ArticlePDF Available

Media Use and Children's Perceptions of Societal Threat and Personal Vulnerability

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

This study examined children's media use (i.e., amount of television and Internet usage) and relationships to children's perceptions of societal threat and personal vulnerability. The sample consisted of 90 community youth aged 7 to 13 years (M = 10.8; 52.2% male) from diverse economic backgrounds. Analyses found children's television use to be associated with elevated perceptions of personal vulnerability to world threats (i.e., crime, terrorism, earthquakes, hurricanes, and floods). An interactive model of television use and child anxiety in accounting for children's personal threat perceptions was supported, in which the strength of television consumption in predicting children's personal threat perceptions was greater for children with greater anxiety. Relationships were found neither between children's Internet use and threat perceptions nor between media use and perceptions of societal threat.
Content may be subject to copyright.
This article was downloaded by:[Comer, Jonathan S.]
On: 21 July 2008
Access Details: [subscription number 795173233]
Publisher: Routledge
Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954
Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK
Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent
Psychology
Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:
http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t775648094
Media Use and Children's Perceptions of Societal
Threat and Personal Vulnerability
Jonathan S. Comer a; Jami M. Furr b; Rinad S. Beidas b; Heather M. Babyar c;
Philip C. Kendall b
aDepartment of Psychiatry, Columbia University,
bDepartment of Psychology, Temple University,
cDepartment of Psychology, Kent State University,
Online Publication Date: 01 July 2008
To cite this Article: Comer, Jonathan S., Furr, Jami M., Beidas, Rinad S., Babyar,
Heather M. and Kendall, Philip C. (2008) 'Media Use and Children's Perceptions of
Societal Threat and Personal Vulnerability', Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 37:3, 622 — 630
To link to this article: DOI: 10.1080/15374410802148145
URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15374410802148145
PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE
Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf
This article maybe used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction,
re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly
forbidden.
The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be
complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should be
independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings,
demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or
arising out of the use of this material.
Downloaded By: [Comer, Jonathan S.] At: 19:20 21 July 2008
Media Use and Children’s Perceptions of Societal Threat
and Personal Vulnerability
Jonathan S. Comer
Department of Psychiatry, Columbia University
Jami M. Furr and Rinad S. Beidas
Department of Psychology, Temple University
Heather M. Babyar
Department of Psychology, Kent State University
Philip C. Kendall
Department of Psychology, Temple University
This study examined children’s media use (i.e., amount of television and Internet usage)
and relationships to children’s perceptions of societal threat and personal vulnerability.
The sample consisted of 90 community youth aged 7 to 13 years (M¼10.8; 52.2%male)
from diverse economic backgrounds. Analyses found children’s television use to be
associated with elevated perceptions of personal vulnerability to world threats (i.e.,
crime, terrorism, earthquakes, hurricanes, and floods). An interactive model of tele-
vision use and child anxiety in accounting for children’s personal threat perceptions
was supported, in which the strength of television consumption in predicting children’s
personal threat perceptions was greater for children with greater anxiety. Relationships
were found neither between children’s Internet use and threat perceptions nor between
media use and perceptions of societal threat.
External forces that influence children’s functioning
provide the contexts within which the development of
youth unfolds. These contexts vary in degree of proxim-
ity to the child and can be described as nested within
one another (Bronfenbrenner, 1979). Bronfenbrenner’s
model and its extensions (e.g., Belsky, 1980; Lynch &
Cicchetti, 1998) emphasize how these contexts influence
one another over time to shape child development.
By extending the scope of our perceptions, techno-
logical advances in mass communications have substan-
tially altered the ecology within which the development
of modern youth unfolds. Televisions have become
long-distance eyes, projecting images to us that exist
in the world far beyond our biological vision and
radios have become long-distance ears, propagating
sounds that originate far beyond our range of hearing.
Broadening availability of the Internet brings us closer
to what communications scholar Marshall McLuhan
(1964) called the ‘‘global village’’ in which the limita-
tions of physical space no longer apply and we can inter-
act with the other side of the world with the same
speed and facility afforded in geographically proximal
interactions.
American youth spend an enormous amount of time
consuming mass media. The U.S. Census reported that
This project was supported by a Graduate Student Research Grant
awarded to the first author by Division 53 of the American Psychologi-
cal Association (Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology), a Presi-
dential Fellowship awarded to the first author by Temple University,
and NIH grants 59087 and 64484 awarded to the final author.
Correspondence should be addressed to Jonathan S. Comer,
Columbia University=NYSPI, Division of Child Psychiatry, 1051
Riverside Drive, Unit 74, New York, NY 10032. E-mail: comerj@
childpsych.columbia.edu
Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology,37(3), 622–630, 2008
Copyright #Taylor & Francis Group, LLC
ISSN: 1537-4416 print=1537-4424 online
DOI: 10.1080/15374410802148145
Downloaded By: [Comer, Jonathan S.] At: 19:20 21 July 2008
98%of American households have a television set (see
Croteau & Hoynes, 1997), and there is a television set
in the bedroom of two thirds of American youth
between the ages of 8 and 18 (Gentile & Walsh, 2002;
Roberts et al., 2004; Wartella, Heintz, Aidman, &
Mazzarella, 1990). Thus, mass mediated messages
provide an increasingly proximal context for child devel-
opment. Roberts, Henriksen, and Foehr (2004) reported
that the average number of daily hours spent viewing
television is 3:19 for youth aged 8 to 10, 3:30 for youth
aged 11 to 14, and 2:23 for youth aged 15 to 18. In fact,
evidence suggests that almost 20%of youth between the
ages of 2 and 17 watch more than 35 hr of television per
week (Gentile & Walsh, 2002). Such television viewing
includes news programming, with even 3- to 5-year-old
children reportedly viewing an average of more than
1 hr of televised news=week (St. Peters, Fitch, Huston,
Wright, & Easkins, 1991). Roberts et al. (2004) reported
that for youth with Internet access, the total average of
Internet usage is approximately 1 hr online.
Empirical work with adults suggests that long-term
consumption of mass media can have deleterious effects
on global perceptions of threat and vulnerability.
Gerbner’s cultivation theory (Gerbner & Gross, 1976;
Gerbner, Gross, Morgan, & Signorielli, 1994) points
to how television portrays a world far more dangerous
than the one in which the average viewer actually
inhabits. Correspondingly, heavy television viewing is
believed to result in distorted perceptions of the world
as more dangerous and threatening than it is, yielding
exaggerated beliefs of a ‘‘mean and scary world.’’
Gerbner’s theory has garnered empirical support (see
Morgan & Shanahan, 1997). In a sample of more than
2,300 adults, Romer, Jamieson, and Aday (2003) found
a robust relationship between greater viewing of
televised news and perceptions of personal and societal
risks. Romer and colleagues also found that greater
exposure to televised news is related to a greater percep-
tion that crime is a problem, even after controlling for
actual crime rates in viewers’ neighborhoods.
Less is known about the relationships between media
use and threat perceptions in children. Such relation-
ships are particularly concerning in youth, given empiri-
cal advances highlighting the contributions of vicarious
learning and verbal threat information in the acquisition
of children’s anxious expectations, avoidant behavior,
physiological arousal, long-term fear beliefs, and other
anxious symptoms (e.g., Field & Lawson, 2003; Field &
Schorah, 2007; Field & Storksen-Coulson, 2007; Gerull
& Rapee, 2002). Consistent with evidence suggesting
that conditioning and direct experience with feared
stimuli may not be necessary for the development of
children’s threat perceptions, Smith and Wilson (2002)
found television news viewing to be predictive of chil-
dren’s crime rate perceptions, and Peterson and Zill
(1981) found children who were heavy television viewers
were more likely than light viewers to report distress and
fears of vulnerability to crime. No research has exam-
ined relationships between television use and children’s
perceptions of threat beyond crime, and no empirical
work has examined Internet use and children’s threat
perceptions.
