ArticlePDF Available

Sensory capacities and the nocturnal habit of owls (Strigiformes)

Authors:

Abstract

Behavioural studies show that in the eye of the Tawny Owl Strix aluco both absolute visual sensitivity and maximum spatial resolution at low light levels are close to the theoretical limit dictated principally by the quantal nature of light and the physiological limitations on the structure of vertebrate eyes. However, when the owl's visual sensitivity in relation to naturally occurring ligh levels is analysed, it is concluded that at night there will often be occasions when vision can only be used to control the owl's behaviour with respect to large objects. Owls are capable of detecting and catching prey by hearing alone. However, absolute auditory sensitivity is not superior to that of mammals (including Man), but does appear to have reached the absolute limit on sensitivity in the aerial environment, which is dictated by the minimum ambient sound level. An explanation of the owl's ability to be active at night based only upon high sensory sensitivity is thus untenable. Many features of the natural behaviour of the Tawny Owl (e.g., the high degree of territoriality, prey catching technique, dietary spectrum) may be interpreted as reflections of an additional requirement for the nocturnal habit beyond high sensory sensitivity: detailed knowledge of local topography.
IBIS
128:
266-277
Sensory capacities and the nocturnal habit
of
owls
(Strigiformes)
GRAHAM R. MARTIN
Departments
of
Zoology and Comparative Physiology, and
of
Extramural Studies,
University
of
Birmingham, P.O.
Box
363,
Birmingham
B15
2TT
Accepted
5
July
1985
Behavioural studies show that in the eye
of
the Tawny Owl
Strzx
alum
both absolute visual sensitivity and
maximum spatial resolution at low light levels are close to the theoretical limit dictated principally by the
quanta1 nature of light and the physiological limitations on the structure
of
vertebrate eyes. However,
when the owl’s visual sensitivity in relation to naturally occurring ligh levels is analysed, it is concluded
that at night there will often be occasions when vision can only be used to control the owl’s behaviour with
respect to large objects.
Owls are capable of detecting and catching prey by hearing alone. However, absolute auditory
sensitivity is not superior to that of mammals (including Man), but does appear to have reached the
absolute limit on sensitivity in the aerial environment, which is dictated by the minimum ambient sound
level.
An explanation
of
the owl’s ability to be active at night based only upon high sensory sensitivity
is
thus untenable. Many features of the natural behaviour
of
the Tawny Owl (e.g., the high degree
of
territoriality, prey catching technique, dietary spectrum) may be interpreted as reflections
of
an additional
requirement
for
the nocturnal habit beyond high sensory sensitivity: detailed knowledge of local
topography.
The nocturnal habit amongst birds is rare. Probably less than 250 species
(c.
30/) are
regularly active outside daylight hours and the majority of these species, such as
certain nightjars (Caprimulgidae), are best regarded as crepuscular rather than
strictly nocturnal in their activity. Some bird species (swiftlets
Collocalia,
and the
Oil Bird
Steatornis caripensis)
are able to guide themselves in the totally dark interior
of caves, using echolocation (Griffin
&
Suthers 1970, Konishi
&
Knudsen 1979);
however, such environments are structurally simple.
It is only amongst the Strigiformes (owls and barn owls) and the Caprimulgi-
formes (frogmouths and their allies) that strictly nocturnal, non-echolocatory birds
which fly in structurally complex environments, such as forests, are found. Not all
species in these orders are nocturnal; indeed, probably less than
40
of the 135 species
of owl may be regarded as strictly nocturnal, in that they are capable of completing all
aspects of their life cycle between sunset and sunrise (Burton 1985).
The nocturnal environment would therefore seem to pose particular limiting
problems for flying birds, and it is typically supposed that these problems are sensory
in nature (Walls 1942, Tansley 1965). This paper presents available data on the
visual and auditory capacities of owls and views these in the context of the ultimate
limits on sensitivity and the critical features of the nocturnal environment. It is
concluded that while the principal problems posed for the nocturnal behaviour of
owls are indeed sensory, the solution
to
those problems involves more than sensory
adaptations. Habitual nocturnal behaviour amongst birds would seem to be
dependent upon behavioural, as well as sensory, adaptations.
