart, 2005), but so far, none of them
showed equally good psychometric properties (Greenwald et al.,
2002; Greenwald et al., 2009; Robusto, Cristante, & Vianello,
2008). In personality research, implicit techniques have been suc-
cessfully employed to measure many constructs, among which
the Big-Five personality traits (e.g. Boldero, Rawlings, & Haslam,
2007; Grumm & von Collani, 2007; Steffens, 2004).
Implicit measures have also been used to investigate scholastic
performance. Nosek, Banaji, and Greenwald (2002) demonstrated
that the attitude toward mathematics predicts unique variance of
the Scientific Aptitude Test (SAT). Kiefer and Sekaquaptewa
(2007) found that the interaction between gender identity and im-
plicit (but not explicit) gender stereotypes predicts both the final
grade and participants’ career goals. The gender-science stereotype
0191-8869/$ - see front matter Ó2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.019
*Corresponding author. Tel.: +39 049 827 6296; fax: +39 049 827 6600.
E-mail address: michelangelo.vianello@unipd.it (M. Vianello).
Personality and Individual Differences xxx (2009) xxx–xxx
Contents lists available at ScienceDirect
Personality and Individual Differences
journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid
ARTICLE IN PRESS
Please cite this article in press as: Vianello, M., et al. Implicit conscientiousness predicts academic performance. Personality and Individual Differences
(2009), doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.019
(according to which men are typically associated with science and
women with arts) also predicts national gender differences in
mathematics achievement (Nosek et al., 2009).
Inter-individual differences in implicit constructs are typically a
valid predictor of behavior. In a recent meta-analysis conducted on
103 different studies (K= 140 independent samples, N= 10967),
Greenwald et al. (2009) observed a moderate significant correla-
tion between IAT-measured implicit constructs and behavior, and
the authors found that this relationship was independent from so-
cial sensitivity (subjects’ eagerness to be perceived positively). IAT-
measured implicit constructs enhance our capability to predict rel-
evant behavior, and this is especially the case when studying so-
cially sensitive research topics or when impression management
is an issue (e.g. in admission tests or job selection).
In this paper we will investigate whether an implicit measure of
conscientiousness (implicit conscientiousness) might represent a
valid alternative to traditional measures when impression man-
agement is an issue. Hence, we hypothesize that it will effectively
predict academic performance even when participants are moti-
vated to fake.
2. First study
This study tests the hypothesis (H1) that implicit conscientious-
ness predicts academic performance at least as much as explicit
conscientiousness.
2.1. Method
2.1.1. Participants and procedure
Fifty psychology students at the University of Padua partici-
pated in the study without any form of financial reward. Data were
collected in individual cubicles at the beginning of the first semes-
ter. Their mean age was 21.77 (SD = 1.03). Although precise data
concerning participants’ gender was not collected, the vast major-
ity were male. After they signed the informed consent, they were
told to read the instructions on the screen. A computerized version
of the explicit conscientiousness scales (BFO and BFQ), with the
items in random order, preceded or followed a Conscientious-
ness-IAT in counterbalanced order. Then, participants indicated
their year of enrollment, the number of examinations successfully
passed, their mean examination mark and were finally thanked
and debriefed.
2.1.2. Measures
2.1.2.1. Independent variables (IV). The study used an IAT-based im-
plicit measure of conscientiousness, and the conscientiousness
scales of the Big Five Observer (BFO, Caprara, Barbaranelli, & Borg-
ogni, 1994) and of the Big Five Questionnaire (BFQ, Caprara, Bar-
baranelli, & Borgogni, 2000) as explicit measures. We used two
explicit measures of conscientiousness because the BFO and the
IAT share the same items and consequently they are more compa-
rable to each other than the IAT and the BFQ. Indeed, the BFO con-
scientiousness scale consists of eight pairs of bipolar adjectives.
One adjective of each pair concerns a conscientiousness element;
the other one concerns its direct antonym. The position, between
the pair of adjectives, that best describes the respondent is given
on a 7 point scale. The BFQ conscientiousness scale has also been
standardized for the Italian population, and it consists of 24 items
evaluated on a scale of 1 (Absolutely untrue for me) to 5 (Abso-
lutely true for me). Example of items in the BFQ scales are: ‘‘Mess-
iness annoys me a lot” and ‘‘I rarely give up”. Reliability indexes are
provided in Table 1. The IAT used the category labels I, Others, Con-
scientious, Not Conscientious. Eight pronouns related to the nom-
inal categories ‘‘I/Others” were used as stimuli to represent them.
The eight bipolar pairs of adjectives of the BFO conscientiousness
scale were used to represent the nominal categories ‘‘Conscien-
tious/Not Conscientious”. A list of all stimuli is available from the
first author. Participants were tested individually in a laboratory.
The seven-block IAT employed (Greenwald, Nosek, & Banaji,
2003) presented stimuli related to the target dimension (I vs. Oth-
ers) and the attribute dimension (Conscientious vs. Not Conscien-
tious) in the center of the computer screen in an alternating
fashion. Blocks 1, 2 and 5 provided practice trials and blocks 3, 4,
6 and 7 provided critical trials. Participants were asked to catego-
rize stimuli as belonging to the concepts displayed at the top-left
or top-right screen corner by pressing, as quickly and accurately
as possible, the response key ‘‘A” or ‘‘L”, respectively. The order
of the test blocks was counterbalanced. Past research successfully
employed the IAT for measuring some or all the Big Five traits,
showing in many ways that IATs can validly assess the semantic
aspect of trait self-concepts. Implicit conscientiousness has been
found to be different from explicit conscientiousness (although
they are moderately correlated) and implicit personality traits
were shown to effectively predict spontaneous behavior (Schnabel,
Asendorpf, & Greenwald 2008; Steffens & Schulze König, 2006).