In their review of terrorism-related media presenta-
tions, Comer and Kendall (2007) discussed the discourse
of disaster threat broadcast by the mass media—in
which daily social, cultural, and political events and
decisions are recast within the threat of future disaster.
Although Comer and Kendall focused on terrorism-
related media messages, the notion that disproportion-
ate media presentations of the possibility (rather than
probability) of personal victimization can adversely
affect children’s threat perceptions may hold true for
all disaster-related media presentations. In the wake of
the September 11 terror attacks, the 2004 tsunamis of
southeast Asia, and Hurricane Katrina, the media has
increasingly covered disaster-related threats. The topic
‘‘How safe are we from potential disasters?’’ has
frequently been a focus in news programming, and the
content of dramatic television programming has increas-
ingly incorporated plots about terrorism and disaster.
Statistically speaking, it is unlikely that the vast majority
of youth will ever experience direct exposure to a disas-
ter. However, given the enormous amount of time youth
spend consuming mass media (Roberts et al., 2004;
St. Peters et al., 1991), a substantial proportion of youth
are being exposed to this ongoing broadcast of disaster
threat, expectation, and alert. Research examining
media consumption and children’s threat perceptions
must examine children’s perceptions of vulnerability
to natural and man-made disasters, in addition to
perceptions of crime.
Parental media regulation has been understudied in
regards to child threat perceptions. Children whose
parents do not impose restrictions on their television
consumption watch more television than children
whose parents do impose such restrictions (e.g., Atkin,
Greenberg, & Baldwin, 1991; Krosnick, Anand, &
Hartl, 2003). Broadly speaking parental rule-setting
regarding children’s television viewing has been associa-
ted with less youth aggression (Nathanson, 1999). How-
ever, relationships between parental media regulation
and children’s threat perceptions remain unexamined.
Our study examined children’s media use (television
and Internet), parental media regulation, and their
relationships to children’s societal and personal risk
perceptions. Threat perceptions related to crime, terror-
ism, earthquakes, hurricanes, and floods were examined.
Analyses examined relationships between media use and
threat perceptions in the context of child anxiety; addi-
tive and interactive models were tested. Such analyses
MEDIA USE AND THREAT PERCEPTIONS 623
Downloaded By: [Comer, Jonathan S.] At: 19:20 21 July 2008
afforded examination of the relationships between
media use and threat perceptions beyond those
accounted for by child anxiety, controlling for self-
selection bias, in which anxious youth seek out greater
quantities of threat-related media.
METHODS
Participants
The sample consisted of 90 community youth aged 7 to
13 years (M
age
¼10.78, SD ¼2.0), and their mothers
(M
age
¼39.87, SD ¼7.96). Participants were recruited
from media advertisements and school-based outreach
in the Philadelphia area and were financially compen-
sated $50 for their time, which included participation in
additional studies. Participants had to be English speak-
ing. Youth (52.2%boys) were 47.8%Caucasian, 47.8%
African American, and 4.4%Other. There were 33.3%
from households that earned less than $39,999, 33.3%from
households earning between $40,000 and $69,999, and
33.3%from households earning above $70,000.
Measures
Television and Internet use. Mothers provided
child and family demographic data. Mothers were asked
whether they placed any restriction on their children’s
television use and Internet use, and if so, to indicate
the maximum quantity of hours=week they allow their
child to use television and Internet. Children indicated
the number of hours=week they watch television and
the number of hours=week they are online. Mothers
indicated the number of hours=week they themselves
watch television and are online.
Trait anxiety. The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for
Children (STAIC; Spielberger, 1973) A-Trait form was
used to assess child anxiety. The Trait form is a 20-item
child self-report measure of relatively stable individual
differences in anxiety. Children rated the extent to which
they typically experience anxiety symptoms (e.g., ‘‘I
worry about making mistakes;’’ ‘‘I am secretly afraid’’)
using 3-point scales ranging 1 (almost never), 2 (some-
times), and 3 (often). The STAIC has demonstrated
acceptable psychometric properties in community sam-
ples, including strong reliability and validity (Muris,
Merckelbach, Ollendick, King, & Bogie, 2002; Seligman,
Ollendick, Langley, & Baldacci, 2004; Silverman &
Rabian, 1999; a¼.86 in our sample).
Threat perception. Consistent with methodology
employed in the child subjective probability judgment
literature (e.g., Dalgleish et al., 1997), children rated the
likelihood of future threats (crime, terrorism, earth-
quakes, and hurricanes=floods) on 7-point Likert scales
from 0 (definitely will not happen)to6(definitely will hap-
pen). Visual and verbal anchors were provided after each
item to assist children’s comprehension of the scale. Chil-
dren provided two estimates for each threat: societal (e.g.,
‘‘What do you think the likelihood is that a major earth-
quake will occur in the U.S. over the next year?’’) and per-
sonal threat estimates (‘‘What do you think the likelihood
is that a major earthquake will directly affect you or your
family over the next year?’’). This eight-item scale is
referred to as the Societal and Personal Risk Assessment
for Youth (SPRAY; see the Appendix). Given evidence
that personal and societal threat perceptions may vary
independently of one another (Romer et al., 2003;
Smith & Wilson, 2002), analyses examined societal and
personal estimates separately (SPRAY(Societal) ¼sum
of Items 2, 4, 6, and 8; SPRAY(Personal)¼sum of
Items 1, 3, 5, 7). The SPRAY evidenced convergent val-
idity (significant correlation between SPRAY(Personal)
and STAIC—Trait; see Table 2) and good internal con-
sistency (a
societal
¼.71, a
personal
¼.71).
Procedures
Following approval of the protocol by the Institutional
Review Board, interested mother–child dyads, recruited
from media advertisements and school-based outreach,
were directed to contact the first author. English-
speaking dyads were scheduled for an appointment at
the Child and Adolescent Anxiety Disorders Clinic of
Temple University. At their appointment, informed
consent was obtained and mothers and children
completed assessments in separate rooms. Child forms
were completed with the assistance of a psychology
graduate student. At the end of their participation, dyads
received financial compensation and were debriefed.
RESULTS
Preliminary Analyses
Means and standard deviations of study variables are
presented in Table 1. It is possible that world events that
occurred during the data collection phase of the study
(e.g., occurrence of disaster) exerted an influence on
children’s threat perceptions. Participants’ ID numbers
were assigned consecutively and sequentially. To
provide a check on potential cohort effects, participant
ID number was not significantly correlated with any
study variables (i.e., all ps>.05), indicating that study
findings are not related to external (i.e., nonstudy)
events that occurred over the timeframe of the study.
624 COMER ET AL.
Downloaded By: [Comer, Jonathan S.] At: 19:20 21 July 2008
Children reported significantly greater television use
than Internet use, t(89) ¼5.12, p¼.001. Age was
significantly related to media use, with older children
using greater amounts of television (r¼.25, p<.05)
and Internet (r¼.30, p<.01). Boys and girls reported
similar amounts of television, F(1, 88) ¼.22, ns, and
Internet use, F(1, 88) ¼.33, ns. Television use and
Internet use did not significantly differ across income
categories, F(2, 84) ¼1.59, ns,andF(2, 84) ¼.12, ns,
respectively. Child television use was not significantly
related to maternal television use (r¼.10, ns), and child
Internet use was not significantly related to maternal
Internet use (r¼.16, ns).
