ArticlePDF Available

Hong Kong children's understanding of television advertising

Taylor & Francis
Journal of Marketing Communications
Authors:

Abstract

This study examines Chinese children's understanding and comprehension of television advertising. A quota sample of 448 children made up of 32 girls and 32 boys from kindergartens and grades 1-6, were personally interviewed in May 1998. The results indicated that children in grade 2 (aged 7-8 years) were beginning to understand what advertising was and were aware of the persuasive intention of television advertising. Over one-third of older children from grade 4 understood that television stations carried advertising for money. Like children in the West, the main reason for liking and disliking commercials depended on their entertainment element. An understanding of television advertising, recall of brands from slogans and comprehension of advertising content were consistently related to the cognitive development of children. Brand recognition from liked and disliked commercials was strong. Comprehension of the key messages of advertising content varied greatly by children's cognitive development and the style of presentation. Ethical issues and public opinions of Hong Kong consumers regarding advertising to children were discussed.
Advertising and children 1
Hong Kong children’s understanding of television advertising
Keywords: television advertising--Hong Kong--children
Dr. Kara Chan
Assistant Professor
Department of Communication Studies
Hong Kong Baptist University
Kowloon Tong, Hong Kong
Fax: (852) 2339-7890
Telephone: (852) 2339-7836
E-mail: karachan@hkbu.edu.hk
Paper being accepted by Journal of Marketing Communications
Dr. Kara K.W. Chan is Associate Professor in the Department of Communication Studies,
Hong Kong Baptist University. She worked in the advertising and public relations profession
and as a statistician for the Hong Kong Government. She now actively involves in research
on Hong Kong and China’s mass communication, advertising, consumer behaviour and
environmental studies. She has published at the International Journal of Advertising, Asian
Journal of Communication, Gazette and the Environmentalist.
This project is funded by the Faculty research grant of Hong Kong Baptist University.
Running head: Advertising and children
October 30, 1998
R05child\jpaper1.doc
Advertising and children 2
Hong Kong children’s
understanding of television advertising
keywords: children, television advertising, cognitive development
Abstract
This study examines Chinese children’s understanding and comprehension of television
advertising. A quota sample of four hundred and forty-eight children, 32 girls and 32 boys
from kindergartens, grade 1 to six, were personal-interviewed in May 1998. Results indicated
that children from grade two (aged seven to eight) began to know what advertising was and
were aware of the persuasive intention of television advertising. Over one third of older
children from grade four understood that television stations carried advertising for money.
Like children in the West, the main reason for liking and disliking of commercial depended on
its entertainment element. Understanding of television advertising, recall of brands from
slogans and comprehension of advertising content were consistently related to cognitive
development of children. Brand recognition from liked and disliked commercials was strong.
Comprehension of key messages of advertising content varied greatly by children’s cognitive
development and the style of presentation. Ethical issue and public opinion of Hong Kong
consumers regarding to advertising to children were discussed.
(166 words)
Advertising and children 3
INTRODUCTION
Advertising today penetrates into the life of every person, including children. The
children’s market is important to advertisers because of the enormous purchasing power of
the children and their parents. A survey of 2,400 children aged 7 to 12 in six countries,
including China, Japan, France, the United Kingdom, Germany and the United States,
indicated that their estimated annual spending power ranged from 1.7 billion US dollars for
Germany to 11.3 billion US dollars for United States. Survey results also indicated active
participation from children in making family purchase decisions in a number of product
categories (Carey, Zhao, Chiaramonte and Eden, 1997). The newly emerging middle classes,
high populations and one-child policies in some Asian countries mean that these ‘little
emperors’ are giving more and more influence over where the family dollar is spent. Research
has suggested that children in China have been quick to make demands and exercise pester
power. They are often the first to learn of new products from friends or from television and
introduce these products to the household (AC Nielsen, 1997).
Children in Hong Kong are exposed to a large amount of advertising, especially
through television advertising. According to a weekly AC Nielsen’s television rating report,
the average rating of TVB-Jade, the dominant Chinese channel, from 7 a.m. to 1 a.m. on a
school day in March 1998 for children 4 to 14 was 12 rating points (equivalent to an audience
size of 103,000). Children watched a lot more television during school holidays. The average
rating of TVB-Jade on an Easter holiday was 17 rating points (40 percent more audience than
a typical school day). A child spending four hours per day watching television may be
exposed to 15,000 commercials every year. What do children in Hong Kong know about
television advertising and what do they learn from it? This study attempts to examine
children’s response to television advertising on aspects relating to the communication process.
The study adopts Piaget’s (1970) theory of cognitive development. The theory identifies
Advertising and children 4
distinct stages of cognitive development and postulates that children would manifest
differences in the ways they select, evaluate, and use information. Children’s responses to
television advertising will therefore be analyzed by school year.
The study is of major interest to both marketers and to public policy officials.
Marketers are keen to know if their advertising is effective while policy makers are concerned
with protecting the interests of the child. The study is particularly important as there is a
paucity of empirical evidence on the topic within the context of children from Asian cultures.
This is surprising at a time when marketing academics make continuous pleas for international
studies on marketing concepts and consumer behaviors (Bradley, 1987; Lee and Green,
1991). As Mainland China is adopting a ‘one-child’ family planning policy, the children have
become the focus of the attention of the parents and grandparents. Children are believed to
exert an even greater influence on the family’s purchase decisions. The study will serve as a
benchmark study for Hong Kong as well as a protocol for future studies in China.
The objectives of this study are:
a) to study children’s ability to understand the purpose of television advertising;
b) to examine children’s attention, recall and comprehension of television advertising;
c) to investigate children’s liking and disliking of television advertising, and
d) to investigate whether the above are related to stages of cognitive development.
The preceding literature review indicates that children’s responses to television
advertising in Western societies consistently depend on age or school year of the respondents.
This study will provide empirical data for comparison with an Eastern society.
LITERATURE REVIEW
Advertising and children 5
In the Western perspectives of child development, Piaget’s (1970) theory of cognitive
development has had great impact on research related to the communication process of
advertising to children. The theory proposes that a child’s ability to think and to reason
progresses through a series of distinct stages. Each stage has its own unique characterization,
yet could vary considerably depending on intelligence, cultural background and social
economic factors. Piaget’s (1970) proposes four stages of cognitive development that can be
summarized as follows:
the sensori-motor stage (0 to 2 years): the behaviour is primarily motor and the child does
not manifest conceptual thinking.
the pre-operational stage (2 to 7 years): the child’s thinking about objects and ideas is
poorly organized and only dominant features are used for making judgements.
the concrete operations stage (7 to 11 years): the child can think conceptually and
organize ideas in a coherent and stable manner.
the formal operations stage (11+ years): the child can think in abstract terms.
Many studies on children’s communication process of television advertising have been
based on Piaget’s cognitive development theory. Other approaches have also emerged to
provide detailed explanation of Piaget’s theory using concepts about information processing
of short-term and long-term memory of commercial information (Roedder, 1981).
Studies generally have indicated that children’s ability to distinguish programs from
commercials increased with age (Meringoff and Lesser, 1980; Robertson and Rossiter, 1974)
and location of the commercial relative to the program (Hoy, Young and Mowen, 1986).