I
986
SENSORY
CAPACITIES
OF
OWLS
267
The principal species discussed here
is
the Tawny Owl
Strix
aluco.
The visual
and auditory capacities of this species have been more comprehensively studied than
those of any other bird, and many aspects
of
its natural history have been studied in
detail. The Tawny Owl has a wide but discontinuous distribution in the palearctic
and oriental zoogeographical regions. Its preferred habitat
is
closed canopy broad-
leaved deciduous woodland, but it will inhabit coniferous and more open woodland
types, parkland and the centres
of
large cities as long as mature trees are present
(Beven 1956, Mikkola 1983). It is argued that the sensory adaptations of this and
other nocturnal species are very similar and most of the problems discussed here
concerning the Tawny Owl apply generally to nocturnal owl species.
Vision
at
low light levels
Absolute sensitivity
Field observations have long been interpreted as evidence that the visual sensitivity
in owls
is
higher than in Man. Indeed the assumption
of
high sensitivity, coupled
with evidence that owl retinae contain many rod photoreceptors was part
of
the
original evidence used
to
support the duplicity theory of
vision
(Schultze 1867). The
assumed high visual sensitivity of owls was correlated with the large size, tubular
shape and frontal placement of owl eyes compared with those typically found in
diurnal birds (Fig.
l),
and these anatomical features became regarded as adaptations
concerned with the nocturnal habit. However, how these particular features were
supposed to facilitate high sensitivity was not discussed in detail (e.g., Walls
1942,
Tansley 1965).
The assumption of high visual sensitivity in owls was questioned by evidence
which suggested that the visual detection
of
infra-red radiation could mediate prey
capture in the Tawny Owl (Vlanderpianck 1934). However, the ocular media
of
this
Parus
Figure
1.
Drawings
of
horizontal sections through the head of the Black-capped Chickadee
Parus
atricapillus
and the Great Horned
Owl
Bubo
viriginianus.
Redrawn to scale from Wood
(1917).
In
Parus
eye shape, size and position are typical of diurnal Passeriforme species, in
Bubo
eye shape and position are
typical
of
other
owl
species, and eye size is similar to that
of
the Tawny
Owl.
268
G.
R.
MARTIN
IBIS
128
bird were subsequently shown to be virtually opaque to infra-red radiation
(Matthews
&
Matthews 1939).
The first experimental studies of visual sensitivity suggested that owl eyes were
between
10
and
100
times more sensitive than that of Man (Hecht
&
Pirenne
1940,
Dice 1945). However, neither of these studies measured absolute sensitivity directly,
nor was it claimed that stimuli were calibrated with a high degree of accuracy. More
recent analysis of visual sensitivity in Tawny Owls has led to a more modest
conclusion in that absolute sensitivity in these birds was found to be higher than that
of Man by an average of 2.5-fold (Martin 1977). This difference is within the normal,
five-fold, range of absolute visual sensitivity found in the healthy human population
and also within the range by which individual human thresholds may differ on a daily
basis (Pirenne
et
al.
1957). Thus we might expect to find individual human subjects
with visual sensitivity greater than that of individual Tawny Owls.
The 2.5-fold average difference in sensitivity between Owl and Man can be
accounted for by a difference in the maximum brightness
of
the retinal image
produced in the owl and human eye (Martin 1982). This suggests that the retinae of
these two eyes are of similar sensitivity; there are good theoretical reasons for
believing that this should be the case. For example, it has been argued that the
human retina has attained the absolute limit of visual sensitivity, which is dictated by
the quanta1 nature of light and the signal-to-noise limitations of extracting
information from an array of photoreceptors which are responding at the limits of
their sensitivity (Pirenne 1962, Barlow 1981). Since the rod photoreceptors of Man
and Owl are very similar in respect of three parameters-dimensions, spectral
sensitivity and specific absorbance of their visual pigments (Bowmaker
&
Martin
1978, Bowmaker
&
Dartnall 1980)-they would absorb a similar proportion of the
light quanta that reaches them (Martin 1982). Therefore unless maximum retinal
image brightness can be greatly increased over that of the human eye, visual
sensitivity much in excess of that in humans is unlikely to be found in any owl
species.