Only 27%of mothers reported that there is a house-
hold rule regarding quantity of child television viewing;
26%reported presence of rule regarding maximum child
Internet use. Eleven mothers (12.2%) reported rules for
both child television and Internet use; of these mothers,
greater restriction were placed on child Internet use than
on television use, t(10) ¼2.84, p<.05. Demographic
factors were unrelated to parental media regulation.
Children of mothers who imposed restrictions on tele-
vision use were not significantly younger than children
of mothers who did not, F(1, 88) ¼1.38, p>.05. Simi-
larly, children of mothers who imposed restrictions on
Internet use were not significantly younger than children
of mothers who did not, F(1, 88) ¼.01, p>.05. Mothers
of girls were no more likely to impose restrictions on
children’s television use, v
2
(1) ¼2.74, p>.05, and Inter-
net use, v
2
(1) ¼2.09, p>.05, than mothers of boys.
Income level was unrelated to television restriction,
v
2
(2) ¼.49, p>.05, and Internet restriction,
v
2
(2) ¼1.13, p>.05. Differences in maternal TV use
were not found between mothers who did and who
did not impose child television use restriction,
F(1, 87) ¼.56, p>.05. Similarly, differences in maternal
Internet use were not found between mothers who did
and who did not impose child Internet use restriction,
F(1, 86) ¼2.05, p>.05.
Threat Perceptions
Children reported significantly greater societal threat
perception than personal threat perception,
t(89) ¼10.14, p<.001. Differences among personal
threat perceptions (i.e., crime, terrorism, earthquakes,
and hurricanes=floods) were not significant. Among
societal perceptions, children reported greater crime
threat perception than earthquake threat perception,
t(89) ¼2.6, p¼.01, and than hurricane=flood threat
perceptions, t(89) ¼2.01, p<.05; remaining societal
threat differences were nonsignificant. Age was
unrelated to children’s personal threat perceptions
(r¼.06, ns) but was positively related to societal
threat perceptions (r¼.26, p<.05), with reports of
societal threat rising with child age. Boys and girls
reported similar levels of personal, F(1, 88) ¼1.20,
p>.05, and societal threat, F(1, 88) ¼1.64, p>.05.
Children across income levels reported similar levels of
personal, F(2, 84) ¼.58, p>.05, and societal threat,
F(2, 84) ¼1.32, p>.05.
Media Consumption and Threat Perceptions
Hierarchical multiple regressions examined the relative
contributions of media consumption, child anxiety,
TABLE 1
Distribution of Study Variables
Variables M SD
Television Use (hr=week) 15.17 12.61
Internet Use (hr=week) 7.58 11.93
Child Anxiety 34.17 7.42
Parental TV Regulation (max hr=week)
a
10.86 7.36
Parental Internet Regulation (max hr=week)
b
5.17 4.12
Threat Perception Personal Societal Personal
c
Societal
c
Terrorism 1.30 2.82 1.43 1.88
Earthquake 1.22 2.56 1.34 1.88
Hurricane=Flood 1.22 2.74 1.33 1.95
Crime 1.11 3.27 1.18 2.15
Total 4.86 11.39 3.89 5.74
a
Estimate reflects data from subsample of 24 mothers (26.7%) who reported presence of a household rule about
maximum amount of child television viewing allowed.
b
Estimate reflects data from sub-sample of 23 mothers (25.6%) who reported presence of a household rule about
maximum amount of child Internet use allowed.
c
Estimates reflect data from sub-sample of 11 mothers (12.2%) who reported presence of household rules about
maximum amount of child television viewing allowed and maximum amount of child Internet use allowed.
MEDIA USE AND THREAT PERCEPTIONS 625
Downloaded By: [Comer, Jonathan S.] At: 19:20 21 July 2008
child age, and their potential interactions in the predic-
tion of child threat perceptions. Given the identified
age differences in media use, for each set of hierarchi-
cal analyses, age was entered in the first step. To
examine the contribution of media consumption
beyond that accounted for by child anxiety (i.e., self-
selection bias), for each set of hierarchical analyses,
anxiety was entered in the second step, and television
and Internet use were entered simultaneously in the
third step. The products of media use and child anxi-
ety (variables centered) were entered in the fourth step,
to examine interactive models. Significant contribu-
tions of child anxiety and media use in Step 3 (in
the absence of significant contributions of their inter-
action terms in Step 4) support additive models of
anxiety and media use. Significant contributions of
their product terms in Step 4 support interactive (i.e.,
moderation) models of anxiety and media use. Table
2 presents the correlations among predictor variables
and child threat perceptions. Correlations among
predictor variables were not problematic (i.e., all corre-
lations <.60) and Variance Inflation Factor values
were more than 2, confirming that multicollinearity
was not a problem.
In the prediction of personal threat perceptions (see
Table 3), child age did not provide a significant contri-
bution, F(1, 88) ¼.32, p>.05. Entering child anxiety
added a significant contribution F
change
(1, 87) ¼5.17,
p<.05, accounting for an additional 6%of the
variation. Entering media use added a significant contri-
bution to the prediction of personal threat perceptions,
F
change
(2, 85) ¼7.50, p<.001, accounting for an
additional 14%of the variation. After controlling for
child age and anxiety, children’s television use remained
positively correlated with personal threat perception
(r
partial
¼.38, p<.001), such that the greater amount
of television children consume, the greater their reports
of personal threat perception. Internet use did not add a
significant contribution in the prediction of personal
threat perception.
TABLE 3
Multiple Regression Analysis Predicting Personal Threat Perception
Variable Entered B SE(B) bt
Step 1
Child Age .12 .21 .06 .57
Step 2
Child Age .01 .21 .01 .02
Child Anxiety .13 .06 .24 2.27
Step 3
Child Age .19 .21 .10 .92
Child Anxiety .11 .05 .22 2.16
TV Use .12 .03 .40 3.77
Internet Use .01 .04 .03 .26
Step 4
Child Age .18 .20 .09 .90
Child Anxiety .13 .05 .24 2.47
TV Use .13 .03 .42 4.05
Internet Use .03 .04 .09 .86
Child Anxiety TV Use
a
.01 .01 .20 2.00
Child Anxiety Internet Use
a
.01 .01 .09 .89
Note:R
2
¼.01 for Step 1; Adjusted R
2
¼.01 for Step 1; DR
2
¼.06 for Step 2 (p<.05); DR
2
¼.14 for Step 3
(p<.001); DR
2
¼.06 for Step 4 ( p<.05).
a
Variables centered before products were generated.
p<.05. p<.001.
TABLE 2
Correlations Among Media Use, Child Anxiety, and Children’s Threat Perceptions
1234 5
1. Personal Threat
2. Societal Threat .24—— —
3. TV Use .36 .06 —
4. Internet Use .08 .17 .34 ——
5. Anxiety .24.05 .01 .01 —
6. Child Age .06 .26 .25 .30 .24
p<.05, two-tailed. p<.01, two-tailed.
626 COMER ET AL.
Downloaded By: [Comer, Jonathan S.] At: 19:20 21 July 2008
Entering the Media Use Anxiety interactions
added a significant contribution to the prediction of per-
sonal threat perception, F
change
(2, 83) ¼3.15, p<.05,
accounting for an additional 6%of variation. Specifi-
cally, an interactive model of child anxiety and television
use was supported (i.e., significant contribution of Child
Anxiety Television Use product term in Step 4), in
which the contribution of television use in predicting
children’s personal threat perceptions depended on
(i.e., was moderated by) level of child anxiety. Simul-
taneously, the contribution of child anxiety in predicting
children’s personal threat perceptions depended on the
amount of television use they reported. Analyses did
not support an interactive effect for the contributions
of child anxiety and Internet use in the prediction
personal threat perceptions.