Children’s comprehension of television advertising and its persuasive intent also increased
with age (Blosser and Roberts, 1985; Levin, Petros and Petrella, 1982; Rubin, 1974;
Advertising and children 6
Wackman, Wartella and Ward, 1977; Ward, 1972;). In a British survey, 80 percent of the
children replied that the main reason for television advertising was because ‘the advertisers
want to sell you something’. The response did not differ by age or by social class, but they
differed by sex. Girls were more likely to know the purpose of advertising than boys
(Greenberg, Fazal and Wober, 1986). Research also suggests that by the age of nine, children
showed marked improvement in understanding ambiguous wording, humour and imagery
found in advertisements (Belk, Mayer and Driscoll, 1984; Brown and Bryant, 1983; Nippold,
Cuyler and Braunbeck-Price, 1988).
Decreased attention during commercials and selective viewing of commercials with
age has been found in many studies (Sheikh, Prasad and Rao, 1974; Ward, 1972; Ward,
Levingson and Wackman, 1972; Zuckerman, Ziegler and Stevenson, 1978). Attention to TV
commercials depended on personal factors and stimulus factors. Personal factors include
parental and peer influence, the level of motivation and attitudes toward commercials.
Stimulus factors include the nature of the TV program, the content of the commercial and the
product advertised (McNeal, 1987).
Other children also believed that advertising was less truthful (Gaineo and Easserman,
1981; Robertson and Rossiter, 1974; Ward, Wackman and Wartella, 1977). All these
observations are generally attributed to greater cognitive maturity and increased experience
with the medium. Van Evra (1995) argued that children become less likely to be persuaded
with age because they use other sources for the information they need and depend less on
television advertising. The commercials therefore become less personally relevant to the
child.
Several studies have reported that children, even the younger ones, were able to encode
and subsequently remember commercials in terms of their component attributes such as
premiums, product symbols and brand names (Gianinno and Zuckerman, 1977; Rubin, 1972;
Advertising and children 7
Shrimp, Dyer and Divita, 1976). Children recalled premium offers more than attributes of a
product, and premium played an important role with younger children in choosing a product
(Comstock and Paik, 1991).
From the advertisers’ perspective, a successful marketing communication campaign has
to be a memorable one because the opportunity to buy a product following its appearance in
mass media is usually delayed. Therefore, it is important to know whether the viewers
understand the message communication through the advertising and what information is
stored and subsequently retrieved from the memory about the advertised brands (Gunter and
McAleer, 1997).
Most of the research literature on advertising and children is based on research
conducted in Western societies, and there are very few comparable studies from Asian
countries. When referring to Chinese perspectives toward child development, Confucianism is
one Chinese ideology that has been widely investigated by Chinese and Western researchers.
Some important characteristics include an emphasis on moralistic orientation toward children
(Ekblad, 1986); filial piety (Hsu, 1981; Kelly and Tseng, 1992); self-fulfillment (Kelly and
Tseng, 1992; Suzuki, 1980); good manners, and the importance of education (Chiu, 1987;
Ekblad, 1986; Ho, 1989). The implications of all these studies suggests that Chinese parents
tend to be more concerned about the moral behaviours in the commercials, and they will exert
more control over their children’s behaviours.
To conclude, the review of literature shows that understanding the purpose of television
advertising and comprehension of advertising content depend on age or school year of the
children. Children in Asian societies are more likely to pay attention to the moral behaviours
in the advertising content.
RESEARCH METHOD
Advertising and children 8
Six advertising and public relations undergraduate students of Hong Kong Baptist
University were recruited and, through personal sources, interviewed four hundred and forty-
eight Chinese children from five to twelve years old in Hong Kong. The children were from a
quota sample of equal number of boys and girls for each school year from kindergarten and
grade one to grade six. Interviewers were trained on the purpose of the study, the structure
of the interview and the skills in soliciting responses. Interviews were conducted at public
libraries, churches, restaurants and parks near school areas in May 1998. Each interview took
about twenty minutes. Efforts were made to minimize interruptions and intrusions by other
family members or friends present. The interviews started with open-ended questions about
what advertising is, the purpose of television advertising, which commercials they like and
dislike and their reasons. Respondents were asked to recall the brands that used specific
slogans. Three slogans were taken from three food commercials. The interviewers then
presented storyboards of four commercials that were telecast in prime times (7 to 10 p.m.)
during the month before the interviews were conducted. The four commercials were selected
to represent products that could be easily understood by children (such as chocolate and tea),
products that were difficult to understand (taxi using gas fuel) and government’s pro-social
messages (a public service announcement about equal right among females and males). These
commercials were selected from a pool of about 100 commercials broadcast in prime time
programmes on two Chinese channels a month before the conduction of the interviews. The
author and one faculty member of the Department of Communication Studies selected eight
commercials of different product categories. They were then tested by personal interview
with five children on their understanding using open-ended questions. The most and the least
easily understood ones were selected. Two other commercials with medium comprehension
responses were selected. Respondents were required to tell what were being said in the
commercials. The interviews were recorded and transcribed. The interviews were coded by a
research assistant and were cross checked by the author. A coding menu was prepared for
Advertising and children 9
closed-ended questions. For open-ended questions, a coding menu was developed after
reviewing all the responses.
FINDINGS
Understanding of television advertising
Children were prompted about their awareness and knowledge about television
advertising. The interviewer mentioned that ‘when we are watching television, sometimes the
program stops and there are other messages coming up’. Children were asked what these are,
what are their purposes and why the television stations carry such messages. Table 1 to Table
3 summarize their responses. Results of the three tables consistently showed a sharp increase
in awareness of advertising, understanding of the selling intent, and programme sponsorship
from grade two onward. The second grade (or equivalent to age eight) was found to be an
important watershed. Younger children below the second grade had difficulty verbalizing
what constituted a commercial. Starting from the second grade, over half of the children
knew that commercials helped to promote products. A small percentage of the respondents
could spontaneously recognize the selling intent of the commercials. In response to the
question ‘what do commercials want you to do’, a much higher number of younger children
showed recognition of the selling intent of commercials. In response to the question ‘why
television stations carry such messages’, less than one quarter of the respondents were able to
identify the fact that advertising paid for free programming. A higher proportion of children
from grade four knew the institutional function of television advertising. Close to half of the
children at grade six (aged eleven to twelve) understood that the television station broadcast
commercials for money. Some children viewed television advertising as part of the
entertainment offered by the television station. They thought that TV stations broadcast
commercials in order to test whether the commercials were funny and to attract audiences.
While understanding the purpose of advertising generally increased with school year (or age),
Advertising and children 10
children in grade five and six were no better than the children in grade three and four on all
three questions.
[TABLE ONE ABOUT HERE ]
[TABLE TWO ABOUT HERE ]
[TABLE THREE ABOUT HERE ]
Liked and disliked commercials
Children were asked to recall their favourite TV commercials and the commercial they
disliked the most. Not all children were able to mention commercials that they liked or
disliked. Older children were more likely to talk about commercials they liked and disliked.
Altogether 222 and 115 commercials were mentioned as the most favourite and the most
disliked commercials respectively. Children were more ready to recall liked than disliked
commercials. This may indicate that children are less critical about television advertising.
