Eric R. Jensen's research while affiliated with University of California, Berkeley and other places

What is this page?


This page lists the scientific contributions of an author, who either does not have a ResearchGate profile, or has not yet added these contributions to their profile.

It was automatically created by ResearchGate to create a record of this author's body of work. We create such pages to advance our goal of creating and maintaining the most comprehensive scientific repository possible. In doing so, we process publicly available (personal) data relating to the author as a member of the scientific community.

If you're a ResearchGate member, you can follow this page to keep up with this author's work.

If you are this author, and you don't want us to display this page anymore, please let us know.

Publications (6)


Figure 1: EL4 and B16-BL6 tumour cells that express NKG2D ligands are rejected by NK cells in syngeneic mice.Con Ig, control immunoglobulin. a–e, C57BL/6 or B6-Rag1-/- mice were treated with the indicated antibodies before inoculation with the indicated number of EL4 (a, b) or B16-BL6 (c–e) tumour cell transductants. Tumour growth was monitored thereafter. Data are representative of at least three experiments. f, g, B6 mice were injected intravenously with 3  105 B16-BL6 transductants. Lung metastases were counted two weeks after the tumour challenge (f), or the lung lobes from representative mice in a separate experiment were examined three weeks after tumour challenge (g).
Figure 2: RMA cells expressing NKG2D ligands are rejected by NK cells and CD8 T cells in syngeneic mice.a, b, B6 (a) or B6-Rag1-/- (b) mice were pretreated with the indicated antibodies and inoculated subcutaneously with the indicated numbers of RMA transductants or RMA/S cells. Fractions are indicated for instances where tumours grew in some, but not all, mice. Data are representative of four experiments. Con Ig, control immunoglobulin. c, Levels of NKG2D ligands determined by staining with fluorescently labelled tetrameric NKG2D. Tumour cell transductants, the YAC-1 lymphoma and the Sa1N fibrosarcoma are compared. WT, wild type. d, The anti-tumour response depends on Rae1 levels. Survival of B6 mice that had been inoculated with 1  105 RMA cells, or RMA cells expressing high (Rae1hi) or intermediate (Rae1int) levels of Rae1, is shown. Terminally moribund mice were killed. Data are representative of two experiments.
Figure 3: Vaccination with ligand-expressing tumour cells confers specific immunity to the corresponding ligand-negative tumour cells.a, B6 mice that had previously rejected ligand-transduced tumour cells (5  106 EL4, 1  104 B16-BL6 or 1  104 RMA transductants) were inoculated subcutaneously with control-transduced tumour cells of the same type (EL4, 5  106; B16-BL6, 1  104; RMA, 1  105). Primary exposure occurred 8–12 weeks before challenge. b, B6 mice that had been vaccinated 12 weeks earlier with each ligand-transduced tumour cell type were injected with the same or different ligand-negative cell lines. The primary and secondary tumour doses were as in a. c, Depletion of CD8+ cells during the primary challenge prevents development of immunity. Re-challenge with control-transduced tumour cells occurred 8–12 weeks after vaccination. Naive B6 mice were challenged in parallel. d, B6 mice were injected with PBS or were vaccinated with 5  106 irradiated (once or three times) or 1  104 living, transduced or untransduced B16-BL6 tumour cells. Mice were challenged 10 days later with 104 untransduced B16-BL6 cells in the opposite flank. Data are representative of two experiments.
Rae1 and H60 ligands of the NKG2D receptor stimulate tumor immunity
  • Article
  • Full-text available

October 2001

·

638 Reads

·

943 Citations

Nature

Andreas Diefenbach

·

Eric R. Jensen

·

·

David H. Raulet

Natural killer (NK) cells attack many tumour cell lines, and are thought to have a critical role in anti-tumour immunity; however, the interaction between NK cells and tumour targets is poorly understood. The stimulatory lectin-like NKG2D receptor is expressed by NK cells, activated CD8+ T cells and by activated macrophages in mice. Several distinct cell-surface ligands that are related to class I major histocompatibility complex molecules have been identified, some of which are expressed at high levels by tumour cells but not by normal cells in adults. However, no direct evidence links the expression of these 'induced self' ligands with tumour cell rejection. Here we demonstrate that ectopic expression of the murine NKG2D ligands Rae1beta or H60 in several tumour cell lines results in potent rejection of the tumour cells by syngeneic mice. Rejection is mediated by NK cells and/or CD8+ T cells. The ligand-expressing tumour cells induce potent priming of cytotoxic T cells and sensitization of NK cells in vivo. Mice that are exposed to live or irradiated tumour cells expressing Rae1 or H60 are specifically immune to subsequent challenge with tumour cells that lack NKG2D ligands, suggesting application of the ligands in the design of tumour vaccines.