2.1.2.2. Dependent variables (DV). Academic performance was mea-
sured with two self-reported indexes of prior academic achieve-
ment, i.e. participants’ mean examination mark (ranging from 18
to 30) and the number of exams successfully passed in the past
year (ranging from 0 to 8).
2.2. Results
The IAT D effect (Greenwald et al., 2003) and the mean scores
on the BFO and BFQ scales were computed as independent vari-
ables (IV) for each participant. All variables can be considered nor-
mally distributed (kurtosis and skewness indexes < 2 SE). Table 1
shows summary statistics, internal consistencies, and Pearson cor-
relations of the implicit conscientiousness, explicit conscientious-
ness and academic performance measures. Our participants
Table 1
Study 1. Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations between study variables.
M(SD)
a
(n. items) Correlations
BFO BFQ Mean Mark Number of Exams
IAT D .39 (.32) .73 (24) .13 .21 .24 .29
*
BFO 4.73 (.71) .73 (8) .72
**
.10 .14
BFQ 3.31 (.37) .76 (24) .05 .03
Mean examination mark 25.97 (1.68) .71
**
Number of exams successfully passed 5.01 (1.45)
The IAT-Number of Exams correlation is greater than the BFQ-Number of Exams correlation (p< .05) whereas the IAT-Number of Exams and the BFO-Number of Exams
correlations are not different (test performed with the Fisher’s rto z’ transformation).
*
p< .05.
**
p< .01.
2M. Vianello et al. / Personality and Individual Differences xxx (2009) xxx–xxx
ARTICLE IN PRESS
Please cite this article in press as: Vianello, M., et al. Implicit conscientiousness predicts academic performance. Personality and Individual Differences
(2009), doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.019
show, on average, a moderate implicit (M
IAT
= .39) and explicit con-
scientiousness (M
BFO
= 4.73 on 7; M
BFQ
= 3.31 on 5).
Importantly, we found that implicit conscientiousness posi-
tively correlates with the number of exams successfully passed
(r= .29, p= .045). Explicit measures of conscientiousness correlate
with each other (r= .72, p< .001), but there is no relationship be-
tween them and participants’ academic performance.
2.3. Discussion
This study provides a first demonstration that implicit consci-
entiousness is positively related to participants’ number of exams
successfully passed each year. These results suggest that implicit
conscientiousness is at least as predictive of academic performance
as explicit conscientiousness. However, this study also presents
important drawbacks. First, there is extensive literature that links
explicit conscientiousness and academic performance. We did ex-
pect a significant relationship between implicit conscientiousness
and academic performance but we also expected an analogous
relationship between explicit conscientiousness and academic per-
formance, and we have not been able to test for eventual modera-
tors that might have obscured this relationship (such as
participants’ willingness to manage their impression). We col-
lected data in a laboratory inside our faculty, and such a setting
might have pushed students to manage their displayed conscien-
tiousness toward a socially desirable level, thus distorting explicit
measures and jeopardizing their predictive validity.
Second, we cannot infer any causality in the relationship we ob-
served. It may be true that participants high in implicit conscien-
tiousness achieve better performance than those that are low in
implicit conscientiousness, but the opposite might also be true. A
prospective design in which academic performance is measured
after implicit conscientiousness and explicit conscientiousness
might clear up this point.
Third, we employed a self-reported measure of participants’
academic performance, which suffers from the same problems that
affect explicit measures. For instance, participants might also have
faked their self-reported academic performance.
We will address all these issues in the following study, in which
we further analyze the relationship between implicit conscien-
tiousness and explicit conscientiousness and academic perfor-
mance adopting a prospective experimental design and direct
measures of academic performance. Furthermore, in the following
study we will also test whether gender moderates the relationship
between explicit conscientiousness and academic performance,
between implicit conscientiousness and academic performance,
or both (see e.g. Schmitt, Realo, Voracek, & Allik, 2008; Weathing-
ton & Moldenhauer, 2008).
3. Second study
This study aims at confirming results from study 1 (H1) and also
test the hypotheses that implicit conscientiousness is less suscep-
tible to faking attempts (H2) and that implicit conscientiousness
accounts for unique variance in the prediction of academic perfor-
mance (H3).
3.1. Method
3.1.1. Participants and procedure
Seventy-one psychology students at the University of Padua
participated in the study with no financial reward. Their mean
age was 23.66 (SD = 2.7), and 39 were male. After they provided in-
formed consent, they were randomly assigned either to the exper-
imental or to the control group. In this study, we experimentally
manipulated participants’ willingness to fake the tests. Instructions
given in the former group told participants that the aim of the
study was to recruit students that would have been offered a
well-paid job at the faculty. Then, participants in the experimental
group (Faking) were asked how interested they would have been in
this job opportunity on a scale from 0 (not at all interested) to 10
(strongly interested). Participants in the control group were not gi-
ven any specific information about the aim of the study. Data were
collected in individual cubicles at the beginning of the first
semester.
3.1.2. Measures
We used the same implicit and explicit measures of conscien-
tiousness from study 1. Academic performance is the actual num-
ber of examinations successfully passed by participants in the
eight months that followed their participation in the study and
their actual mean mark in those exams. This information, which
is public in Italy, was downloaded from a public area in the univer-
sity’s website.
3.2. Results
Before the analyses, we checked for normality and outliers. All
variables can be considered normally distributed (kurtosis and
skewness indexes < 2 SE).
3.2.1. Manipulation check
Explicit measures of conscientiousness were correlated to par-
ticipants’ interest in the job ostensibly offered in the faking condi-
tion (r
BFO
= .36, p= .02; r
BFQ
= .31; p= .045). Hence, we can state
that our manipulation worked as expected.