Table 4 summarizes product categories of commercials cited as most liked by children.
[TABLE FOUR ABOUT HERE ]
Results indicated that food and drink, toys and mobile phone commercials were most
frequently cited as commercials they liked. These three product categories accounted for
nearly eighty percent of the total number of recalls. Older children (fifth and sixth graders)
lost interest in toy commercials and developed a marked appreciation for mobile phone and
sports goods commercials. Table 5 summarizes the reasons for liking of commercials.
Children said they liked these commercials because the commercials were interesting and
funny, they liked the products, or they liked the celebrities used. Older children liked
commercials using celebrities. This accounted for their high appreciation of mobile phone
commercials that used movie stars and pop singers as endorsers. Over half of the respondents
Advertising and children 11
at all school years mentioned funny and interesting as their reasons for liking those
commercials. Many children cited McDonalds, Kinder chocolate and Yakult drink
commercials as their favourite commercials because the story lines were cute and entertaining.
Children liked commercials with cartoon figures, funny story or their favourite movie/TV
stars. In responding to this question, younger children were particularly prone to confuse
these commercials with the products. For example, children said they liked programme
announcement because they liked to watch that particular programme.
[TABLE FIVE ABOUT HERE ]
Table 6 summarizes product categories of 116 most disliked commercials. Mobile
phones, food and drink commercials, public service announcements, and supermarket
commercials were most frequently cited. These four categories accounted for fifty-five
percent of the total number of recalls. Table 7 summarizes the reasons for disliking these
commercials. The children disliked these commercials because they did not understand them,
they found them boring or horrifying, or they disliked the products (especially for liquor and
female personal products). Respondents found some public service announcement
advertisements terrifying. For example, an anti-drug commercial featuring distorted images
after a young man taking drugs, a commercial featuring a television set dropped from a high
level that landed on and hurt a group of children playing, and a commercial about a fire in the
household.
Some children disliked commercials that were exaggerated and false. Some children
did not like certain commercials because of ‘incorrect’ behaviours. For example, a nine-year
old girl said she did not like any beer commercials because they contained pornography. An
eleven years old girl did not like a Yakult drink commercial featuring a young boy showing
passion for a little girl because ‘love affairs is no good at that age’. Children also learned that
anti-social behaviours, such as shoplifting and driving under the influence of alcohol, were not
Advertising and children 12
acceptable from the public service announcements. This indicates that children are able to
evaluate television advertising in the light of normative lessons learned elsewhere. Older
children criticized certain commercials for the use of celebrities that they did not like. An
interesting observation was that celebrities polarized reactions from fans and non-fans. For
example, many children who liked the mobile phone commercial featuring pop singer Leon
Lai disliked another mobile phone commercial featuring singer Aaron Kwok.
[TABLE SIX ABOUT HERE ]
[TABLE SEVEN ABOUT HERE ]
For the 222 most liked commercials, a majority of respondents (85 percent)
spontaneously recalled the brand names. But for the 115 most disliked commercials, nearly
sixty percent mentioned the brand names. Results indicated that brand recognition was much
stronger for brands liked, than disliked.
[TABLE SEVEN ABOUT HERE ]
Recall of brand names from slogans
Children were prompted to recall brand names for three slogans and the results were
summarized in Table 8. The first slogan was for the Kinder chocolate commercials targeted at
mothers with young children. The second slogan was for the Nestle milk powder commercials
targeted at children aged seven to twelve. The slogan has been used for nearly fourteen years.
The third slogan was for the Maltesers chocolate commercial targeted at youngsters. Results
indicated that the overall brand recall ranged from 18 to 32 percent. Chi-square analysis
examined whether recall and comprehension of television advertising relate to school year.
All chi-square values were significant, indicating that comprehension was consistently related
to school year or age. Children in kindergartens and first graders performed poorly while
second and third graders performed very well. A small proportion of respondents recalled
Advertising and children 13
either an alternate brand or a different product category. The target audiences of the
commercials performed better than other groups. This suggests that children learn more from
commercials that use models from their age groups.
[TABLE EIGHT ABOUT HERE ]
Comprehension of television advertising
Children were presented with storyboards of four selected commercials and they were
asked to explain what the commercials said. Results in Table 9 indicated that there was a
significant difference in respondents’ comprehension of television advertising. The overall
proportion of children who could give the key messages ranged from 27 percent to 63
percent. All chi-square values were found significant, indicating that comprehension was
consistently related to school year or age. Children in kindergartens and grade one found
difficulty in identifying key messages. They seemed to be preoccupied with what a
commercial should tell or what they were interested to know. In both the chocolate and tea
commercials, a lot of children mentioned that the commercial was about the good taste (coded
as partial/wrong understanding). They failed to recognize that there were a variety of selling
points for food. Children’s comprehension was clearly limited by personal experience. The
commercial about taxi using gas fuel caused the most confusion, because driving was
unfamiliar to them. Many children mixed up public service announcements advocating similar
issues, including the use of unleaded fuel or turning off the engine when the car stopped.
Surprisingly, pro-social messages communicated well even to younger children when they
were presented in a ‘children-friendly’ way. The public service announcement on ‘human
right’ gave some specificity to the idea of equal rights in an animated form featuring animated
boy and girl characters. After a voice-over that everyone had the right to choose his or her
occupation, the girl said that she wanted to be a doctor. The boy rejected her idea and the
voice over asserted equal rights. The commercial provided a concrete example of what a
violation of right represents.
Advertising and children 14
DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSION
The result on children’s cognitive response to television advertising obtained in this
study was similar to previous studies. Grade-related or age-related changes in understanding
were consistently found. A remarkable increase in understanding what advertising is, and the
purpose of television advertising at grade two (aged seven to eight) was also consistent with
Piaget’s (1970) proposed stage on concrete operations. The children entering this stage of
cognitive development were able to think conceptually, and organized the selling idea in a
more coherent manner. This indicates that Piaget’s (1970) theory of cognitive development
finds empirical support in the Chinese society. However, the apparent growth in
understanding of television advertising has to be weighted against the relative abilities of
younger and older children to verbally express themselves, as well as their abilities to
articulate the difference between programmes and commercials. Answers to three questions
‘what television advertising is’, ‘what is its purpose’ and ‘why TV stations carry these
messages’ revealed a range of responses to different levels of understanding of television
advertising. Many younger children who found difficulty expressing what a commercial is
could still identify that the commercials wanted them to try or to buy things. A higher
proportion of children from grade four (aged nine to ten) could recognize the institutional
function of television advertising. According to Piaget’s (1970) classification system, children
at these ages begin to enter the formal operations stage where they can think in more abstract
terms. The findings have public policy implication that younger children need greater
protection against persuasive marketing communication in the mass media. For children over
eight, they have probably developed a cognitive defense and understand the selling intent of
television advertising.