An hypothetical vertebrate eye with
a
focal length similar to that of the Tawny
Owl
[
E
17
mm; (Martin 1982)] but whose visual sensitivity was
10
or
100
times that
of Man, would require entrance pupil diameters of
25
mm and 81 mm respectively.
Clearly such eyes would have to be both absolutely large as well as peculiar in their
general design-quite unlike the typical tubular shape of the owl eye (Fig.
l),
and
indeed quite unlike any vertebrate eye
so
far described (Martin 1983). Furthermore,
it has been argued that a relatively long focal length, such as that found in the Tawny
Owl’s eye, is an essential feature
of
an eye designed to function throughout the
naturally-occurring range of night time luminance levels (Martin 1982). This is
because a long focal length will produce a large retinal image and thus permit
flexibility in the way the retinal image is sampled by the photoreceptor array. Such
flexibility is necessary to maximise the amount of information which can be extracted
from the retinal image over a wide range
of
luminances (Snyder
et
al.
1977). [The
naturally occurring nocturnal luminance range is discussed below]. Thus, given this
constraint of a long focal length, it would seem that an owl eye with an image
brightness even
10,
let alone
100
times higher than that of Man, is unlikely to have
evolved.
Absolute visual sensitivity in the Tawny Owl is approximately
100
times higher
than that
of
the Pigeon
Calumba
livia
(Blough 1955) (Fig.
2),
a strictly diurnal bird
species. This difference can be attributed principally to differences in retinal neural
mechanisms, since pigeon and owl eyes differ little in both the maximum brightness
of
their retinal image and in the parameters of individual rod photoreceptors (Martin
1982).
1986
I
0
0
-I
-
-
h
+
3
.-
4"
-2
-3
I
c
1
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
SENSORY
CAPACITIES OF
OWLS
I
269
mst
mxs
mm
sr
mrn
-
hobrtats
ODed-;
'
Cloud
-
mst
mxs
-
mm
+
sr
Brood leoved
woodlond conopy
-
Cloud
-
Figure
2.
Top. Minimum separable visual acuity (expressed as loglo of the reciprocal
of
the minimum
separable angle) as
a
function of luminance in Man, Pigeon and two owl species (cdm-*, candela per
square metre).
Bottom. Naturally occurring luminance levels of a grass
or
leaf litter substrate during day-time and night-
time. Luminance ranges within open habitats and under
a
broad leaved woodland canopy are shown. Each
horizontal bar indicates the maximum likely range
of
luminance which can be experienced in the habitat
under the conditions indicated. Day-time luminance levels (the four horizontal bars to the right) are for
latitude
50"
at the time of the summer solstice, and show the luminance range
from
sunrise
(sr)
to
maximum midday sun (ms). Night-time luminance levels (the four horizontal bars to the left) show the
range from maximum moonlight
(mm)
to minimum starlight
(mst);
maximum starlight without moon
(mxs)
is also indicated. Luminance levels experienced under maximum cloud cover are shown for both
day- and night-time in the two habitat types. The total
diurnal luminance range
of
5.6 log,, units
is
the
range which may be experienced between maximum sunlight without cloud cover in an open habitat, and
at sunrise with cloud cover beneath woodland canopy. The total
nocturnal luminance range
of 6.31
loglo
units is the range from maximum moonlight without cloud in an open habitat, to minimum starlight with
cloud beneath the woodland canopy. Points
A,
B and
C
indicate the mean
behaviourally-determined
absolute oisual threshold
of Pigeon, Man and Owl respectively (modified from Martin, 1982).
(0-0)
Human; Pirenne
et
al.
(1957);
(0-0)
Human; Shlaer (1937);
(U----U)
Owl
Bubo;
Fite (1973);
(V----V)
Owl
Strix;
Martin and Gordon (1974);
(Op)
Pigeon;
Columba,
Blough (1971);
(0..