3.2.2. Descriptive statistics
Table 2 shows summary statistics, internal consistencies and
Pearson correlations of implicit conscientiousness, explicit consci-
entiousness and academic performance measures. Participants, on
average, are moderately conscientious at the explicit measure
(M
BFO
= 4.78 on 7; M
BFQ
= 3.44 on 5), and lightly conscientious at
the implicit measure (M
IAT
=.45, SD = .26). Implicit measures of con-
scientiousness do not differ for men and women, whereas women
reported a higher level of explicit conscientiousness than men did
(M
BFOmen
= 4.58, M
BFOwomen
= 5.03, t
(69)
= 2.04, p= .04; M
BFQmen
=
3.33, M
BFQwomen
= 3.57, t
(69)
= 1.87, p= .06). On average, partici-
pants successfully passed 2.94 exams (SD = 1.82) in the eight
months that followed their participation in the study, with a mean
mark of 25.64 out of 30 (SD = 2.71). No gender differences were
found on these variables. Explicit conscientiousness is higher in
the Faking group (Control Group: M
BFO
= 4.39, M
BFQ
= 3.29; Faking
Group: M
BFO
= 5.04, M
BFQ
= 3.54; t
BFO
= 3.13, df = 70; p= .003;
t
BFQ
= 1.93, df = 70; p= .06) whereas implicit conscientiousness is
equal across groups (Control Group: M
IAT
= .42; Faking Group:
M
IAT
= .48; t
IAT
= .90, df = 70; p= .37). Hence, we can accept H2.
3.2.3. Hypotheses testing
As we can see from Table 2, explicit and implicit measures of
conscientiousness are significantly correlated to participants’ num-
ber of successful exams and not to their mean mark. Hence, impli-
cit conscientiousness facilitated students in passing more exams
without affecting their marks (H1). In order to test for moderating
effects of group membership (Faking vs. Control) and gender (both
were dummy coded) we followed recommendations by Baron and
Kenny (1986). Before the analyses, variables were centered around
their mean (hence we subtracted the mean of the variable from
any individual score) to allow an adequate interpretation of even-
tual interaction effects and to avoid the risk of multicollinearity
(Aiken & West, 1991). Then, each IV was tested separately
M. Vianello et al. / Personality and Individual Differences xxx (2009) xxx–xxx 3
ARTICLE IN PRESS
Please cite this article in press as: Vianello, M., et al. Implicit conscientiousness predicts academic performance. Personality and Individual Differences
(2009), doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.019
regressing at the first step the IV on the DV, then adding the cate-
gorical moderator variable (MV) as an IV and finally adding the
interaction variable (the product term between the IV and the
MV). At each step, we tested whether the change in R
2
from the
previous step was different from zero. While a
D
R
2
0 at the first
and second step indicates a significant main effect of the indepen-
dent or moderator variables, a
D
R
2
0 in the third step suggests
that the moderation effect is different from zero. Although our
manipulation significantly increased participants’ explicit consci-
entiousness but not implicit conscientiousness, the relationship
between these predictors and participants’ academic performance
is not moderated by our manipulation (faking strategies). On the
contrary, the moderation effect of gender on the relationship be-
tween the number of exams successfully passed and implicit con-
scientiousness (but not explicit conscientiousness) tends to be
significant (
D
R
2
= .05, b= .51, p= .06, see Table 3). The effects of
implicit conscientiousness on participants’ academic performance
are greater for men than for women (see Fig. 1).
Lastly, we investigated the differential predictive validity of im-
plicit and explicit measures of conscientiousness (H3), entering
them in the following steps (explicit first) in a hierarchical linear
regression. implicit conscientiousness accounts for a relevant per-
centage of performance variance that is not already accounted for
by the explicit measures (
D
R
2BFQ
= .07; p= .03;
D
R
2BFO
= .045;
p= .07).
3.3. Discussion
This study further explored the relationship between implicit
and explicit conscientiousness and academic achievement, mea-
sured as (1) the number of successful examinations taken in the
eight months after the laboratory phase and (2) participants’ mean
mark in these exams. In line with previous literature (see e.g. Stef-
fens, 2004), we found that explicit measures of conscientiousness
can be easily faked, whereas implicit measures are more resistant.
We found that both implicit and explicit conscientiousness in-
crease participants’ number of exams successfully passed in the
past year without affecting their mean mark. In addition, we
showed that the predictive power of implicit conscientiousness
cannot be accounted for by the explicit measures. Gender moder-
ated the effect of implicit (but not explicit) conscientiousness on
performance, such that its effect on performance is stronger for
men than for women. Interestingly, and in line with previous
literature (e.g. Schmitt et al., 2008), men also reported being less
conscientious than women, but implicit conscientiousness was
not different across genders.
4. General discussion
In the literature, implicit measures of attitudes and stereotypes
were successfully employed to predict scholastic and academic
achievement (Kiefer & Sekaquaptewa, 2007; Nosek et al. 2009). Re-
sults of our studies showed that academic achievements are pre-
dicted by implicit conscientiousness as well. In study 1 we found
that implicit conscientiousness predicted participants’ mean mark
and in study 2 implicit conscientiousness predicted the number of
exams successfully passed. These differing results seem to suggest
that the effect of conscientiousness is different according to partic-
ipants’ experience at the university. Indeed, whereas most students
in the first study were enrolled in the first year, those involved in
the second one were enrolled in the third (and last) year. We
hypothesize that in a first stage implicit conscientiousness brings
students to improve their mean marks, whereas at the end of their
degree course conscientious students pass more exams without
lowering their mean mark.
Results of our studies provide the first demonstration that im-
plicit conscientiousness predicts academic performance. Moreover,
we have shown that implicit conscientiousness is incrementally
valid when compared to explicit measures. This result emphasizes
the importance of implicit constructs and processes in personality
theory and applications. Pending further investigation, this result
also suggests that implicit measures should be included in research
and interventions on vocational guidance and school admission
Table 2
Study 2. Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations between study variables.