The findings that children liked food and beverages commercials were similar to that
obtained by Ward (1972) and Yavas and Abdul-Gader (1993). This is probably because
Advertising and children 15
children are more familiar and experienced with the product category. Fun and enjoyment
was the main reason for liking a commercial. However, there were great differences in the
reasons for disliking commercials. Hong Kong children frequently mentioned ‘product’ and
‘incorrect behaviours’ as reasons. This indicated that Chinese children placed more emphasis
on the moralistic orientation of commercials than Western children. They held stronger good
and bad beliefs about product nature as well as social behaviours. Compared with findings of
Ward (1972) and Yavas and Abdul-Gader (1993), Hong Kong children more frequently
mentioned ‘celebrity/personality’ as a reason for both liking and disliking. This suggested that
use of ‘personal’ or ‘social’ influence appealed more to Chinese children than Western
children. This may also be due to the heavy use of celebrity commercials in Hong Kong.
Brand recognition for liked and disliked commercials were strong among children.
Commercials enjoyed and disliked by children are more likely to be remembered. However,
the influence of the brand information on purchase intention and purchase behaviour needs
further investigation. Advertisers also need to recognize the trade-offs for using a celebrity.
What appeals to one group likely drives away another group.
Overall brand recall from slogans was low, reconfirming previous findings that brand
names were difficult to remember. Slogans help little in brand recognition. Advertisers
should consider other strategies to enhance brand recognition such as incorporating brand
name in slogans and package recognition.
There has been little research on how well children understand the key messages of
television advertising. Findings of this study revealed that comprehension of advertising
content varied greatly by children’s cognitive development and the style of presentation.
Using simple dialogue, concrete examples and animated figures, children demonstrated fairly
good understanding of pro-social messages. This finding should be encouraging for
government officials and educators. The government should produce more campaigns to
Advertising and children 16
cultivate pro-social behaviours and to protect children’s interest, of course in a children-
friendly way. For example, younger children can learn how to respond to sexual harassment
through a government publicity campaign.
Many critics and parents fear that children are particularly susceptible to commercial
appeals because young viewers lack the necessary cognitive skills to defend themselves
against what are often highly attractive and persuasive messages (Choate, 1975). Other
undesirable consequences listed by the critics are the creation of parent-child conflict (Atkin,
1975) and disappointment when children are denied their requests (Robertson and Rossiter,
1974). The Broadcasting Authority regulates television advertising in Hong Kong. As a
former colony of the United Kingdom, the code of practice on advertising standards in Hong
Kong resembles that of the United Kingdom. It states that ‘no product or service may be
advertised in association with a program intended for children or which large numbers of
children are likely to see, which might result in harm to them physically, mentally or morally,
and no method of advertising may be employed which takes advantage of the natural credulity
and sense of loyalty of children’ (Broadcasting Authority, 1993). Hong Kong consumers
seem to be quite satisfied with the overall television advertising standard, as the number of
complaints is extremely small. The 6.6 million population only made an average of 378
complaints every year on television advertising in the past five years (Broadcasting Authority,
1998). Consumers’ reaction did influence advertisers’ decision. A McDonalds commercial
received 46 complaints shortly after its debut, after it seemed to encourage children telling
lies. As a result, the client pulled out the commercial in question before the Broadcasting
Authority investigated the complaints. In a quota sampling survey of 691 Hong Kong
consumers, Chan and Ruidl (1996) found that 47 percent of the respondents did not agree that
television advertising should be banned on children’s programme (vs. 32 percent held neutral
position and 21 percent agreed). In the same survey, 61 percent of the respondents agreed
that the content of television advertising should be more closely regulated by the government
Advertising and children 17
(Chan and Ruidl, 1996). This indicates that Hong Kong consumers prefer a tighter control of
advertising messages than complete banning to advertising to children.
To conclude, a majority of Hong Kong children from grade two knew what advertising
was and developed appreciation of television commercials. Older children were aware of the
selling intention as well as the institutional function of television advertising. Understanding
of television advertising, recall of brands from slogans and comprehension of advertising
content were clearly related to cognitive development of children. Advertisers targeting at
children should make the commercials funny and interesting so that children will like them and
remember their brands.
Advertising and children 18
Table 1. Understanding what television advertising is by school year
School Year (%)
Kind G1 G2 G3 G4 G5 G6 Total
n=64 64 64 64 64 64 64 448
Low 3 14 14 3 0 11 9 8
Medium 6 14 53 55 67 63 67 46
High 3 13 3 8 13 6 3 7
Don’t know 88 59 30 34 20 20 20 39
Total 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100
Notes: Low: name specific products, part of programmes, interrupts programmes; Medium:
advertise things, inform people about things to buy; High: get people to buy, sponsor
programmes
Table 2. Understanding the purpose of television advertising by school year
School Year (%)
Kind G1 G2 G3 G4 G5 G6 Total
n=64 64 64 64 64 64 64 448
Introduce product,
entertainment
11 6 17 5 5 5 5 8
Make people buy
things, promote
products
31 44 61 78 78 66 61 60
Sponsor programmes 0 0 0 0 2 0 0 0
Don’t know 58 50 22 17 16 30 34 32
Total 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100
Table 3. Understanding why television stations carry advertising by school year
Year (%)
Kind P1 P2 P3 P4 P5 P6 Total
n=64 64 64 64 64 64 64 448
Introduce product,
entertainment
9 3 13 3 9 11 0 7
Make people buy
things, promote
products
13 13 25 27 20 23 16 19
Sponsor programs 0 6 9 19 36 36 47 22
Don’t know/others* 78 78 53 52 34 30 38 52
Total 100 100 100 100 100 100 100 100
‘Others’ include to attract audience (n=4), to test products (3), to test commercials (3), to
educate (3), to give audience a break (1)
Advertising and children 19
Table 4. Product categories of commercials liked by school class (total mentions=222)
Year (%)
Kind,G1 G2-3 G4-6 Total
total mention 42 81 99 222
Food and drink 57 63 52 57
Toys 14 15 8 12
Mobile phones 7 3 12 8
Restaurants and fast food shops 2 12 3 6
Supermarkets 0 2 6 4
Programme announcements 7 1 3 3
Electrical appliances 5 1 3 3
Sports goods 2 0 6 3
Movie and entertainment 0 0 2 1
Public Transportation 0 0 3 1
Public services announcements 3 0 0 1
Property 0 0 1 1
Bank 0 0 1 0
Car and related 2 0 0 0
Like all 0 1 0 0
100 100 100 100