.
,
.O)
Pigeon; Hodos and Leibowitz (1977). Hodos
et
al.
(1976).
G.
R.
MARTIN
IBIS
128
2
70
Spatial resolution
Theoretical considerations of retinal image analysis (Snyder
et
al.
1977) suggest that
spatial resolution at low light levels in Man and Owls should be similar, and there is
experimental evidence that this is
so
(Fig. 2). Spatial resolution of any image must
decrease with luminance level regardless of the device (e.g., photoreceptors,
photographic emulsion, photodiodes, etc) used to analyse the image. High spatial
resolution of an image of low luminance is not possible theoretically, regardless
of
the
degree of contrast in the image (Snyder
et
al.
1977).
There is commercially available equipment which serves
to
enhance human
visual resolution at night; it does
so
either by increasing the size and brightness of the
image, and/or by unemploying a non-visible part of the electromagnetic spectrum
which is at a higher intensity than the visible wavelengths
(e.g.,
infra-red). Because
of the similarities in rod photoreceptor parameters in human and owl eyes, it may be
seen that an owl could achieve spatial resolution superior to that of Man by
employing only one element of the strategy used by the commercial devices, i.e.,
increased image-size and brightness. However, as argued above it would seem that
there is little possibility that this can occur.
Vision at high light levels
Behavioural (Martin
&
Gordon 1974a,b), electrophysiological (Martin
et
al.
1975)
and anatomical (Bowmaker
&
Martin 1978) studies all furnish evidence that the
Tawny Owl visual system functions adequately at high, day-time, light levels.
Evidence from analysis
of
the visual pigments of cone photoreceptors and their
associated oil droplets (Bowmaker
&
Martin 1978) corroborates behavioural
investigations of wavelength discrimination (Martin 1974) which suggest that the
owl is not capable of such subtle wavelength discriminations as diurnal bird species
such as the Pigeon
Colurnba
livia (Wright 1979). However, Figure
2
shows that
maximum spatial resolution in two owl species-(the Tawny Owl and the Great
Horned Owl
Bubo
virginianus)-and in the pigeon is very similar and is attained in all
three species at a similar luminance level. This implies that, during the day-time,
owls are not at a visual disadvantage compared with at least some obligate diurnal
bird species, such as the Pigeon. All of these species are, however, inferior in their
maximum spatial resolution compared with either man or with a diurnal bird of prey,
the American Kestrel
Falco
sparvevius
(Hirsch 1982). Maximum resolution in these
latter species is approximately five times higher than in the tawny owl at a similar
high luminance level.
Binocular vision
The owls in general are often regarded as having the most frontally placed eyes
amongst the birds (Fig. 1) and that this has resulted in them possessing some of the
largest binocular fields to be found in this vertebrate class. However, in the Tawny
Owl the optic axes of the eyes are not parallel, but diverge by
55"
and maximum
binocular field width equals only 48" (Martin 1984a). By comparison, the optic axes
in Man and Pigeon diverge by 10" and 132" respectively and the maximum widths of
their binocular fields are approximately 140" and 22". The function of binocularity
and the factors which may account for the known interspecific differences in
binocular field width amongst vertebrates has received much discussion, but there is
little concensus on either of these topics (Martin 1984a). Thus it is not clear whether
the relatively large binocular field
of
the owl compared with that
of
the Pigeon can be
271
correlated directly with the nocturnal habit, as has often been assumed (Walls 1942,
Tansley
1965,
Mikkola 1983, Burton
1985).
I
986
SENSORY
CAPACITIES
OF
OWLS
Vision in relation to naturally occurring light levels
Figure
2
shows that the light levels which may be experienced within the nocturnal
environment are highly variable. Depending upon such factors as the degree of
vegetation cover, the altitude and phase of the moon and the presence of cloud,
incident light levels at night may differ by over one million-fold. In addition, the
reflectance of natural substrates can differ by a factor of up to 100-fold [e.g., fresh
snow compared with black soil (Krinov
1947)]
so
the range of luminance levels an eye
might be exposed
to
after sunset may cover eight orders of magnitude. Thus, for the
purposes of understanding the sensory problems of the nocturnal habit the simple
‘nocturnal’ label can be misleading. For example, nocturnal bird species which live
in predominantly open habitats such as the majority of the Caprimulgiformes
(nightjars, etc.), and some owls, may never experience the lowest naturally occurring
luminance levels to which the nocturnal woodland owls are frequently exposed.