M(SD)
a
(n. items) Correlations
BFO BFQ Mean Mark Number of Exams
IAT D .45 (.26) .86 (24) .14 .03 .13 .27
*
BFO 4.78 (.92) .80 (8) .81
**
.16 .35
**
BFQ 3.44 (.56) .90 (24) .16 .28
*
Mean Examination Mark 25.69 (2.72) .09
Number of exams successfully passed 3.03 (1.96)
*
p< .05.
**
p< .01.
Table 3
Results of the hierarchical linear regression on academic performance.
Step Independent variable added
D
R
2
(p)bt(df)p
Intercept
1 Implicit conscientiousness .07 (.04) .44 2.88 (61) <.01
2 Moderator (Male vs. Female) .02 (.24) .56 2.24 (60) .03
3 Interaction (Impl. Consc. *Mod.) .05 (.06) .51 1.90 (59) .06
Note: values in the last three columns refer to the third step (R
2
= .15).
0
1
2
3
4
5
6
Low Implicit
Conscientiousness
High Implicit
Conscientiousness
Number of succesful exams
Males
Females
Fig. 1. The moderation effect of gender on the relationship between implicit
conscientiousness and the number of exams successfully passed by participants.
4M. Vianello et al. / Personality and Individual Differences xxx (2009) xxx–xxx
ARTICLE IN PRESS
Please cite this article in press as: Vianello, M., et al. Implicit conscientiousness predicts academic performance. Personality and Individual Differences
(2009), doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.019
procedures. However, how these constructs interact with other
important predictors of performance such as intelligence and core
self-evaluations remains to be clarified and we hope future re-
search will address this important question.
A second contribution concerns the different effects of our
manipulation on implicit and explicit conscientiousness and the
gender differences we observed in explicit, but not implicit, consci-
entiousness. In line with previous studies, we found that encourag-
ing participants to fake the tests increased their explicit but not
their implicit conscientiousness. However, faking did not moderate
the relationships between implicit and explicit measures of consci-
entiousness and academic performance. In addition, we found that
men were less conscientious than women according to explicit
measures, but implicit conscientiousness was not different across
genders. Gender differences in explicit conscientiousness – and
personality traits in general – are an established notion, and they
have recently been linked to levels of human development (e.g.,
in terms of a long and healthy life, equal access to knowledge
and education, and economic wealth) achieved by a given culture.
Schmitt et al. (2008) proposed that ‘‘heightened levels of sexual
dimorphism result from personality traits of men and women
being less constrained and more able to naturally diverge in devel-
oped nations. In less fortunate social and economic conditions, in-
nate personality differences between men and women may be
attenuated” (p. 168). Yet, our results suggest a less innatist inter-
pretation. If implicit gender differences are smaller than explicit
gender differences, as it seems, it is reasonable to hypothesize that
gender differences in personality traits can be ascribed to social
role requirements (which influence more explicit measures), or
even to social desirability, rather than to traits. A test of this
hypothesis, by using implicit measures of both conscientiousness
and gender-personality stereotype in cross-cultural investigations,
might lead to interesting results that could solve the ‘‘nature vs.
nurture” debate in this area.
Lastly, results of study 2 suggest that gender tends to moderate
the relationship between performance and implicit, but not expli-
cit, conscientiousness. Implicit conscientiousness predicts aca-
demic performance more strongly in men than in women,
whereas explicit conscientiousness influences performance of both
genders. We think a moderating effect of gender was actually pres-
ent in both relationships, but we failed to see it while employing ex-
plicit measures because of their malleability. We suspect that
participants were influenced in their responses by the social repre-
sentation of men and women’s respective personalities, which in
our culture depicts the latter as more careful, more tidy, less
superficial and less lazy than men. This gender-conscientiousness
stereotype might have led females to increase their explicit consci-
entiousness and/or males to decrease it (in males, this phenomenon
has already been observed by Schmitt et al., 2008). Further, it is
likely that social desirability effects were more intense for less con-
scientious females and for more conscientious males, because of
their greater distance from the social norm. This effect, mainly
due to the greater controllability of explicit measures, might have
obscured a moderating effect that, in contrast, was observed with
the implicit measure, which is more resistant to faking.
A limit of our studies is that we still lack evidence of the condi-
tions under which implicit measures are more predictive than ex-
plicit measures of conscientiousness. We hypothesized that
impression management strategies would be important modera-
tors of these relationships, and tested this hypothesis in study 2.
Yet, we failed to find a significant effect. Future research might
be addressed to this important point.
In conclusion, our studies suggest that implicit conscientious-
ness positively predicts academic performance, that it is more
resistant than explicit measures to faking attempts, and that the
implicit measure has relevant incremental validity over two differ-
ent explicit measures. Finally, we have shown that gender tends to
moderate the implicit-performance and not the explicit-perfor-
mance relationship. These results confirm the importance of the
implicit component in personality traits and emphasizes their
importance in predicting behavior. Although these findings must
be seen as preliminary, they are strong enough to open important
research questions that, if adequately addressed, could lead to an
improved comprehension of important phenomena.
References
Aiken, L. S., & West, S. G. (1991). Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting
interactions. Newbury Park: Sage.
Bar-Anan, Y., Nosek, B. A., & Vianello, M. (2009). The sorting paired features task: A
measure of association strengths. Experimental Psychology, 56, 329–343.
Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in
social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical
considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.
Barrick, M. R., & Mount, M. K. (1991). The big five personality dimensions and job
performance: A meta-analysis. Personnel Psychology, 44, 1–26.
Blair, I. (2002). The malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice. Personality
and Social Psychology Review, 6, 242–261.
Boldero, J. M., Rawlings, D., & Haslam, N. (2007). Convergence between GNAT-
assessed implicit and explicit personality. European Journal of Personality, 3,
341–358.
Borkenau, P., & Ostendorf, F. (1993). NEO-FFI. Neo-Fünf-Faktoren Inventar nach Costa
und McCrae – deutsche Fassung [Neo Five Factor Inventory acc. to Costa and
McCrae – German version]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.
Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., & Borgogni, L. (1994). BFO, big five observer. Manuale.
Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali.
Caprara, G. V., Barbaranelli, C., & Borgogni, L. (2000). BFQ, big five questionnaire.
Manuale (2nd ed.). Firenze: Organizzazioni Speciali.
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2004). A possible model for understanding
the personality-intelligence interface. British Journal of Psychology, 95, 249–264.
Di Fabio, A., & Busoni, L. (2007). Fluid intelligence, personality traits and scholastic
success: Empirical evidence in a sample of Italian high school students.
Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 2095–2104.
Do Yeong, K. (2003). Voluntary controllability of the implicit association test (IAT).
Social Psychology Quarterly, 66, 83.
Fiedler, K., & Blümke, M. (2005). Faking the IAT: Aided and unaided response control
on the implicit association tests. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27,
307–316.
Greenwald, A. G., Banaji, M. R., Rudman, L. A., Farnham, S. D., Nosek, B. A., & Mellott,
D. S. (2002). A unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and
self-concept. Psychological Review, 109, 3–25.
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual
differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480.
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the
implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216.
Greenwald, A. G., Poehlman, T. A., Uhlmann, E., & Banaji, M. R. (2009).
Understanding and using the implicit association test: III. Meta-analysis of
predictive validity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 17–41.
Grumm, M., & von Collani, G. (2007). Measuring big-five personality dimensions
with the implicit association test – Implicit personality traits or self-esteem?
Personality and Individual Differences, 43(8), 2205–2217.
Higgins, D. M., Peterson, J. B., Pihl, R. O., & Lee, A. G. M. (2007). Prefrontal cognitive
ability, intelligence, big five personality, and the prediction of advanced
academic and workplace performance. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 93, 298–319.
Holden, R. R. (2008). Underestimating the effects of faking on the validity of self-
report personality scales. Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 311–321.
Kiefer, A. K., & Sekaquaptewa, D. (2007). Implicit stereotypes, gender identification,
and math-related outcomes. A prospective study of female college students.
Psychological Science, 18, 13–18.
Komar, S., Brown, D. J., Komar, J. A., & Robie, C. (2008). Faking and the validity of
conscientiousness: A Monte Carlo investigation. Journal of Applied Psychology,
93, 140–154.
Lane, K. A., Banaji, M. R., Nosek, B. A., & Greenwald, A. G. (2007). Understanding and
using the implicit association test: IV: Procedures and validity. In B.
Wittenbrink, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes: Procedures and
controversies (pp. 59–102). New York: Guilford Press.
Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Math = male, me = female,
therefore math me. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 44–59.
Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Sriram, N., Lindner, N. M., Devos, T., Ayala, A., et al. (2009).
National differences in gender-science stereotypes predict national sex
differences in science and math achievement. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences, 106, 10593–10597.
Payne, B. K., Cheng, C. M., Govorun, O., & Stewart, B. D. (2005). An inkblot for
attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality
and Social Psychology, 89, 277–293.
M. Vianello et al. / Personality and Individual Differences xxx (2009) xxx–xxx 5
ARTICLE IN PRESS
Please cite this article in press as: Vianello, M., et al. Implicit conscientiousness predicts academic performance. Personality and Individual Differences
(2009), doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.019
Robusto, E., Cristante, F., & Vianello, M. (2008). Assessing the impact of replication
on implicit association test effects by means of the extended logistic model for
the assessment of change. Behavior Research Methods, 40, 954–960.
Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (1998). The validity and utility of selection methods in
personnel psychology: Practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of
research findings. Psychological Bulletin, 124, 262–274.
Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. E. (2004). General mental ability in the world of work:
Occupational attainment and job performance. Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 86, 162–173.
Schmitt, D. P., Realo, A., Voracek, M., & Allik, J. (2008). Why can’t a man be more like
a woman? Sex differences in Big Five personality traits across 55 cultures.
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 168–182.
Schnabel, K., Asendorpf, J. B., & Greenwald, A. G. (2008). Understanding and using
the implicit association test: V. measuring semantic aspects of trait self-
concepts. European Journal of Personality, 22, 695–706.
Steffens, M. C. (2004). Is the implicit association test immune to faking?
Experimental Psychology, 51, 165–179.
Steffens, M. C., & Schulze König, S. (2006). Predicting spontaneous big five behavior
with implicit association tests. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 22,
13–20.
Weathington, B. L., & Moldenhauer, H. A. (2008). Gender role and personality as
predictors of peer and self leadership evaluations. E-Journal of Applied
Psychology, 4(1), 7–14.
6M. Vianello et al. / Personality and Individual Differences xxx (2009) xxx–xxx
ARTICLE IN PRESS
Please cite this article in press as: Vianello, M., et al. Implicit conscientiousness predicts academic performance. Personality and Individual Differences
(2009), doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.11.019
... As a stable personality factor, conscientiousness is closely related to adolescent grit (Duckworth et al., 2007). Diligence and selfdiscipline are two basic characteristics of conscientiousness that can effectively motivate adolescents to achieve their learning goals and increase their courage when facing learning difficulties (Vianello et al., 2010). Adolescents' conscientiousness is also influenced by parenting style. ...
... Researchers generally agree that personality is composed of five factors: neuroticism, agreeableness, extroversion, conscientiousness, and openness (Judge & Zapata, 2015), commonly referred to as the Big Five personality traits. Among them, conscientiousness reflects the characteristics of individual diligence, effort, and selfdiscipline (Vianello et al., 2010). This study found that adolescents' conscientiousness can significantly predict academic grit. ...
... Among the Big Five personality traits, conscientiousness is the closest to academic grit. Diligence, effort, and focus are the core characteristics of conscientiousness, and can prompt individuals to adhere to long-term goals and stay focused throughout the learning process (Vianello et al., 2010), all of which contribute to the development of outstanding academic grit. Therefore, conscientiousness is a vital personality trait in promoting the development of academic grit. ...