In the prediction of societal threat perceptions (see
Table 4), child age (entered in Step 1) provided a signifi-
cant contribution, F(1, 88) ¼6.56, p<.05, with societal
threat perceptions increasing with age. Child anxiety did
not add a significant contribution, F
change
(1, 87) ¼1.39,
p>.05, nor did media use (entered in Step 3), F
change
(2,
85) ¼.38, p>.05. Interactive models of media use and
child anxiety were not supported—that is, the Media
Use Anxiety interactions did not add a significant con-
tribution to the prediction of societal threat perception,
F
change
(2, 83) ¼1.04, p>.05.
Parental Media Regulation and Threat Perceptions
Regarding personal threat perceptions, significant differ-
ences were found neither between children whose mothers
restricted their television use and those whose mothers did
not, F(1, 88) ¼1.58, p>.05, nor between children whose
mothers restricted their Internet use and those whose
mothers did not, F(1, 88) ¼.04, p>.05. Regarding
societal threat perceptions, significant differences were
found neither between children whose mothers restricted
their television use and those whose mothers did not,
F(1, 88) ¼.04, p>.05, nor between children whose
mothers restricted Internet use and those whose mothers
did not, F(1, 88) ¼1.20, p>.05.
DISCUSSION
Consistent with previous work (Gentile & Walsh, 2002;
Roberts et al., 2004), children reported on average more
than 2 hr of daily television use and 1 hr of Internet use,
with media use increasing with age. Within a child sam-
ple, our findings provide partial support for Gerbner’s
(Gerbner & Gross, 1976; Gerbner et al., 1994) notion that
heavy television viewing is related to heightened percep-
tions of risk. Children’s television use was associated with
elevated perceptions of personal (but not societal) vulner-
ability to world threats (i.e., crime, terrorism, earth-
quakes, hurricanes, and floods), and this finding was
particularly strong in youth with high anxiety. Significant
relationships were not found between children’s Internet
use and threat perceptions. Although children’s societal
and personal risk estimations were significantly related
to one another, children reported significantly greater
perceptions of societal risk, and (unlike perceptions of
personal risk) these perceptions were not associated with
TABLE 4
Multiple Regression Analysis Predicting Societal Threat Perception
Variable Entered B SE(B) bt
Step 1
Child Age .76 .30 .26 2.56
Step 2
Child Age .84 .30 .29 2.78
Child Anxiety .10 .08 .13 1.18
Step 3
Child Age .78 .33 .27 2.40
Child Anxiety .09 .08 .12 1.14
TV Use .02 .05 .04 .38
Internet Use .05 .06 .10 .86
Step 4
Child Age .79 .33 .28 2.42
Child Anxiety .12 .08 .14 1.28
TV Use .01 .05 .03 .24
Internet Use .03 .06 .06 .47
Child Anxiety TV Use
a
.01 .01 .12 1.04
Child Anxiety Internet Use
a
.01 .01 .07 .62
Note:R
2
¼.07 for Step 1; Adjusted R
2
¼.06 for Step 1; DR
2
¼.02 for Step 2, ns;DR
2
¼.01 for
Step 3, ns;DR
2
¼.02 for Step 4, ns.
a
Variables centered before products were generated.
p<.05. p<.01.
MEDIA USE AND THREAT PERCEPTIONS 627
Downloaded By: [Comer, Jonathan S.] At: 19:20 21 July 2008
children’s media use. Thus, our findings suggest that the
development of children’s societal and personal risk per-
ceptions may be governed by distinct pathways.
Television usage and anxiety both predicted chil-
dren’s personal threat perceptions, but the contribution
of television use in predicting children’s personal threat
perceptions depended on (i.e., was moderated by) the
level of the child’s anxiety, supporting an interactive
model of television use and child anxiety in accounting
for children’s personal threat perceptions. Simul-
taneously, the strength of child anxiety in predicting
children’s personal threat perceptions was greater for
children who consume greater amounts of television.
Interactive models were not supported when considering
children’s Internet use, or when accounting for chil-
dren’s societal threat perceptions.
Consistent with previous work examining media use
and children’s anxious symptoms (e.g., Pfefferbaum
et al., 2001; Saylor, Cowart, Lipovsky, Jackson, & Finch,
2003; Smith & Wilson, 2002), our study relied on chil-
dren’s retrospective self-reports of media exposure.
Future work may do well to have children report on their
media use via diary methods or personal digital assis-
tants. In addition, whereas our study broadly examined
quantity of media use, future work needs to examine
particular types of media presentations and their relation-
ships with children’s threat perceptions. Televised news
may be particularly problematic for youth (Cantor &
Nathanson, 1996; Smith & Wilson, 2002), and research
has found that news coverage of traumatic events can
result in posttraumatic stress disorder symptomatology
and extended distress (Comer & Kendall, 2007; Otto
et al., 2007; Pfefferbaum et al., 2001; Saylor et al., 2003;
Simons & Silveira, 1994). Moreover, our study findings
are correlational, and as such causal inferences cannot
be made. Our findings controlled for child anxiety, to
account for the potential that anxious youth may both
be vulnerable to high personal threat perceptions and
seek out extended media use, but prospective longitudinal
work is needed. In addition, experimental work in which
children are presented with different forms of media pre-
sentations (e.g., news broadcasts, dramatic program-
ming) will be helpful in this regard. Finally, findings
may not generalize to youth in regions marked by more
frequent and=or destructive disasters and adverse events
(e.g., regions beset by recurrent terrorist attacks). Future
work comparing youth from different regions is needed
to examine whether associations between media use and
threat perceptions are affected by actual probabilities
of the occurrence of adverse events.
Implications for Research, Policy, and Practice
Associations between media use and perceptions of
threat and vulnerability are particularly concerning in
children, given that children are still developing a stable
sense of security about their worlds and may have less
control over the media they consume. Given associa-
tions identified between television use and children’s
personal threat perceptions, it is worrisome that more
than 70%of mothers reported the absence of a house-
hold rule regarding their children’s television use. Con-
tinued empirical efforts are needed to identify the
optimal extent to which parents should regulate youth
television exposure. Although not examined in our
study, future work needs to examine how parents pro-
cess media messages with their children, helping them
distinguish threatening possibilities depicted by the
media from the actual probabilities of their personally
experiencing such events. It may be that promoting
media literacy in youth (Comer & Kendall, 2007;
Nathanson, 1999, 2001) can offset the link between
media use and children’ threat perceptions of personal
vulnerability. In addition, given that a disproportionate
cognitive emphasis upon the possibilities (rather than
probabilities) of unwanted events lies at the heart of
many childhood anxiety disorders (e.g., Daleiden &
Vasey, 1997; Kendall & Treadwell, 2007; Muris & van
Doorn, 2003; Spence, Donovan, & Brechman-
Toussaint, 1999; Treadwell & Kendall, 1996), offsetting
threat perceptions associated media use may benefit
from the extensive gains made in the treatment of child-
hood anxiety disorders (e.g., Barrett, Dadds, & Rapee,
1996; Kendall et al., 1997).
Whereas greater long-term television use predicts
societal threat perceptions in adults (e.g., Romer et al.,
2003), our findings suggest that this may not be the case
with children. Future work should examine children’s
media use and threat perceptions within the context of
cognitive development. Threat perception involves the
ability to mentally represent the future, requiring a
capacity to go beyond that which is observable and con-
sider that which is possible (see Vasey, Crnic, & Carter,
1994). It may be that the ability to mentally represent
future threat to oneself emerges earlier than the ability
to mentally represent future threat to others. Supporting
this notion, analyses found age to be unrelated to
personal threat perceptions but significantly related to
societal threat perceptions, with reports of societal
threat increasing with age.