Figure
2
indicates that, even under cloud cover, the sky itself should always be
visible to both Man and Tawny Owl (though not to the Pigeon) and thus some kind
of pattern vision should be possible by viewing objects in silhouette against the sky.
However, under a woodland canopy the minimum natural luminance even of grass
or leaf litter, let alone black earth or similar low reflectance substrates, is low enough
to render these substrates invisible both to Man and to Tawny Owl.
Also,
because
the reflectances of many natural substrates and objects are very similar (Krinov
1947), the visual scene will be of low contrast.
So
there will be many occasions,
especially under a vegetation canopy, when even large objects will be invisible
despite being viewed at average luminance levels which are above absolute visual
threshold. While this invisibility of natural objects can be readily verified by personal
‘observation’, predicting the nature, size and distance at which various objects might
just be detected at a given luminance is problematic, principally because there is little
information on spatial resolution beyond measures of minimum separable acuity
(involving high contrast stimuli) at low luminance levels. However, something
of
the
problem can be appreciated by the following prediction which can be made from the
data of Figure 2: at a luminance level approximately
10
times higher than absolute
visual threshold (i.e., in the middle of the luminance range which commonly occurs
under a broad-leaved woodland canopy at night) even a dark-coloured rodent
60
mm
in length walking across snow will be invisible to both Man and
owl
until within
a
viewing distance of
3
m.
It would seem, therefore, that at the lower naturally occurring light levels the
visual system can only be used both by Man and Owls
to
control behaviour with
respect to large objects, and cannot detect such objects as small branches or prey
items.
Audition
Auditory
localization
It has been clearly demonstrated that audition can play an important part in prey
capture by owls. Barn Owls
Tyto
alba
can detect and capture live rodent prey by
pouncing from a perch in total darkness, guided only by the sound that the animal
G.
R.
MARTIN
IBIS
128
272
produces as
it
moves through leaf litter (Payne 1971). The outer ears of many owl
species are large, and exhibit complex bilateral asymmetry in size, shape and position
(Norberg 1977). It has been proposed that these anatomical features are an essential
part of the auditory localization system, functioning principally in the vertical
location of sounds in the frontal plane (Norberg 1978, Knudsen
&
Konishi 1979).
While studies of auditory localization in the Barn Owl (Knudsen
et
al.
1979), make it
clear that audition itself is sufficient to mediate prey capture, it is also clear that the
Barn Owl’s ability to localize white noise sound sources (which approximate leaf
litter rustle) is not superior to that of Man (Mills 1958, Roffler
&
Butler 1968).
Auditory sensitivity
As
is
the case with visual sensitivity, it was proposed that owls were markedly
superior to man in their absolute auditory sensitivity (Konishi 1973). However, this
conclusion was based upon the result obtained from a single subject, and it is known
(Sivian
&
White 1933, Masterton
et
al.
1969) that measures of minimum auditory
thresholds exhibit large variability due to both intraspecific and procedural
differences.
More detailed interspecific comparisons of auditory thresholds in birds and
mammals have led to the conclusion that both owls and mammals, including Man,
have similar absolute auditory sensitivity and that this sensitivity coincides with the
auditory masking level produced by minimum naturally occurring ambient sound
(Martin 1984b). This suggests that the ultimate limit on auditory sensitivity in
vertebrates
is
determined not by physiological factors, such as thermal agitation
of
fluids within the cochlea (Harris 1967)
,
or ‘self-noise’ (Diercks
&
Jeffress 1962), but
by a source common to all non-aquatic vertebrates: the minimum ambient sound
level. Auditory sensitivity greater than that dictated by the minimum ambient sound
is unlikely to evolve, since sounds below this minimum will always be inaudible due
to the auditory masking produced by the ambient noise.