Article
Full-text available
Academic grit is a core quality that has a beneficial effect on students’ academic adaptation and development. In the current study, both environmental and personal factors were incorporated into an exploration of the aspects affecting academic grit. A total of 4,034 Chinese adolescents completed the Academic Grit, Positive Parenting Style, Personal Growth Initiative, and Conscientiousness Scales. The results show that there was a positive association between a positive parenting style and adolescents’ academic girt. The total effect of a positive parenting style on academic grit was 0.41. The mediating effect of conscientiousness and personal growth initiative accounted for 37% and 51% of the total effect, respectively. This study examined the influences and mechanisms of academic grit, providing a theoretical basis and directional reference for future intervention studies.
... There are now numerous studies that have examined if implicit methods might successfully measure personality or self-concept traits (Asendorpf et al., 2002;Back et al., 2009;Dentale et al., 2019;Grumm & von Collani, 2007;Kolnes et al., 2021;Purol et al., 2022;Steffens and König, 2006;Vianello et al., 2010;Vianello et al., 2013;Vecchione et al., 2017); for a review see De Cuyper et al. (2017) though see Lazarević et al. (2021) for contrary evidence. For example, Vianello et al. (2010: Study 1) developed an implicit measure of "conscientiousness" and found that this was associated with academic performance (defined as the number of exams passed in the last year), whereas an explicit measure of conscientiousness was not related to performance. ...
... There are now numerous studies that have examined if implicit methods might successfully measure personality or self-concept traits (Asendorpf et al., 2002;Back et al., 2009;Dentale et al., 2019;Grumm & von Collani, 2007;Kolnes et al., 2021;Purol et al., 2022;Steffens and König, 2006;Vianello et al., 2010;Vianello et al., 2013;Vecchione et al., 2017); for a review see De Cuyper et al. (2017) though see Lazarević et al. (2021) for contrary evidence. For example, Vianello et al. (2010: Study 1) developed an implicit measure of "conscientiousness" and found that this was associated with academic performance (defined as the number of exams passed in the last year), whereas an explicit measure of conscientiousness was not related to performance. In study 2, Vianello et al. (2010) showed that both the implicit and explicit measures of conscientiousness predicted future academic performance and that the implicit measure had incremental validity over explicit measures. ...
... For example, Vianello et al. (2010: Study 1) developed an implicit measure of "conscientiousness" and found that this was associated with academic performance (defined as the number of exams passed in the last year), whereas an explicit measure of conscientiousness was not related to performance. In study 2, Vianello et al. (2010) showed that both the implicit and explicit measures of conscientiousness predicted future academic performance and that the implicit measure had incremental validity over explicit measures. These results confirm that implicit measures of personality can have value above and beyond what is extracted by explicit, self-report, measures of personality and may become a valuable method as an adjunct to understanding and even predicting behaviour. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study explored whether psychopathic traits might be measured using indirect or “implicit” measures. Three Implicit Association Tests, reflecting each domain of the Triarchic model of psychopathy: Boldness, Meanness and Disinhibition, were completed by mixed-gender, community-based samples (ns = 334, 376, 358). Each IAT demonstrated good internal reliability and showed concurrent validity. Associations with self-report scores on a validated explicit measure of Triarchic psychopathy exceeded those generally reported in the personality IAT literature. The IATs displayed discriminant validity, only correlating with their counterpart domain scores, predictive validity to some explicit measures of known correlates of psychopathy (e.g., aggression), and were robust to socially desirable responding. With further development, the IATs may be a useful research tool alongside self-report psychopathy measures.
... The Implicit Association Test (IAT) (Greenwald et al., 1998) has received increased attention in the field of personality psychology, because some researchers are of the opinion that IATs could measure personality in a way that is unbiased by self-distortion (Becker & Menges, 2013;Perugini & Banse, 2007). IATs have also shown predictive value for various criteria, such as non-cheating behaviour (Perugini & Leone, 2009), academic outcomes (Vianello et al., 2010), and job performance (Vecchione et al., 2016). However, despite their rising popularity, IATs are not widely used for assessment and selection in universities and other organisations, largely because important questions remain about measurement issues. ...
... p < .05 (Steffens & Schulze König, 2006;Vianello et al., 2010). Finally, Rowatt et al. (2006) assessed the reaction times of humility vs. arrogance word associations with an IAT and compared these scores to students' academic course grades. ...
Article
The goal of this research was to develop an implicit association test for honesty-humility (IAT-HH) and assess its validity. Construct validity was established by conducting correlations and confirmatory factor analysis. A model with honesty-humility facets measured by two related but distinct method factors (self-report vs. implicit) showed superior fit, suggesting that our IAT-HH captured important substantive variance, but also IAT-specific variance. We further investigated criterion-related validity in relation to academic behaviour of Belgian university students (N = 161), using both desirable and undesirable criteria. Our results suggest that IATs of honesty-humility may show measurement overlap with self-reports. However, the IAT-HH could neither predict self-report nor behavioural academic criteria. Predicting behaviour with the IAT-HH appears to be complex and is discussed.
... It is seen that men are more planned and systematic in their approach to work. Social factors in India which free men from most domestic responsibilities may result in their greater ability to approach work in an organized manner [33]. Employees with less than 5 years' experience followed by those with 6 to 10 years dominate in conscientious behavior. ...
Research
Full-text available
Identifying the factors that encourage the work behavior that is most crucial to organization's success and discovering the factors that promote this behavior is of utmost importance for sustainable excellent performance in this highly competitive business environment. This paper attempts to measure the relationship among Demographic factors and Organizational Citizenship Behavior, Emotional Intelligence and Personality factors of managerial employees in the IT sector of Bangalore city. The research design was elaborate and descriptive in nature.406 managers from the IT sector consisted of the sample for the study. The results reveal that Demographic factors impact specific dimensions of Organizational Citizenship Behavior, Emotional Intelligence and Personality of the employees. These findings can be of significance while deciding Human Resource strategies for Talent acquisition, Training and Development and Compensation management.