Download
Share

Localization interference between free-field signals

October 1996

·

3 Reads

The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America

With headphones, the perceived lateral position of one frequency band (the target) can be affected by the presence of a second band with different interaural parameters (the interferer). This binaural interference persists despite a stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) from interferer to target of up to 320 ms [L. R. Bernstein and C. Trahiotis, J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 94, 735?742 (1993)]. The current study presented targets and interferers in anechoic space from pairs of speakers chosen from an array of 12. A visual pointer was used to indicate the perceived location of the target. Localization of a sinusoidally amplitude?modulated (SAM) target (carrier = 500 Hz, modulator = 250 Hz) was unaffected by the presence of an SAM interferer (carrier = 4000 Hz). When the target was the higher frequency, however, there were large individual differences, with some listeners showing interference while others did not. Unlike the studies with headphones, when interference was seen a distractor to target SOA of only 40 ms was sufficient to eliminate the interference. [Work supported by a grant from the NIH, NIDCD 00087.]


Detecting changes in distance in an anechoic and echoic environment

May 1994

·

3 Reads

The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America

Increasing the distance to an acoustic source produces: (a) low‐pass filtering of the spectrum through absorption by the medium, (b) reduction in the overall sound‐pressure level of the stimulus, and (c) changes in the temporal and directional relations between sounds along the primary acoustic pathway and those on secondary pathways (echoes). A simulated open‐field environment [Hafter et al., J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 92, 2334 (1992)] consisting of an anechoic chamber and multiple computer‐controlled speakers was used to study the discriminability of changes in distance. Simulated acoustic reflections were from a single, fully reflective wall. Assuming spectral cues would be ineffective with small changes of distance, the primary concern was with the roles of level and echoes. Other considerations included the changing temporal overlap of primary and secondary stimuli for lengthening trains of wideband, midfrequency (4‐kHz center) impulses, and distance perception for single words and short phrases. Subjects reported that with echoes they always heard a change in distance, but without echoes, they heard either a change in distance orloudness. Thresholds were smallest when the changes were interpreted as distance, thus performance was generally better with echoes.


Localization in an Echoic Environment

December 1992

·

7 Reads

·

10 Citations

The effects of echoes on azimuthal discrimination and localization in the lateral plane were studied using single wide band clicks. Over much of the frontal quadrant, echoes decreased discrimination by no more than a factor of 2, though effects were greater near the aural axis. In general, localizations were of perceptually fused images closer to the source than the echo, but for large echo delays, the image sometimes broke into parts.


Localization in a virtual echoic environment

October 1992

·

7 Reads

·

6 Citations

The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America

Virtual auditory environments are generally created by filtering stimuli through head‐related transfer functions that mimic reflections and resonances of the shoulders, head, and external ears, and then presenting the sounds through earphones. Here, a different approach has been adopted in which multiple loudspeakers are used in an anechoic chamber to create virtual sources and echoes in space. Individual digital filters are used to equalize the loudspeakers and the computer/interface system allows for creation of multiple virtual sources and echoic surfaces. The locations of sources and echoic surfaces can be roved from trial to trial over a wide range of distances and over the eight separate azimuths allowed by the apparatus. Spatial discrimination is measured with two‐alternative forced choice psychophysics while, for localization, subjects use a laser pointer to indicate the directions of apparent sources. For wideband clicks (cf=4 kHz), these results show that the presence of single echoes increases spatial thresholds by a factor of about 2 across the range of source azimuths from 0° to 90°. The pointing data show that apparent locations of the sources are moved toward those of the echoes by about 4° to 5°, a result that is unaffected by the magnitudes of the source–echo separation in either azimuth (8° to 40°) or delay (1 to 30 ms). At longer delays, subjects sometimes report double images, one near the direction of the source and the other near that of the echo.


Citations (3)


... The term "precedence effect" (PE) (Wallach et al., 1949) was initially used to denote the listeners' ability to successfully localize sound sources in reverberant environments. However, the precedence effect is not absolute because reflections can extend a small influence on localization and resolution performance (Hafter et al., 1991;Rakerd & Hartmann, 1985). The precedence effect is also known by various aliases such as: "the law of the first wavefront" and the "first-arrival effect". ...

Reference:

Aspects of the Precedence Effect for simultaneous and non-simultaneous Auditory Streams (UNPUBLISHED)
Localization in an Echoic Environment
  • Citing Chapter
  • December 1992

... In the preliminary auditory profi le test battery, spatial hearing was measured psychophysically by means of a virtual minimum audible angle (MAA) test, and by two virtual spatial speech reception threshold (SRT) tests in noise, the spatial SRT benefi t test and the binaural SRT benefi t test (Van Esch et al, 2013). A virtual version of the minimum audible angle (MAA) test was chosen (Mills, 1985;Hafter et al, 1992;Grantham et al, 2003). Van Esch et al (2013) reported a small learning effect but no signifi cant effect of test centre, and good correspondence between test and retest measurements. ...

Localization in a virtual echoic environment
  • Citing Article
  • October 1992

The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America

... 37 NK cells, on the other hand, can recognize cancer cells that have reduced MHC class I expression as well as stressinduced ligands like RAE-1. 38,39 The protein analysis showed that Sulanemadlin significantly induced the expression of MHC class I molecules, the NK cell activating ligand RAE-1 as well as PD-L1 ( Figures 2D-2F), while treatment with an unspecific stapled peptide (non-binder) did not alter the expression of any of the analyzed proteins (Figures 2A-2F). ...

Rae1 and H60 ligands of the NKG2D receptor stimulate tumor immunity

Nature