It may be concluded, therefore, that under any given environmental conditions
the ability
of
both owls and man to detect and locate a particular sound is likely to be
very similar.
The sensory problem
of
nocturnality
The conclusion to be drawn from the above discussion of both the visual and
auditory capacities of owls is that an explanation of the ability of these birds to be
active at night, based only upon high sensory sensitivity, is untenable. Owls possess a
visual system whose absolute sensitivity and resolution approach the theoretical
maxima (dictated principally by the quanta1 nature of light). Even
so,
vision cannot
function adequately to mediate prey detection
or
flight in the spatially complex
environment
of
a woodland habitat under the full range of nocturnal light
conditions. Also, although auditory sensitivity in
OW~S
appears
to
have reached the
ultimate limit possible in the aerial environment, audition cannot of itself permit the
detection of obstacles, although
it
can clearly serve in the detection
of
prey items
which either emit sounds
or
make sounds as they move.
It has been hypothesized that much,
if
not all, human perception is dependent
upon cognition as well as immediate sensory input (see, for example, Gregory
(1974), Frisby (1979)). This is exemplified by studies of car-driving at night-time. In
completing this task people frequently, sometimes habitually, drive in a manner
which is beyond the control of information immediately available via their visual
system (Hills 1980). Accidents are usually avoided, however, since the visual
I
986
SENSORY
CAPACITIES
OF
OWLS
273
information that is available (e.g., detection of road markings and lights on other
vehicles) is supplemented by general knowledge of the nature of roads and traffic and
specific knowledge of local topography.
Since both Man and Tawny Owl are equally restricted in their main tele-
receptive sensory capacities, a similar analysis would seem applicable to the
nocturnal behaviour of the Tawny Owl. Thus, it may be proposed that knowlege of
both the general characteristics of the environment and specific details of the local
topography are important for the mediation of the bird’s behaviour under nocturnal
conditions. The nocturnal habit
of
owls must be dependent upon behavioural and
cognitive adaptations, as well as sensory adaptations.
Natural history
of
the tawny owl
Long-term studies of the natural history of Tawny Owls (Southern 1970, Hirons
1976, 1985, Hardy 1977) have shown that this species exhibits a number of unusual
behavioural traits. Perhaps the most important single feature of the Tawny Owl’s
natural history is its sedentary habit which is manifested
by
its high degree of
territoriality.
The birds are territorial throughout the year and territorial boundaries alter
little, if at all, during a bird’s life time. The defended area supplies all of the food
requirements
of
the individual bird. Also, both male and female share the same
territory throughout the year, although there is some evidence that females
occasionally move territories. In these features the Tawny Owl differs not only from
the majority of bird species, including sympatric diurnal raptors of similar habitat
preferences (e.g., the Sparrowhawk
Accipiter nisus
and Goshawk
A. gentilis
(Newton
1979, Marquiss
&
Newton 1981, Kenward 1982)), but also appears to differ from the
majority
of
other owl species.
No other owl species has received such detailed study as the Tawny Owl, but
summarised data for Western Palearctic species of owl (Cramp 1985) indicates that
only in the Ural Owl
Strix uralensis
do individuals behave like the Tawny Owl and
typically defend an exclusive hunting and breeding territory throughout the year.
Both the Tawny and Ural Owls are nocturnal, with extensive woodland the preferred
hunting and breeding habitat.
There are, however, species in which individuals
or
pairs have, on occasion, been
reported as maintaining a territory throughout the year (Barn Owl, Eagle Owl
Bubo
bubo,
Little Owl
Athene noctua,
Long-Eared Owl
Asio
otus,
Tengmalm’s Owl
Aegolius funereus).
These species are also regarded as nocturnal but they prefer
habitats which are essentially more open than those frequented by the Tawny and
Ural Owls, with scattered trees or woodland glades and margins an essential
component. It would be of interest to know more detail of the particular
circumstances (especially habitat characteristics) which are correlated with the
occurrence
of
a sedentary habit in certain individuals
of
these species.