Moreover, the finding that television use was related
to children’s perceptions of personal vulnerability to
events that, statistically speaking, the majority of youth
in the study had not directly experienced adds to the
increasing body of literature documenting the contri-
butions of verbal information and vicarious learning
in the acquisition of fear and anxiety in youth (see
Field & Lawson, 2003; Field & Schorah, 2007; Field &
Storksen-Coulson, 2007; Gerull & Rapee, 2002). Find-
ings are consistent with evidence suggesting that
628 COMER ET AL.
Downloaded By: [Comer, Jonathan S.] At: 19:20 21 July 2008
conditioning and direct experience with feared stimuli
may not be necessary for the development of children’s
threat perceptions (see Field, 2006).
REFERENCES
Atkin, D. J., Greenberg, B. S., & Baldwin, T. F. (1991). The home
ecology of children’s television viewing: Parental mediation and
the new video environment. Journal of Communication,41, 40–52.
Barrett, P. M., Dadds, M. R., & Rapee, R. M. (1996). Family treat-
ment for childhood anxiety: A controlled trial. Journal of Consulting
and Clinical Psychology,64, 333–342.
Belsky, J. (1980). Child maltreatment: An ecological integration.
American Psychologist,35, 320–335.
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development.
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Cantor, J., & Nathanson, A. I. (1996). Children’s fright reactions to
television news. Journal of Communication,46, 139–152.
Comer, J. S., & Kendall, P. C. (2007). Terrorism: The psychological
impact on youth. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice,14,
179–212.
Croteau, D., & Hoynes, W. (1997). Media=society: Industries, images,
and audiences. Thousand Oaks, CA: Pine Forge Press.
Daleiden, E. L., & Vasey, M. W. (1997). An information-processing
perspective on childhood anxiety. Clinical Psychology Review,17,
407–429.
Dalgleish, T., Taghavi, R., Neshat-Doost, H., Moradi, A., Yule, W., &
Canterbury, R. (1997). Information processing in clinically
depressed and anxious children and adolescents. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry,38, 535–541.
Field, A. P. (2006). Is conditioning a useful framework for understand-
ing the development and treatment of phobias. Clinical Psychology
Review,26, 857–875.
Field, A. P., & Lawson, J. (2003). Fear information and the develop-
ment of fears during childhood: Effects on implicit fear responses
and behavioural avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy,41,
1277–1293.
Field, A. P., & Schorah, H. (2007). The verbal information pathway to
fear and heart rate changes in children. Journal of Child Psychology
and Psychiatry,48, 1088–1093.
Field, A. P., & Storksen-Coulson, H. (2007). The interaction of path-
ways to fear in childhood anxiety: A preliminary study. Behaviour
Research and Therapy,45, 3051–3059.
Gentile, D. A., & Walsh, D. A. (2002). A normative study of family
media habits. Applied Developmental Psychology,23, 157–178.
Gerbner, G., & Gross, L. (1976). Living with television: The violence
profile. Journal of Communication,26, 173–199.
Gerbner, G., Gross, L., Morgan, M., & Signorielli, N. (1994). Growing
up with television: The cultivation perspective. In J. Bryant &
Zillman (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in theory and research
(pp. 17–41). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Gerull, F. C., & Rapee, R. M. (2002). Mother knows best: Effects of
maternal modeling on the acquisition of fear and avoidance behav-
iour in toddlers. Behaviour Research and Therapy,40, 279–287.
Kendall, P. C., Flannery-Schroeder, E., Panichelli-Mindel, S. M.,
Southam-Gerow, M. A., Hennin, A., & Warman, M. (1997). Ther-
apy for youths with anxiety disorders: A second randomized clinical
trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,65, 366–380.
Kendall, P. C., & Treadwell, K. (2007). The role of self-statements as a
mediator in treatment for anxiety-disordered youth. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology,75, 380–389.
Krosnick, J. A., Anand, S. N., & Hartl, S. P. (2003). Psychosocial
predictors of heavy television viewing among preadolescents and
adolescents. Basic and Applied Social Psychology,25, 87–110.
Lynch, M., & Cicchetti, D. (1998). An ecological-transactional analysis
of children and contexts: The longitudinal interplay among child
maltreatment, community violence, and children’s symptomatology.
Development and Psychopathology,10, 235–257.
McLuhan,H. M. (1964). Understanding media.New York: McGraw-Hill.
Morgan, M., & Shanahan, J. (1997). Two decades of cultivation
research: An appraisal and a meta-analysis. Communication
Yearbook,20, 1–45.
Muris, P., Merckelbach, H., Ollendick, T., King, N., & Bogie, N.
(2002). Three traditional and three new childhood anxiety question-
naires: Their reliability and validity in a normal adolescent sample.
Behaviour Research and Therapy,40, 753–772.
Muris, P., & van Doorn, M. (2003). ‘‘Danger is lurking everywhere,
even in parts of a jigsaw puzzle’’: Anxiety-related threat perception
abnormalities in children. Behaviour Change,20, 151–159.
Nathanson, A. I. (1999). Identifying and explaining the relationship
between parental mediation and children’s aggression. Communi-
cation Research,26, 124–143.
Nathanson, A. I. (2001). Factual and evaluative approaches to modify-
ing children’s responses to violent television. Journal of Communi-
cation,54, 331–336.
Otto, M. W., Henin, A., Hirshfeld-Becker, D. R., Pollack, M. H.,
Biederman, J., & Rosenbaum, J. F. (2007). Posttraumatic stress dis-
order symptoms following media exposure to tragic events: Impact
of 9=11 on children at risk for anxiety disorders. Journal of Anxiety
Disorders,21, 888–902.
Peterson, J. L., & Zill, N. (1981). Television viewing in the United
States and children’s intellectual, social, and emotional develop-
ment. Television and Children,2, 21–28.
Pfefferbaum, B., Nixon, S. J., Tivis, R. D., Doughty, D. E., Pynoos,
R. S., Gurwitch, R. H., et al. (2001). Television exposure in children
after a terrorist incident. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological
Processes,64, 202–211.
Roberts, D. F., Henriksen, L., & Foehr, U. G. (2004). Adolescents and
media. In R. M. Lerner & L. Steinberg (Eds.), Handbook of ado-
lescent psychology (2nd ed., pp. 487–521). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Romer, D., Jamieson, K. H., & Aday, S. (2003). Television news
and the cultivation of fear of crime. Journal of Communication,53,
88–104.
Saylor, C. F., Cowart, B. L., Lipovsky, J. A., Jackson, C., & Finch,
A. J. (2003). Media exposure to September 11: Elementary school
students’ experiences and posttraumatic symptoms. American
Behavioral Scientist,46, 1622–1642.
Seligman, L. D., Ollendick, T. H., Langley, A. K., & Baldacci, H. B.
(2004). The utility of measures of child and adolescent anxiety: A
meta-analytic review of the Revised Children’s Manifest Anxiety
Scale, the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children, and the Child
Behavior Checklist. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psy-
chology,33, 557–565.
Silverman, W. K., & Rabian, B. (1999). Rating scales for anxiety and
mood disorders. In D. Shaffer, C. P. Lucas, & J. E. Richters (Eds.),
Diagnostic assessment in child and adolescent psychopathology (pp.
127–166). New York: Guilford.
Simons, D., & Silveira, W. (1994). Post-traumatic stress disorder in
children after television programmes. British Medical Journal,308,
389–390.
Smith, S. J., & Wilson, B. J. (2002). Children’s comprehension of and
fear reactions to television news. Media Psychology,4, 1–26.
Spence, S. H., Donovan, C., & Brechman-Toussaint, M. (1999). Social
skills, social outcomes, and cognitive features of childhood social
phobia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology,108, 211–221.