... We find that boarding has a more significant impact on girls' conscientiousness of the Big Five personality traits, which is consistent with previous research on gender differences in conscientiousness [69]. This may be attributed to female personality traits and the role of gender as a moderating variable affecting individual commitment [70]. Social role theory also suggests that individuals of different genders develop different senses of group identity, causing them to behave differently in different social situations [71]. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Since China adopted a policy to eliminate rural learning centers, boarding has become an important feature of the current rural student community. However, there is a lack of consensus on the impact of boarding schools on students' cognitive and non-cognitive development. This study investigates the effect of boarding schools on the development of cognitive and non-cognitive abilities of junior high school students in rural northwest China. Methods Using a sample of 5,660 seventh-grade students from 160 rural junior high schools across 19 counties, we identify a causal relationship between boarding and student abilities with the instrumental variables (IV) approach. Results The results suggest that boarding positively influences memory and attention, while it has no significant effect on other cognitive abilities such as reasoning, transcription speed, and accuracy. Furthermore, we find no significant association between boarding and the development of non-cognitive skills. Conclusions Given the widespread prevalence of boarding schools in rural regions, our study highlights the growing importance of improving school management to promote the development of students’ cognitive abilities and integrating the development of non-cognitive or social-emotional abilities into students’ daily routines.
... Prior reviews were optimistic about the potential for experimental and implicit measures of conscientiousness (e.g., Roberts et al., 2014). At the time, however, implicit measures were a mystery as they failed to correlate with self-or observer reports of conscientiousness (e.g., Vianello et al., 2010), a mystery that still remains (Vecchione et al., 2014). The fate of experimental approaches, such as techniques to assess executive functioning and inhibitory control, has been called entirely into question . ...
Article
Full-text available
Why does conscientiousness matter for education? How is conscientiousness conceptualized in the field of research on education? How do socio-emotional (SE) skills relate to conscientiousness? In an effort to help answer these questions, we review the current research on conscientiousness in education. Specifically, we examine (1) how conscientiousness is defined, (2) the assessment of conscientiousness, (3) the relation between conscientiousness and educational outcomes, (4) whether too much conscientiousness may be a bad thing, (5) the relation between conscientiousness and conceptually related educational constructs, (6) the changeability of conscientiousness and the importance of that fact to education, and (7) the challenges of assessing conscientiousness across cultures.
... These address unconscious/implicit tendencies, thought to better predict spontaneous and long-term behavioral patterns. Among those using this approach are Vianello, Robusto, and Anselmi (2010) who correlated implicit measures of Conscientiousness with academic performance; and Miner and Raju (2004) who compared implicit risk-propensities among low and high-growth entrepreneurs. Another approach might be experimental, which directly assess behaviors through laboratorycontrolled tasks. ...
Article
Full-text available
Among the Big Five personality dimensions (i.e., Agreeableness, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Neuroticism, and Openness), it is Conscientiousness that, more so than the others, predicts entrepreneurial intention, performance, and actual business creation (e.g., Zhou, Seibert and Lumpkin, 2010). Yet, despite its salience, the various facets of Conscientiousness and their association with specific entrepreneurial behaviors are not clearly understood. The present study cross-references the psychology literature and uncovers an extended set of thirteen Conscientiousness facets, ordering them in terms of prevalence and salience, and then links them to specific entrepreneurial behaviors. Following this, it also provides a host of implications as well as future research directions.
Article
This paper reports on the coping strategies of mature female students whilst studying a Foundation Programme at a Business School. In recent years, research has focussed less on the problems that confront adult learners and more on how they exercise personal agency and cope with the multifarious challenges of combining study with part-time employment and a home life. In particular, interest has focussed on the personality and how specific traits are associated with performative success. This paper reports on two aspects of personality that appear in the literature: conscientiousness and resilience as these are associated with the exercise of personal agency. The research reports on interviews with five mature female students, three of whom are mothers with caring responsibilities. The findings were analysed using Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis and provide insight into the exercise of successful personal agency. This paper also offers a model of how to conceptualise the support for mature students with their complex and varied needs.
Thesis
Full-text available
The objective of this research was to develop a causal model of bank branches’ performance. This model consisted of 8 latent variables, as managerial competency (α = .92), conscientiousness (α = .78), leadership self-efficacy (α = .77), perceived organizational support (α = .81), achievement orientation (α = .76), workplace friendship (α = .84), subordinates’ performance, and bank branches’ performance. The sample consisted of 281 branch managers form a private bank. The research results indicated that the model was valid and well fitted to the empirical data (Chi-square = 15.44, df = 8, p = .051, GFI = .99, AGFI = .94). The predictors accounted for 60% of the variance in bank branches’ performance.