Table 5. Reasons for liking commercials by school class (total mentions=212)
Year (%)
Kind,G1 G2-3 G4-6 Total
total mention 39 76 97 212
Entertainment 62 55 56 57
Beauty 0 0 4 2
Product 18 34 16 23
Celebrity/character 18 11 24 18
Others 3 0 0 0
Total mentions 100 100 100 100
Advertising and children 20
Table 6. Product categories of commercials disliked by school class (total mentions=115)
Year (%)
Kind,G1 G2-3 G4-6 Total
total mention 17 40 58 115
Mobile phones 0 18 24 18
Food and drink 47 13 12 17
Public services announcements 6 13 9 10
Supermarkets 6 15 7 10
Toys 6 13 3 7
Property 0 8 5 5
Programme announcements 12 0 5 4
Alcohol & cigarette 0 8 3 4
Restaurants and fast food shops 6 0 3 3
Clothing 0 3 5 3
Female personal products 0 3 3 3
Medicine 0 0 7 3
Bank 0 0 7 3
Electrical appliances 12 0 0 2
Public Transportation 0 5 0 2
Movie and entertainment 0 0 2 1
Insurance 0 3 0 1
Sports goods 6 0 0 1
Hotel 0 0 2 1
Car and related 0 0 2 1
Dislike all 0 3 0 1
Total mentions 100 100 100 100
Table 7. Reasons for disliking commercials by school class (total mentions=116)
Year (%)
Kind,G1 G2-3 G4-6 Total
20 38 58 116
Entertainment 35 34 47 41
Beauty 20 11 19 16
Product 20 34 14 22
Celebrity/character 20 5 10 10
Not true 0 5 7 5
Just don't like 0 3 0 1
Incorrect behaviour 5 8 3 5
Total mentions 100 100 100 100
Advertising and children 21
Table 8. Recall of brand names from slogans by school year
Year (%)
Kind,G1 G2-3 G4-6 Total
Slogan 1: No more worry for mother (Kinder
chocolate)
Correct brand recall 15 44 35 32
Wrong recall 5 5 7 6
Don't know 80 52 58 62
Chi-square=28 (sign. At 0.0001)
Slogan 2: A cow reared on the 15th floor (Nestle milk
powder)
Correct brand recall 9 30 40 28
Wrong recall 2 6 1 3
Don't know 89 64 59 69
Chi-square=44 (sign. At 0.0001)
Slogan 3: More than one way of enjoyment
(Maltesers chocolate)
Correct brand recall 4 21 26 18
Wrong recall 13 12 8 10
Don't know 84 67 66 71
Chi-square=27 (sign. At 0.0001)
Advertising and children 22
Table 9. Comprehension of television advertising by school year
Year (%)
Kind,P1 P2-3 P4-6 Total
Kinder chocolate
Full understanding 45 72 70 63
Partial/wrong understanding 37 24 29 30
Don't know 19 4 1 7
Chi-square=51 (sign. At 0.0001)
Lipton 3 in 1 tea
Full understanding 14 41 57 40
Partial/wrong understanding 50 39 32 39
Don't know 36 20 10 20
Chi-square=66 (sign. At 0.0001)
Taxi using gas fuel
Full understanding 13 17 44 27
Partial/wrong understanding 32 55 34 40
Don't know 55 28 22 33
Chi-square=72 (sign. At 0.0001)
Equal right
Full understanding 27 70 84 63
Partial/wrong understanding 34 12 5 15
Don't know 40 19 11 22
Chi-square=114 (sign. At 0.0001)
Note: Responses were coded as full understanding if they contained the key messages
(Kinder chocolate: cat wants to get the chocolate; Lipton tea: refresh or relief from pressure;
taxi using gas fuel: the fuel produces less smoke; equal right: girls can do the job that she
like).
Advertising and children 23
References:
AC Nielsen (1997) SRG News, Issue 84, July, 1-2.
Atkin, C.K. (1975) Effects of television advertising on children. E. Lansing, MI: Michigan
State University Press.
Belk, R., Mayer, R. and Driscoll, A. (1984) Children’s recognition of consumption
symbolism in children’s products. Journal of Consumer Research, 10, 386-397.
Blosser, B.J. and Roberts, D.F. (1985). Age differences in children’s perceptions of message
intent. Communication Research, 12(4), 455-484.
Bradley, M.F. (1987) Nature and significance of international marketing: A review. Journal
of Business Research, 15:June, 52-56.
Broadcasting Authority (1993). Television Code of Practice on Advertising Standards, Hong
Kong: Government Printer.
Broadcasting Authority (1998). Letter dated 8 September in response to the author’s written
enquiry.
Brown, D. and Bryant, J. (1983) Humour in the mass media, In McGhee P.E. and Goldstein,
J.H. (eds), Handbook of Humour Research, vol.2, Applied Studies, New York:
Stringer-Verlag.
Carey, G., Zhao, X., Chiaramonte, J. and Eden, D. (1997) Is there one global village for our
future generation? Talking to 7-12 year olds around the world. Marketing and
Research Today. February, 12-16.
Chan, K. and Ruidl, R. (1996) Predicting attitudes toward television advertising: The view
from Hong Kong. 20th IAMCR Scientific Conference and General Assembly,
August 18-22, Sydney, Australia.
Chiu, L. H. (1987) Child-rearing attitudes of Chinese, Chinese-American, and Anglo-
American mothers. International Journal of Psychology, 22: 409-419.
Choate, R.B. (1975) Petition of the Council on children, media and merchandising to issue a
trade regulation rule governing the private regulation of children’s television
advertising, filed before the Federation Trade Commission, Washington, DC.
Comstock, G. and Paik, H. (1991) Television and the American Child. San Diego, CA:
Academic Press.
Ekblad, S. (1986) Relationships between child-rearing practices and primary school children’s
functional adjustment in the People’s Republic of China. Scandinavian Journal of
Psychology, 21: 697-715.
Gaines, L. and Esserman, J.F. (1981) A quantitative study of young children’s comprehension
of television programmes and commercials, In J.F. Esserman (ed.), Television
Advertising and Children, New York: Child Research Service.
Advertising and children 24
Gianinno, L. J. and Zuckerman, P.A. (1977) Measuring children’s responses to television
advertising. Unpublished paper with a summary version appearing in C. Leavitt (ed.),
Proceedings: American Psychological Association Revision 23, 85th Annual Convention,
San Francisco, August.
Greenberg, B.S., Fazal, S. and Wober, M. (1986) Children’s views on advertising. London:
Independent Broadcasting Authority, Research Report.
Gunter, B. and McAleer J. (1997) Children and Television, second edition, London:
Routledge.
Hoy, M.G.; Young, C.E. and Mowen, J.C. (1986) Animated host selling advertisements:
Their impact on young children’s recognition, attitudes, and behavior. Journal of
Public Policy and Marketing, 5, 171-184.
Ho, D. (1989) Continuity and variation in Chinese patterns of socialization. Journal of
Marriage and the Family, 51: 149-163.
Hsu, F.L.K. (1981) American and Chinese: Passage to differences, third edition, Honolulu:
University Press of Hawaii.
Kelly, J.L., and Tseng, H. M. (1992) Cultural differences in child rearing: A comparison of
immigrant Chinese and Caucasian American mothers. Journal of Cross-Cultural
Psychology, 23(4): 444-455.
Lee, C. and Green, R.T. (1991) Cross-cultural examination of the Fishbein Behavioral
Intentions Model. Journal of International Business Studies. 22(2), 289-305.
Levin, S.R., Petros, T.V., and Petrella, F.W. (1979) Pre-schoolers’ discrimination of
television programming and commercials. Paper presented at the Biennial meeting of
the Society for Research in Child Development, San Francisco, March.
McNeal, J.U. (1987) Children as consumers: Insights and implications. Massachusetts:
Lexington Books.
Meringoff, L.K. and Lesser, G.S. (1980) Children’s ability to distinguish television
commercials from program material. In R. P. Adler, G.S. Lesser, L.K. Meringoff, T.S.
Robertson, J.R. Rossiter, and S. Ward (eds.), The effects of television advertising on
children (pp.29-42). Lexington, MA: Lexington Books.
Nippold, M.A., Cuyler, J.S. and Braunbeck-Price, R. (1988) Explanation of ambiguous
advertisements: a developmental study with children and adolescents, Journal of Speech
and Hearing Research, 31, 466-474.