MEDIA USE AND THREAT PERCEPTIONS 629
Downloaded By: [Comer, Jonathan S.] At: 19:20 21 July 2008
Spielberger, C. D. (1973). Manual for the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory
for Children. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
St. Peters, M., Fitch, M., Huston, A. C., Wright, J. C., & Eakins, D. J.
(1991). Television and families: What do young children watch with
their parents? Child Development,62, 1409–1423.
Treadwell, K. H., & Kendall, P. C. (1996). Self-talk in anxiety disordered
youth: States-of-mind, content specificity, and treatment outcome.
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,64, 941–950.
Vasey, M. W., Crnic, K. A., & Carter, W. G. (1994). Worry in
childhood: A developmental perspective. Cognitive Therapy and
Research,18, 529–549.
Wartella, E., Heintz, K., Aidman, A., & Mazzarella, S. (1990).
Television and beyond: Children’s video media in one community.
Communications Research,17, 45–65.
APPENDIX
Societal and Personal Risk Assessment for Youth
1) What do you think the likelihood is that a terrorist
attack will directly affect you or your family over the
next year?
0123456
It definitely
will not happen
It might
happen
It definitely
will happen
2) What do you think the likelihood is that a terrorist
attack will occur in the US over the next year?
0123456
It definitely
will not happen
It might
happen
It definitely
will happen
3) What do you think the likelihood is that a major
earthquake will directly affect you or your family over
the next year?
0123456
It definitely
will not happen
It might
happen
It definitely
will happen
4) What do you think the likelihood is that a major
earthquake will occur in the US over the next year?
0123456
It definitely
will not happen
It might
happen
It definitely
will happen
5) What do you think the likelihood is that a major hur-
ricane or flood will directly affect you or your family
over the next year?
0123456
It definitely
will not happen
It might
happen
It definitely
will happen
6) What do you think the likelihood is that a major hur-
ricane or flood will occur in the US over the next year?
0123456
It definitely
will not happen
It might
happen
It definitely
will happen
7) What do you think the likelihood is that you or some-
one in your family will be the victim of a bad crime?
0123456
It definitely
will not happen
It might
happen
It definitely
will happen
8) What do you think the likelihood is that crime will
rise in the US over the next year?
0123456
It definitely
will not happen
It might
happen
It definitely
will happen
630 COMER ET AL.
... Another limitation is that most previous studies focused on adult participants except for one enrolling urban-school youths (Weems et al., 2012). As children's media use is reportedly linked to threat perception (Comer et al., 2008), it is crucial to understand how exposure to natural disaster media coverage affects mental health in this population. Here, we analysed data from 159 parents in the two years following the 2011 Great East Japan Earthquake to address these issues. ...
... Similar findings have been reported among urban-school youths (Weems et al., 2012). However, no research has studied preschool children who are generally sensitive to threatening events (Comer et al., 2008). These findings are pertinent to a post-disaster intervention targeting media literacy among parents of children (Comer et al., 2008). ...
... However, no research has studied preschool children who are generally sensitive to threatening events (Comer et al., 2008). These findings are pertinent to a post-disaster intervention targeting media literacy among parents of children (Comer et al., 2008). Promoting media literacy may nullify the link between exposure to natural disaster media coverage and threat perceptions (Comer & Kendall, 2007), preventing subsequent mental health problems. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Exposure to natural disaster media coverage is associated with mental health problems, but its long-term impacts are still unclear. Also, no study has analysed the psychological impact of exposure to natural disaster media coverage among children who are generally sensitive to threatening events. Objective: We aimed to examine how television images of victims after the 2011 Great East Japan Earthquake were associated with mental health among children and their parents. Methods: In 2012, questionnaires for sociodemographic factors were distributed to 2053 families. Parents who provided written consent were contacted in 2013 and invited to provide information on mental health problems (outcome) and retrospectively provide information on television watching at the time of the earthquake (exposure). We used data from 159 parents who completed the survey as the final sample. We used a dichotomous variable to evaluate exposure to media coverage. Multivariable regression was used to examine the association between exposure to television images of victims and mental health, adjusting for potential confounders. Bias-corrected and accelerated bootstrap confidence intervals (CIs) were used. Results: Exposure to television images of victims was significantly associated with worse psychopathology among children (β, 1.51; 95% CI, 0.07–2.96) and greater psychological distress among their parents (β, 1.49; 95% CI, 0.28–2.70). Child psychopathology and parental psychological distress were significantly correlated (r = 0.36, p < .001). Conclusions: Exposure to television images of disaster victims may produce long-term impacts on mental health among children and their parents. To reduce the likelihood of mental health problems associated with disasters, clinicians may recommend reducing exposure to television images of victims.
... In addition, following the Oklahoma City bombing, among those indirectly exposed, emotional reactions to the media coverage were associated with subsequent posttraumatic stress (Pfefferbaum et al., 2003). Indeed, the amount of TV and internet use among children elevated their perception of personal vulnerability to world threats, indicating an impact on overall threat appraisal (Comer, Furr, Beidas, Babyar, & Kendall, 2008). Consequently, based on socialcognitive theory and the extant research, the present study explores the potential mediators of core beliefs, personal and societal threat appraisals, and initial emotional reactions to the media coverage of the AMV event. ...
... Response options range from 0 = It definitely will not happen to 6 = It definitely will happen. The SPRAY showed convergent validity through its significant correlation to anxiety and adequate internal consistency (Comer, Furr, Beidas, Babyar, & Kendall, 2008). Cronbach's α was .83 for personal risk and .87 for societal risk in the current sample. ...
... Consistent with theory and prior research (Bandura, 2001;Comer, Furr, Beidas, Babyar, & Kendall, 2008;Dubow et al., 2007), we found media exposure to AMV influenced personal threat appraisal, which was significantly associated to depression, and approached significance for anxiety. The repeated exposure to media images of mass shootings in the United States can elevate an adolescents' perception that it can happen to them personally. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Traditional and social media coverage of acute mass violence (AMV; e.g., terrorism, mass shootings) create an environment where the possibility of being the victim of AMV is constantly portrayed and this media exposure has been linked to distress among people not directly affected. We explored how initial emotional reactions to media exposure to AMV, threat perception, and core beliefs may mediate the media exposure to current anxiety or depression symptoms relationship. Method: Adolescents (N = 342) in the United States aged 13-17 years old (M = 15.43, SD = 1.29; 71.6% female) completed online surveys asking about time spent watching AMV coverage in the media, initial emotional reactions to the media coverage, threat perception, core beliefs, and current anxiety and depression symptoms. Mediation was tested with PROCESS (Hayes, 2018) for anxiety and depression. Sex and having lived in a community exposed to an AMV event were control variables. Results: Bootstrap confidence intervals (95%) for the unstandardized indirect effects of core beliefs, initial anxious emotional reactions, and personal threat perception based on 5,000 bootstrap samples did not include zero, providing support for mediation. Core beliefs and initial anxious emotional reactions were mediators between time-consuming AMV-related media and current anxiety symptoms. Personal threat perception and initial anxious emotional reactions mediated the relationship between time-consuming AMV-related media and current depression symptoms. Conclusion: The impact of media exposure to AMV on depression and anxiety can be understood through its influence on initial anxious reactions, core beliefs, and threat perception. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
... Research has shown that not all disaster education programs were proved to be effective (Johnson et al., 2014;Toyosawa et al., 2010). Some disaster education programs, if implemented improperly, may increase children's fear/anxiety for disaster by exposing them to more disaster-related information (Comer et al., 2008). Parents are the main executors of family-based disaster education, whose mental health status may influence the effectiveness of family-based disaster education. ...