Article
Full-text available
This article summarizes the practical and theoretical implications of 85 years of research in personnel selection. On the basis of meta-analytic findings, this article presents the validity of 19 selection procedures for predicting job performance and training performance and the validity of paired combinations of general mental ability (GMA) and the 18 other selection procedures. Overall, the 3 combinations with the highest multivariate validity and utility for job performance were GMA plus a work sample test (mean validity of .63), GMA plus an integrity test (mean validity of .65), and GMA plus a structured interview (mean validity of .63). A further advantage of the latter 2 combinations is that they can be used for both entry level selection and selection of experienced employees. The practical utility implications of these summary findings are substantial. The implications of these research findings for the development of theories of job performance are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
In this article, we attempt to distinguish between the properties of moderator and mediator variables at a number of levels. First, we seek to make theorists and researchers aware of the importance of not using the terms moderator and mediator interchangeably by carefully elaborating, both conceptually and strategically, the many ways in which moderators and mediators differ. We then go beyond this largely pedagogical function and delineate the conceptual and strategic implications of making use of such distinctions with regard to a wide range of phenomena, including control and stress, attitudes, and personality traits. We also provide a specific compendium of analytic procedures appropriate for making the most effective use of the moderator and mediator distinction, both separately and in terms of a broader causal system that includes both moderators and mediators. (46 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
Full-text available
In reporting Implicit Association Test (IAT) results, researchers have most often used scoring conventions described in the first publication of the IAT (A. G. Greenwald, D. E. McGhee, & J. L. K. Schwartz, 1998). Demonstration IATs available on the Internet have produced large data sets that were used in the current article to evaluate alternative scoring procedures. Candidate new algorithms were examined in terms of their (a) correlations with parallel self-report measures, (b) resistance to an artifact associated with speed of responding, (c) internal consistency, (d) sensitivity to known influences on IAT measures, and (e) resistance to known procedural influences. The best-performing measure incorporates data from the IAT's practice trials, uses a metric that is calibrated by each respondent's latency variability, and includes a latency penalty for errors. This new algorithm strongly outperforms the earlier (conventional) procedure.
Article
Full-text available
This paper investigated the relationship among gender role, personality and peer and self evaluations of leadership behavior. Participants formed teams to complete a leaderless group activity and then completed personality, gender role, and leadership questionnaires. Results indicated that low femininity and high extraversion scores were predictive of leadership behaviors. However, high femininity had little relation to self-rated leadership regardless of extraversion. Results also suggest that high levels of masculinity and conscientiousness were predictive of high peer evaluations of leadership ability. A weak correlation was found between self and peer evaluations of leadership which along with the other results suggests that individuals that rate themselves as good leaders are not necessarily perceived as good leaders by others. Implications and directions for future research are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
College students, especially women, demonstrated negativity toward math and science relative to arts and language on implicit measures. Group membership (being female), group identity (self = female), and gender stereotypes (math = male) were related to attitudes and identification with mathematics. Stronger implicit math = male stereotypes corresponded with more negative implicit and explicit math attitudes for women but more positive attitudes for men. Associating the self with female and math with male made it difficult for women, even women who had selected math-intensive majors, to associate math with the self. These results point to the opportunities and constraints on personal preferences that derive from membership in social groups.
Article
Full-text available
According to theories brought forward recently, implicit measures based on reaction times, for instance Implicit Association Tests (IATs), should predict spontaneous behavior better than explicit measures. We applied five IATs to the measurement of the Big Five personality factors and tested whether the IATs predicted spontaneous behavior. The results show that, although implicit and explicit measures of personality dimensions were related at times, the correlations between them and with behavior suggest that these constructs should be differentiated. IATs predicted spontaneous behavior, but explicit measures did not. In contrast, explicit measures, but not IATs, were related to transparent self-ratings of behavior.
Article
Abstract Implicit Association Tests (IATs) often reveal strong associations of self with positive rather than negative attributes. This poses a problem,in using the IAT to measure associations involving traits with either positive or negative evaluative content. In two studies, we employed non-bipolar but evaluatively balanced Big Five traits as attribute contrasts and explored correlations of IATs with positive (e.g. sociable vs. conscientious) or negative (e.g. reserved vs. chaotic) attributes. Results showed,(a) satisfactory internal consistencies for all IATs, (b) explicit–explicit and implicit–implicit correlations that were moderate to high and comparable,in strength after both were corrected for attenuation and (c) better model fit for latent variable models that linked the implicit and explicit measures to distinct latent factors rather to the same factor. Together, the results suggest that IATs can validly assess the semantic aspect of trait self-concepts and that implicit and explicit self-representations are, although correlated, also distinct constructs. Copyright # 2008 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Key words: implicit personality self-concept; semantic associations; valence associa- tions; Big Five Often we realize very quickly whether we like or dislike something or somebody,without
Article
The present article reviews evidence for the malleability of automatic stereotypes and prejudice. In contrast to assumptions that such responses are fixed and ines- capable, it is shown that automatic stereotypes and prejudice are influenced by, (a) self- and social motives, (b) specific strategies, (c) the perceiver's focus of atten- tion, and (d) the configuration of stimulus cues. In addition, group members' indi- vidual characteristics are shown to influence the extent to which (global) stereo- types and prejudice are automatically activated. This evidence has significant implications for conceptions of automaticity, models of stereotyping and prejudice, and attitude representation. The review concludes with the description of an initial model of early social information processing. Given a thimbleful of facts we rush to make general- izations as large as a tub. … Life is short, and the de- mands upon us for practical adjustments so great, that we cannot let our ignorance detain us in our daily transactions. (Allport, 1954, p. 9)
Article
This study investigated the relation of the "Big Five" personality di- mensions (Extraversion, Emotional Stability, Agreeableness, Consci- entiousness, and Openness to Experience) to three job performance criteria (job proficiency, training proficiency, and personnel data) for five occupational groups (professionals, police, managers, sales, and skilled/semi-skilled). Results indicated that one dimension of person- ality. Conscientiousness, showed consistent relations with all job per- formance criteria for all occupational groups. For the remaining per- sonality dimensions, the estimated true score correlations varied by occupational group and criterion type. Extraversion was a valid pre- dictor for two occupations involving social interaction, managers and sales (across criterion types). Also, both Openness to Experience and Extraversion were valid predictors of the training proficiency criterion (across occupations). Other personality dimensions were also found to be valid predictors for some occupations and some criterion types, but the magnitude of the estimated true score correlations was small (p < .10). Overall, the results illustrate the benefits of using the 5- factor model of personality to accumulate and communicate empirical findings. The findings have numerous implications for research and practice in personnel psychology, especially in the subfields of person- nel selection, training and development, and performance appraisal.