Piaget, J. (1970) The stages of the intellectual development of the child. In P.H. Mussen
(ed.), Readings in child development and psychology. New York: Harper and Row.
Roedder, D. L. (1981) Age differences in children’s responses to television advertising: An
information processing approach. Journal of Consumer Research, 8(September), 144-
153.
Robertson, T.S. and Rossiter, J.R. (1974) Children and commercial persuasion: An
attribution theory analysis. Journal of Consumer Research, 1(June), 12-20.
Advertising and children 25
Rubin, R.S. (1974). The effects of cognitive development on children’s response to television
advertising. Journal of Business Research, 2 (October), 409-419.
Sheikh, A.A., Prasad, V.K. and Rao, T.R. (1974). Children’s TV commercials: A review of
research. Journal of Communication, 24(4), 126-136.
Shrimp, T., Dyer R. and Divita, S. (1976) An experimental test of the harmful effects of
premium-oriented commercials on children. Journal of Advertising Research, 3: 1-11.
Suzuki, B.H. (1980) The Asian-American family. In M.D. Fantini and Cardenas (Eds.).
Parenting in a multicultural society. New York: Longman.
Van Evra, J.P. (1995) Advertising’s impact on children as a function of viewing purpose.
Psychology and Marketing, 12(5), 423-432.
Wackman, D.B., Wartella, E., and Ward, S. (1977) Learning to be consumers: The role of
the family. Journal of Communication, 27(Winter), 138-151.
Ward, S. (1972) Children’s reactions to commercials. Journal of Advertising Research, 12
(April), 37-45.
Ward, S., Levingson, D., and Wackman, D.B. (1972) Children’s attention to television
advertising. Television and Social Behavior, vol. 4. Washington, D.C.: Government
Printer Office, 491-516.
Ward, S., Wackman, D.B., and Wartella, E. (1977) How children learn to buy: The
development of consumer information processing skills. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage
Publications, Inc.
Yavas, U. and Abdul-Gader, A. (1993). Impact of TV commercials on Saudi children’s
purchase behaviour. Marketing Intelligence and Planning, 11(2), 37-43.
Zuckerman, P., Ziegler, M., and Stevenson, H.W. (1978) Children’s viewing of television and
recognition memory of commercials. Child development, 49, 96-104.
Advertising and children 26
Professor Philip J. Kitchen
Editor, Journal of Marketing Communications
Chapman and Hall
2-6 Boundary Row,
London SE1 8HN, UK
October 30, 1998
Dear Professor Kitchen,
Re-submission of Manuscript
Thank you for your letter on 15 September, 1998 with valuable comments on the
manuscript titled ‘Hong Kong children’s understanding of television advertising’. The
manuscript has been revised with the following changes:
Comment Changes
Literature review Updated references are added
Discussion of objectives Elaborated in more details
Ethical issue on advertising to
children
Added in the discussion section
Please kindly reconsider it to be published in the Journal of Marketing Communications.
Please acknowledge when you received it and do not hesitate to contact me for any inquiries.
Thank you.
Yours truly,
Kara Chan
Associate Professor
Advertising and children 27
Journal of Marketing Communications
Chapman and Hall
2-6 Boundary Row,
London SE1 8HN, UK
June 23, 1998
Dear editor,
Re: Submission of Manuscript
Please find attached four copies of the manuscript titled ‘Hong Kong children’s understanding
of television advertising’ for your consideration of publication. Please acknowledge when you
received it and do not hesitate to contact me for any inquiries. Thank you.
Yours truly,
Kara Chan
Assistant Professor
Advertising and children 28
Prof. Herbert H. Tsang
Chairman
Research Committee
3 March 1999
Dear Prof. Tsang
Re: FRG Research Project entitled “Benchmark study on communication process and
effects of television advertising on children in Hong Kong” (FRG/97-98/II-24)
Thank you for your support to carry out the above project. The research has been
successfully completed and a copy of the final report is now enclosed for your consideration.
One journal paper has been accepted by the Journal of Marketing Communications
for publication. The paper received good comments from the journal reviewers. Another
journal paper and a conference paper are being prepared. The current study provides us an
important benchmark for the understanding of the impact of television advertising on
children.
Yours sincerely,
_____________________
Dr. Kara Chan
Associate Professor
Dept. of Communication Studies
c.c. Dean, School of Communication
Head, Department of Communication Studies
List of Publication
1999 Hong Kong children’s understanding of television advertising, accepted by Journal
of Marketing Communications, forthcoming
Advertising and children 29
Benchmark study on communication process and effects
of television advertising on children in Hong Kong
Dr. Kara Chan
Associate Professor
Department of Communication Studies
Hong Kong Baptist University
Kowloon Tong
Hong Kong
Fax: (582) 2339-7890
Telephone: (582) 2339-7836
E-mail: karachan@hkbu.edu.hk
Keyword: Television advertising – children – Hong Kong
This study is supported by Faculty Research Grant of the Hong Kong Baptist University.
Running head: Advertising and children
March 3, 1999
\r05child\jpaper1.doc
... In addition, Scammon and Christopher (1981) noted that a factor that contributes to like or dislike of an advertisements is the nature of the product or service itself. Chan (2000) stated that children preferred food, toys, drink and mobile phones and dislike advertisements that are about supermarkets or public service announcements. This result is corresponding with Scammon and Christopher's research where they found children prefer or like food advertisements. ...
... A television commercial or advertisement is paid and produced by an organization to convey a message a product or service. Food, drinks, toys and mobile phones are found to be children's preferred advertisements as they found to be interesting and funny (Chan, 2000). The use of cartoon figures and favourite television personalities has added to children's liking for a product. ...
... Thus, children tend to enjoy advertisements that have some elements or aspects to their favorite films and programs. This is also supported by Chan's (2000) research. Chan in his study in Hong Kong found that children preferred food, drink, toys and mobile phones advertisements. ...
Article
Full-text available
Objective This study examines the relationship between Malay children verbal reception and contextual features in fast food advertisements. Methodology/Technique There are four items have been used to measure verbal reception and 26 items to measure contextual features. Structured questionnaire was developed based on previous literatures and a total of 50 pre-school children participated in this study. Findings The findings disclosed that there is a positive correlation exists between verbal receptions and rewarding, imitation and humour and negative correlation between verbal reception, modelling and realism contextual features. Type of Paper Empirical paper Keywords: Verbal Reception, Contextual Features, Children
... Çocukların reklam içeriğini ve kilit mesajları anlamalarında bilişsel gelişim özelliklerinin ve reklamların sunuluş tarzının önemli olduğu bilinmektedir. 7-8 yaş grubundaki çocukların reklamcılığın ne olduğunu ve ikna etmeye yönelik niyetlerini anladıkları bulunurken, 10 yaş çocuklarının neredeyse yarısının reklamların televizyon kanallarında para için gösterildiğini bildikleri belirlenmiştir (Chan, 2000). Okul öncesi dönem çocuğu ise hızından ve sesinden bir reklamı televizyon programından ayırt edebilse de çocuklar için hazırlanmış programlardaki kahramanların ürün olarak kullanılmaları (bebekler, arabalar vb.) bu yaş grubundaki çocuklar için reklam ve program arasındaki ayrımı bulanıklaştırmaktadır (Berk, 2013). ...