... Partly consistent with the study hypothesis, results showed that a high level of parental worry could attenuate the significant and negative link between family-based disaster education and young elementary children's internalizing problems. Previous studies have indicated that some disaster education programs may have a negative impact on children's mental health by exposing children in the threat of disaster in an improper way (Comer et al., 2008;Johnson et al., 2014;Toyosawa et al., 2010). During the COVID-19 pandemic, overworried parents may reiterate the threats of the pandemic or take unnecessary protection measures for their children in name of family-based disaster education. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective: Internalizing and externalizing problems are prevalent in disaster-exposed children but few studies have investigated these problems in relation to parental factors. This study examined how parental worry and family-based disaster education related to children’s internalizing and externalizing problems during the outbreak of COVID-19 in China. Method: Parents reported parental worry, family-based disaster education and their children’s (5–8-year-old young elementary schoolchildren [n = 245] and 245 9–13-year-old early adolescents [n = 245]) internalizing and externalizing problems. Results: Data analysis showed that (a) across ages, parental worry related to children’s internalizing and externalizing problems significantly and positively; (b) the significant and negative relationships between family-based disaster education and internalizing and externalizing problems were only supported in young elementary schoolchildren; and (c) high level of parent worry attenuated the negative link between family-based disaster education and young elementary schoolchildren’s internalizing problems. Conclusion: This study expands our knowledge about relationships between parental worry and children’s disaster-related well-being, and highlights the importance of adapting family-based disaster education to different ages. Data suggest that parents of young elementary schoolchildren and early adolescents both should avoid showing excessive worry in front of their children during the pandemic to help reduce their children’s internalizing and externalizing problems. Effective family-based disaster education can mitigate young elementary schoolchildren’s emotional distress and behavioral problems, the effect of which may be maximized if parents can avoid being overly worried. Parents of early adolescents should support their children in acquiring pandemic-related information independently and encourage them to seek support outside the family.
... Wheeler (2015) also inferred that TV viewing affects the psychology of children significantly but the nature of effects depends upon the content which is being watched. Comer et al. (2008) inferred that the more children watch television, the more their psyche is affected. According to Mares and Woodard (2005), prosocial use of media affects child socialization and the type of content affects psychology of children. ...
... The results suggest that due to increased media exposure, child's psychology also developed accordingly. This finding validates H1, and is in line with previous researches by Comer et al. (2008), Kostyrka-Allchorne, Cooper and Simpson (2017), Mares and Woodard (2005), and Wheeler (2015) who reported that media exposure affects the psychology of children significantly. Table 2 further indicates a positive relationship between media exposure and self-concept. ...
Article
Full-text available
This study explores the effect of media exposure on child’s psychology according to parental perceptions. Aggression and self-concept are selected as parameters of children psychology. The combined role of education and occupation of mothers as moderators in relationship between media exposure and children’s psychology are also analyzed. A survey was conducted with 400 parents of school going children aged between 6 to 12 years. The results show positive association of media exposure with psychology, self-concept and aggression among children according to their parents. Furthermore, the education of mother moderates the relation between media exposure and self-concept positively, while mother’s occupation does not moderate this relationship. The results also indicate that mother’s education negatively moderates the relationship between media exposure and aggression among children, however the occupation of mother positively moderates this relation. This study concludes that there is a strong effect of media exposure on children psychology as perceived by their parents. Received: 12 September 2020 / Accepted: 2 December 2020/ Published: 17 January 2021
... Exposure to threat information via media has been found to increase MPQ_69_1_03.indd Page 59 03/05/23 6:08 PM perceived vulnerability (Comer et al., 2008;Smith & Wilson, 2002), and even increase risk of developing anxiety or depressive disorders (Hoven et al., 2005). This impact is even greater on younger children (Otto et al., 2007) and may also contribute to lifelong fears. ...
Article
Animal fears are common, emerging in early childhood and often continuing into adulthood. This study explores the outcomes of positive and negative storybooks about animals on children's attitudes and behaviors. Ninety-six children (ages 4–8 years) were exposed to either negative or positive information about two animals (snakes and frogs) via age-appropriate storybooks, and fear beliefs and avoidance behaviors were then measured. Our results suggest that prior knowledge influences learning and behavior, with children exhibiting more fear towards snakes than frogs, regardless of condition. Accordingly, children who showed fewer fear beliefs were more likely to reach for the animals. In addition, storybook information impacts learning and fear, with children exhibiting more fear in the negative storybook conditions than positive storybook conditions. Storybook information also influenced behavioral avoidance, especially for snakes, with more children reaching for the snake when they received positive information rather than negative information. Additionally, across negative conditions, more children reached for the frog compared to the snake. Finally, parental and child characteristics were associated with more both self-reported fear and observed fear. Implications for parents and educators are discussed.
... Taking the first scenario into consideration, our results suggest that social media use can help gathering and dispersing relevant and useful information, which help the public be more aware of the situation they are faced with. Guided by previous scholarship on situation awareness (42)(43)(44), the current study suggests that exposure to crisis information through social media is positively related with situation awareness which also predict preventive behavior for the public. Previous studies have shown that general risk information has a limited impact on protective behavior, while personalized risk feedback is more likely to trigger preventive intentions and behaviors (45). ...
Article
Full-text available
Background In an outbreak of an infectious disease especially, online media would usually be an important channel for people to get first-hand knowledge and evaluate risks of the specific emergency. Although there has been increasing attention to the effect of social media use during epidemics and outbreaks, relatively little is known about the underlying mechanism by which social media plays a role in people's cognitive, affective and preventive responses. Objective With an objective to advance current knowledge surrounding social media effects on people's cognition, affection and health protective behaviors during epidemics, we aim to examine the associations between social media exposure to COVID-19 risks related information and preventive behavior of the public, and also the role that situation awareness and crisis emotions including anxiety and fear played. Methods An online cross-sectional survey was conducted in China a total of 632 participants were recruited. Measures included exposure to COVID-19 information through social media, situation awareness, anxiety, fear and protective behaviors that participants have taken. We have performed the descriptive statistical analysis, correlation and mediation analysis to test the research hypotheses. Results Findings indicated that situation awareness was positively associated with social media use (B = 0.54, p < 0.001), anxiety (B = 0.95, p < 0.001) and fear (B = 0.87, p < 0.001), and preventive behavior (B = 0.68, p < 0.001). Social media use would also have an indirect effect on anxiety (indirect effect = 0.40; 95% CI = [0.34, 0.46]) and fear (indirect effect = 0.35; 95% CI = [0.29, 0.42]) through situation awareness. The serial mediation effect of situation awareness and fear in the correlation between social media use and preventive behavior has been testified (indirect effect = 0.04, 95% CI = [0.01, 0.08]). Conclusions Social media use might influence the adoption of preventive behaviors through triggering situation awareness and fear. Therefore, health communication regarding COVID-19 prevention should target people with less internet access and low eHealth literacy. Understanding the positive role of negative crisis emotions during outbreaks could also help communicators and policymakers develop appropriate strategies to make people proactive to avoid the remaining health hazard.
Article
Full-text available
Background: Rapid technological developments enable the immediate transmission of armed conflict events through a variety of media channels, inducing mass anxiety, fear, and helplessness. Youth are particularly vulnerable and face new challenges as a result of this exposure. The effects of media exposure to such events on psychological distress and post-traumatic symptoms were examined. Methods: A total of 161 participants aged 13-18 years completed a questionnaire battery that included measures of media exposure to armed conflict events, previous direct exposure to armed conflict events, psychological distress, post-traumatic symptoms, dispositional optimism, and self-mastery. A structural equation model (SEM) approach was employed for data analysis. Results: The extent of media exposure to armed conflict was directly associated with psychological distress and post-traumatic symptoms. Dispositional optimism moderated the association between media exposure and psychological distress, while self-mastery moderated the association between media exposure and post-traumatic symptoms. The effects of the Internet factor of media exposure, which included social media, were particularly disturbing as neither of the resilience factors moderated negative outcomes. Conclusions: The findings suggest that clinical interventions to enhance dispositional optimism and self-mastery as well as other potential resilience factors can protect adolescents from the severe effects of media exposure to violent armed conflict events. Developmental and public health implications related to vulnerabilities and resilience during adolescence are discussed.