Article
Full-text available
Reklamların çocukların gelişimleri ve sosyal normları yapılandırma üzerindeki etkisi büyüktür. Günümüzde reklamcılık ile eğlence arasındaki sınır bulanıklaşmıştır. Reklam içeriklerinin birçok medya platformundaki içeriklere entegrasyonu nedeniyle ticari ve eğlence amaçlı medya içeriğinin çocuklar tarafından ayırt edilmesi zorlaşmış olduğundan, bu çalışmada bir çocuk kanalında yayımlanan reklamların ikna taktikleri açısından incelenmesi amaçlanmıştır. Bu kapsamda Cartoon Network çocuk kanalındaki 25 farklı markaya ait reklam çalışmaya dahil edilmiştir. Bunun yanı sıra reklamlar hedef kitle, rol alan karakterler, reklam türü, kullanılan teknik ve sektör açısından da incelenmiştir. Nitel model ve durum deseni kullanılan araştırmada içerik analizi ve betimsel analiz yapılmıştır. Çalışmanın sonucunda çocuk kanalında yayımlanan reklamlarda özellikle büyük vaat/iddia, süper insan, ödül, hikâye ve oyun gibi birçok ikna taktiği kullandığı belirlenmiştir. Ayrıca incelenen reklamlardaki ikna modellerine de örnekler verilmiştir. Çocukların reklamları işlemleme biçimleri yetişkinlerden farklı olarak daha çok otomatik, duygusal tepkiler yoluyla olduğundan reklam içerik ve özelliklerine karşı bilinçli olmak ve reklam okuryazarlığı becerilerini çocuklara kazandırmak önemli görülmektedir.
... The incorporation of these new audio-visual resources has generated investigation into the benefits of their use both for acquiring academic content in distinct disciplines and for understanding the information transmitted by mass media (Chan, 2000;Lawlor & Prothero, 2003;Nikken & Peeters, 2009;Quain, Bokunewicz, & Criscione-Naylor, 2018). Paul and Wang (2012) highlighted the importance of the role of adults as learning facilitators, since they can control viewing by pausing the image, and by discussing and reviewing content to facilitate students' understanding of the information. ...
Article
The main objective of this study was to explore the benefits of the use of educational audio-visual materials in facilitating learning for students with hearing loss. The study analysed whether students with hearing loss had a visual learning preference when they watched an audio-visual and if the images present contributed to the retention of more information by this group. The study sample was made up of 28 participants, from 7 to 9 years old, of whom 14 had prelingual hearing loss and 14 were age- and sex-matched students without hearing loss. They were all schooled together in general education classrooms in an oral modality. They were asked to watch an educational video and then to describe its contents orally. The results obtained from analysing the references to the video content indicate that, despite the fact that the hard of hearing group made greater reference to video content transmitted from the images than the group without hearing loss, the effect does not achieve statistical significance. The study reinforces the idea that deafness does not determine a specific learning preference.
... Bijmolt, Claassen, & Brus, 1998;Stutts et al., 1981;E.Rozendaal et al., 2011). On the question of children's understanding about the advertisements, advertising selling and persuasive intent, some studies suggests that children may not understand advertising until they reach the age of seven or eight years (Chan, 2000), while other researchers states that their skill of comprehension does not develop until the child is 12 years (Edling, 1999). According to Livingstone and Helsper (2006, p 562), although children as young as 7 or 8 (Piaget's Concrete Operational Stage) understand that advertising is different from information, yet, it is not until they are about 12 years old (Piaget's Formal Operational Stage) that they can articulate a critical thought about the comprehension of advertising and advertiser's intent. ...
Article
Market for children consumption is growing worldwide and hence the marketing effort. Marketers are keen to know the behavioural secrets to influence children’s consumptions. The promotional efforts are need to be more fruitful and focused, therefore imperative to understand the conceptual advertising literacy level of the targeted child consumers. In order to reveal the related facts, the study examines the conceptual advertising literacy level of children in India. In a computer assisted survey of 392 children between the age brackets of 8-12 years, the researchers examine their ability of advertising recognition, understanding of advertising selling intent and understanding of advertising persuasive intent regarding brand promotion efforts. Results confirm that advertising recognition and understanding selling intent is high in children but children do not acquire a clear understanding of advertising persuasive intent until the age of 9-10 years. Furthermore, children’s understanding of advertising’s selling intent emerges before their comprehension of its persuasive intent. KEYWORDS: Children, Advertising Literacy, Persuasive Intent, Selling Intent
... The literature about children and commercial advertising has explored a great variety of topics including the influence of children on family purchases after exposure to commercials and the parent-child conflict (Buijzen & Valkenburg, 2003;Ebster et al., 2009;Page et al., 2018;Valkenburg, 2004), content analyses of television advertisements and persuasive marketing techniques to promote products to children (Hebden et al., 2011;Jenkin et al., 2014;Kim et al., 2016;Puggelli & Bertolotti, 2014), children's comprehension of advertising and, in particular, their ability to understand the persuasive intent of commercial advertising (Carter et al., 2011;Chan, 2000;Oates et al., 2002), children's ability to recognize commercial advertisements on television and on the Internet Blades et al., 2013;Carter et al., 2011) and finally, the influence of advertising on children's brand awareness and brand preferences (Boyland & Halford, 2013;Pecheux & Derbaix, 1999;Valkenburg & Buijzen, 2005). ...
Article
Full-text available
This paper suggests a model that considers the effects of the children’s attitude toward Public Service Announcements—PSAs (measured through the likeability of PSAs) on their behavioural intention to eat fruits and vegetables. The suggested model was tested through an empirical analysis conducted with children aged 8 to 11 and the data were analysed through partial least squares – structural equation modeling (PLS-SEM) multigroup analysis. The findings indicate that the positive emotions PSAs generate and PSA credibility mediate the effects of the children’s perceived likeability of PSAs on their behavioural intention to eat fruits and vegetables. Overall, understanding the connections between PSA likeability, positive emotions, PSA credibility and behavioural intentions can facilitate the development of further social advertisements aimed at children covering healthy eating. Therefore, the findings of this study are relevant for non-profit organizations, government institutions and advertisers interested in creating effective social messages aimed at children.
... An understanding of the market structure is believed to develop throughout childhood and into adolescence (John 1999). For example, Chan's (2000) survey showed that children's understanding of television advertising started to develop from grade 2 (age 7-8), and children's understanding of why television stations carry advertising increased from grade 4 (age 9-10). Thus, literacy programs with more components, including structure literacy, could be more effective for older children but less effective for younger children. ...
Article
Full-text available
Advertising literacy education can be developed based on three literacy components: content, grammar, and structure literacy. Content literacy refers to an understanding of the content of ads (e.g., themes and ideas), grammar literacy concerns an understanding of the grammar of ads (e.g., visual techniques) used for persuasion, and structure literacy refers to an understanding of the social and economic structure of the media industry that affects production of ads (e.g., ownership and revenue models). This study tested the immediate and delayed effects of different literacy education components in an ad literacy program on children’s advertising knowledge and criticism, and the moderating role of age in these effects. To do so, we conducted an experiment based on a 3 literacy component (content literacy, content + grammar literacy, and content + grammar + structure literacy) × 2 age (third versus fifth graders) × 2 time (immediate test versus one-year delayed test) design. Results showed that adding the structure literacy component resulted in greater knowledge for fifth graders (ages 10 and 11) but not for third graders (ages eight and nine). In addition, adding the structure literacy component resulted in lower criticism. Theoretical and practical implications are discussed.