Article
Full-text available
U radu su predstavljeni rezultati istraživanja stavova srednjoškolaca u Republici Srbiji o uticaju sistema nacionalne bezbednosti na prevenciju bezbednosnih pretnji. Istraživanje je zasnovano na metodi polustrukturisanog anketnog upitnika, koji je obuhvatio pitanja o uticaju vojske, policije i službi bezbednosti na prevenciju raznorodnih bezbednosnih pretnji. Rezultati istraživanja pokazuju da srednjoškolci percipiraju policiju kao ključnog subjekta sistema nacionalne bezbednosti s obzirom na doprinos prevenciji bezbednosnih pretnji. Nakon policije sledi vojska, kao drugi subjekt prema značaju, dok su službe bezbednosti percipirane kao subjekt koji ima najmanje izražen uticaj na prevenciju bezbednosnih pretnji. Srednjoškolci percipiraju korupciju, medijsku propagandu, narkomaniju i kriminalitet kao preovlađujuće bezbednosne pretnje, koje izuzetno ili u velikoj meri ugrožavaju bezbednost.
Chapter
Early animal-focused fear development can influence the way that children engage with animals and their environment, presenting potential barriers to full and active participation in outdoor educational experiences. The ability to recognise and retain information about dangerous animals holds an evolutionary advantage for humans. Children will demonstrate content bias towards, and retain such ‘danger’ information over time. However, fear responses towards animals can be decreased through positive vicarious counter-conditioning and desensitisation. It is thus crucial to understand which learning experiences may prevent children from fully taking part in outdoor or animal related learning in order to support their full and active participation across environments.
Article
The present study investigated the effects of prior levels of general life stress on young adults’ cognitive and emotional reactions to media coverage of terrorism. University students (n = 94) completed an online baseline survey, after which they were randomly assigned to a terrorism or non-terrorism media exposure condition. Approximately 1–2 weeks later, participants attended an experimental session in which they were shown a real life news clip about a recent terrorist event (terrorism media exposure) or a non-violent societal risk (non-terrorism media exposure). Participants filled out a brief online survey immediately before and after the intervention. Socio-demographic factors and perceived life stress were assessed at baseline. Perceived susceptibility to terrorism, terrorism-related worry, and support for security measures were measured at pre- and post-intervention. Data were analyzed using a series of two-way analyses of covariance (ANCOVA) controlling for pretest scores and relevant socio-demographic factors. Significant interaction effects were found between media exposure and perceived life stress on support for the following security measures: video surveillance, mandatory bag checks at universities, and security patrols at universities during the day. More specifically, participants with lower levels of stress scored similarly on these measures in the terrorism and non-terrorism media exposure groups, whereas among those with higher stress levels, scores were significantly greater in the terrorism compared to non-terrorism media exposure group. Thus, young adults with elevated levels of perceived life stress were more responsive to media coverage of terrorism compared to those with lesser life stress when it comes to a desire for regulatory action. This indicates that higher levels of general life stress may sensitize young people towards the impact of threatening information.
Article
Full-text available
A family-based treatment for childhood anxiety was evaluated. Children (n = 79) aged 7 to 14 who fulfilled diagnostic criteria for separation anxiety, overanxious disorder, or social phobia were randomly allocated to 3 treatment conditions: cognitive–behavioral therapy (CBT), CBT plus family management (CBT + FAM), and waiting list. The effectiveness of the interventions was evaluated at posttreatment and at 6 and 12 months follow-up. The results indicated that across treatment conditions, 69.8% of the children no longer fulfilled diagnostic criteria for an anxiety disorder, compared with 26% of the waiting-list children. At the 12-month follow-up, 70.3% of the children in the CBT group and 95.6% of the children in the CBT + FAM group did not meet criteria. Comparisons of children receiving CBT with those receiving CBT + FAM on self-report measures and clinician ratings indicated added benefits from CBT + FAM treatment. Age and gender interacted with treatment condition, with younger children and female participants responding better to the CBT + FAM condition.
Article
Full-text available
The present study assesses children's reactions to television news. A total of 125 children from two grade levels (K-3rd vs. 4th-6th) were interviewed about their comprehension of and emotional responses to everyday news stories. The results show that older children are more likely to understand, as well as be frightened by, television news than are younger children. Both age groups were able to recall and describe news stories that made them feel upset. However, age-related differences were observed in the types of stories that younger and older children recalled as scary. Finally, there is some evidence that repeated exposure to television news affects children's perceptions of how much crime occurs in distant communities. The findings are consistent with developmental differences in how children process mass media messages.
Article
Full-text available
This study investigated the relationship between childhood anxiety disorders, the valence and content of self-statements, and the impact of treatment on the internal dialogue. Participants (151 8- to 13-year-olds) included 71 youth with anxiety disorders and 80 control participants. Positive and negative self-statements and a states-of-mind (SOM) ratio were examined. Results indicated that the negative self-statements and SOM ratio (but not positive self-statements) of children with anxiety disorders significantly predicted anxiety. Results also indicated that negative (but not positive cognition) and SOM ratio predicted improvement in anxiety after treatment and mediated treatment gains. Results of analyses to explore the content specificity hypothesis were mixed. The impact of negative self-talk on children's anxious symptomatology and favorable treatment outcome is discussed.
Article
This chapter presents a theoretical review and meta-analysis of cultivation research. The authors examine the roots of cultivation analysis, as developed by George Gerbner and colleagues, and review the progress made in cultivation research since its inception in the 1970s. They also review some of the critiques that have been made of cultivation theory over the years and provide their own critical review and responses. They then offer a meta-analysis of empirical findings from 20 years of cultivation research. This meta-analysis shows an average cultivation effect of .09. Much, but not all, of the variation in cultivation findings reported in the literature can be attributed to sampling error alone. Yet, although the authors tested a variety of hypothetical moderator variables, they found no specific moderator variables. The analysis suggests that many theoretical arguments tend to fade into the background when the corpus of cultivation findings is viewed from a meta-analytic perspective.
Article
Children's access to a variety of media sources (primarily broadcast and cable television and videocassette recorders) is surveyed and the extent of content diversity provided by those sources is assessed. The study was conducted in a single community in order to evaluate whether, as the Federal Communications Commission has claimed, new technologies have increased the diversity of available children's programming. Findings indicate that children have access to a number of old and new media, with more than two thirds of the sample living in households that receive cable television and own a videocassette recorder. Over-the-air broadcasting and videocassettes offer little diversity in children's content, but cable enhances children's programming choices substantially.
Article
A survey of 394 parents and children in 2nd through 6th grades was conducted to explore the relations between parental mediation of violent television and children's generalized and TV-induced aggressive tendencies. In addition, explanations for why mediation is related to aggression were sought by exploring a number of intervening variables. It was found that parental active mediation and restrictive mediation were both negatively related to children's generalized and TV-induced aggressive inclinations, whereas parental coviewing was positively related to children's TV-induced aggressive tendencies. The data also revealed that parental mediation works by first influencing either how important children perceive violent TV to be or how much attention they grant this content, which, in turn, influences aggressive tendencies. Hence, parental mediation seems to socialize children into an orientation toward TV that makes them less vulnerable to negative effects.