... Children over the age of eight can attribute the selling intent to a commercial, understanding that advertisements are designed to encourage people to buy things (Gunter, Oates, & Blades, 2005;Young, 1990). Children older than 10 or 11 can comprehend the relationship between advertisements, television stations, and programs (Andronikidis & Lambrianidou, 2010;Chan, 2000;Lawlor & Prothero, 2003; understand that concepts such as audience targeting and segmentation are one component of conceptual advertising literacy (Rozendaal, Lapierre, Van Reijmersdal, & Buijzen, 2011); and recognize the tactics that advertisements use to enhance persuasion, such as making exaggerated product claims or making the product look more appealing than it really is (Mallalieu, Palin, & Laczniak, 2005;Robertson & Rossiter, 1974). ...
Chapter
Introduction Open any introductory marketing textbook and you will learn that the role of the firm is to create, communicate, and deliver value to the consumer who, in turn, takes the passive role of paying and consuming. For many years, this was, in fact, how marketers, consumer researchers, and psychologists perceived these two roles; the notion of consumer input into value creation was almost entirely neglected.This began to change when researchers in the area of innovation identified product users modifying and innovating on their own. In fact, von Hippel, De Jong, and Flowers (2012) found that in a representative sample of UK consumers, more than 6 percent had engaged in product modification or innovation during the prior three years, resulting in annual product development expenditures 1.4 times larger than the respective research and development (R&D) expenditures of all UK firms. More broadly, what emerged was the concept of “democratizing innovation,” that getting users actively involved in the process of new product development (NPD) can be a great source of value to the consumer and, thus, the firm (von Hippel, 2005). Today, consumer input is a recognized force in new product development, so much so that the Marketing Science Institute (MSI) listed it as one of its top priorities for exploration for 2008 through 2010.A parallel development in the marketplace has been that firms are going after smaller and more well-defined segments (Dalgic & Leeuw, 1994; Kotler & Armstrong, 2013). This is due to a number of factors, including the abundance of brands competing in many sectors; the rapid growth in media outlets, particularly online; and the increasing amount of information available on individual consumers. The result is that, in both media (Nelson-Field & Riebe, 2011) and products (Dalgic, 2006), the use of niche marketing is on the rise, while mass marketing is becoming an increasingly less viable option, particularly for new products.These two developments, consumer involvement in design as well as smaller target markets, have resulted in the practice of self-customization, where instead of offering ready-made products, the firm equips consumers with the tools to customize and design their own product. This can be viewed as the ultimate form of niche marketing, where the resulting segments consist of individuals.
Chapter
Brian Young continues his journey by examining the child at school developing as an autonomous consumer (with a little help from the Bank of Mum) and how strands of socialisation shape children’s understanding of money and brands. Being able to control and manage behaviour is key to serious development. These recent developments in children’s development of their executive control in different situations are summarised and the updated review of work on advertising to children and the child’s understanding of brands is essential reading.
Article
Full-text available
Three groups of mothers with different cultural backgrounds, 397 Chinese living in Taiwan, 95 Chinese-Americans immigrated from Taiwan, and 213 Anglo-Americans, were administered the Parental Attitude Research Instrument (PARI). Significant differences were found on all three attitudinal factors and on all 23 attitudinal scales. The results showed that (1) the Chinese mothers were most restrictive, the Anglo-American mothers were least restrictive, and the Chinese-American mothers were intermediate on the continuum of authoritarian-control; (2) the Chinese-American mothers were more likely lo approve the expression of hostility or rejection than the Chinese or Anglo-American mothers; (3) the Chinese-American mothers were more democratic than the Chinese mothers, and the Chinese mothers, more democratic than the Anglo-American mothers.
Article
The use of program characters to promote products (host selling) was investigated in an experiment. Two variables were manipulated: 1) whether the commercial character was the same as or different than the program character and, 2) the position of the commercial relative to the program. The results revealed that young children, aged three to seven, could distinguish a host-selling commercial from programming. Additionally, the commercial's location relative to the program affected their ability to recognize the commercial. Finally, the children experienced greater positive attitude change toward the advertised product when they viewed a nonhost-selling as compared to a host-selling commercial.
Article
Explanation of linguistic ambiguity occurring in a naturalistic context was examined for children and adolescents at various ages. Subjects ages 9 through 18 were asked to explain the meanings of 14 ambiguous advertisements taken from recent magazines, newspapers, and brochures. All of the ads were lexically ambiguous and contained a word or phrase that had both a physical and a psychological meaning (e.g., "Introducing the Upper Crusts. Two sensational new entrees from Stouffer's"). Results were that older subjects explained the meanings correctly more frequently than younger subjects, although adolescents who were 18-years-old did not perform perfectly. It was also found that the psychological meanings of the ads were more difficult to explain than the physical meanings. The results were interpreted in relation to previous developmental studies of ambiguity involving sentences, jokes, and riddles , and Studies of figurative language explanation.
Article
The literature on Chinese patterns of socialization, which derives from diverse sources in both Chinese and English and is addressed to different groups of readers, is critically reviewed. Methodological issues are discussed with respect to two central issues: continuity versus change through time, and variation across geographical locations. Three major variables, age, sex, and social class, are systematically considered in relation to these two issues. The principal conclusions are that (a) noticeable departures from the traditional pattern in different geographical locations are evident, even within relatively short periods of time, but continuity with the past remains highly discernible; and (b) important though subcultural-ethnic and cross-national differences are, they appear less significant when gauged against commonalities in the general pattern.
Article
Children four through eleven years old viewed informational (excerpts from network news), persuasive (child-oriented and adult-oriented commercials), educational (excerpts from instructional spots), and mixed (child-oriented public service announcements) messages. They were then interviewed about message content, type, intent, believability, and belief criteria. Comprehension of narrative content was high, even among the youngest children. Most young children were able to identify messages for which common labels exist (news; commercials), but few attached labels to educational spots or public service announcements. Correct articulation of message intent occurred primarily among older children; few under age eight correctly identified the intent of any message type. There was an age-related trend toward the use of functional cues to aid in message identification, and toward reality testing as the appropriate basis for evaluation of message believability. The evidence indicates that young children may interpret messages in informational terms regardless of message intent (e.g., to persuade or instruct).
Article
Children in grades 2, 3, and 4 were videotaped while watching a standard 15-min television presentation in a seminaturalistic setting. Videotapes were analyzed for attention to television, viewing patterns, and alternate activities. Recognition memory of auditory and visual content of the commercials and of the products was tested. Comparisons were made of the children's behavior during the program and during the commercials. In general, the children's behavior was marked by low levels of attention, a decline in attention during commercials, and poor recognition memory of commercial content. Attention was more strongly related to visual than to auditory recognition. Evidence for rapid habituation to the content of commercials was obtained.