No
diurnal and/or crepuscular species of Western Palearctic owl (Snowy Owl
Nyctea scandiaca,
Pygymy Owl
Glaucidium
passerinurn,
Hawk Owl
Surnia
ulula,
Great Grey Owl
Strix
nebulosa,
Short-Eared Owl
Asio flarnrneus)
appears to be as
territorial as the nocturnal owls. In these species the majority
of
pairs maintain a
territory (which need not coincide with the hunting range) during the breeding
season only.
Of the Western Palearctic species only the Scops Owl
Otus
scops
presents an
exception to these general conclusions in that the species is regarded as essentially
nocturnal, but there are no records of sedentary individuals
or
pairs. Scops Owl
requires the cover of trees for roosting and nesting but hunt large insect prey over
274
G.
R.
MARTIN
IBIS
128
open ground. Northern populations of the species are migratory, while southern
ones are partially migratory or resident. However, it is not known whether any of the
resident individuals maintain a territory outside the breeding season although local
populations are known
to
be gregarious at this time.
In the case of the Tawny Owl, possession of a territory seems essential for the
survival of the bird during the annual cycle.
A
high proportion
(?600/)
of
young
birds die each year, many
of
starvation, and it is believed that the majority of these
birds do not hold territories and that their starvation is directly attributable to this
factor (Hirons
et
al.
1979,
Hirons
1985).
Mortality amongst territory holders is less
than
15%
per annum. Once established in its territory (in Southern England, in the
preferred habitat type, approximatrely
12
ha) an individual owl (both male and
female alike) is likely to remain in that area all of its life (average life expectancy,
5
+
years: oldest ringed bird,
18
years
7
months). Territorial boundaries are defended
vigorously against neighbours and non-territorial intruders are excluded from
territories. However, if a territory becomes vacant it is normally re-occupied before
the adjacent resident birds expand into it.
The dietary spectrum of the Tawny Owl is broad. It seems that since Tawny
Owls forage exclusively within the boundaries
of
their territory, they must turn to
alternative prey items during the annual cycle. Thus, unlike the owls of similar body
weight, but which live in open habitats and tend to be specialist feeders (e.g., the
Short-Eared Owl), the Tawny Owls’ response to a shortage of prey of optimum size
(small mammals and birds) is not to forage further afield, but to forage within its
territory for smaller items, such as earthworms, which at certain times of the year can
form a substantial proportion of the diet (Southern
1970).
Not only do the birds remain in their territory throughout their life, but they are
also consistent in the way that the territory is used (Hardy
1977).
Thus, hunting,
roosting and feeding tend to take place at regularly-used perches. The hunting
technique is predominantly ‘perch-and-pounce’, where the owl waits for prey to
come into the vicinity of the chosen perch. It does not quarter for prey on the wing as
do owls
of
open habitats (e.g., the Short-Eared Owl) although hunting Tawny Owls
have been recorded as flying into a roost of small passerines (Cramp
1985).
Hunting
on the wing has been recorded in the Tawny Owl but this was apparently anomalous
behaviour (Nilson
1978).
It was recorded in summer at a high latitude when light
levels were higher than the normal nocturnal range.
It is also noteworthy that Southern
et
al.
(1954)
concluded that young Tawny
Owls, during the protracted period of dependence on their parents (approximately
2i-3
months post fledging), make little or no effort to feed themselves, but
concentrate on learning their way about the parental territory. Also, there is evidence
that even adult Tawny Owls may not always be able to negotiate obstacles within
their territory. Thus, when disturbed or surprised at night, Tawny Owls have been
known to fly into branches and even tree trunks (Hirons, pers. com.) thus suggesting
that vision was not sufficient to guide these birds about obstacles.
Conclusion
Territoriality and the nocturnal
habit
Many features
of
the natural history of the Tawny Owl becomes explicable in the
light of the sensory limitations and the behavioural and cognitive aspects
of
nocturnality discussed above. Thus, the high degree of territoriality may be
interpreted as essential to permit prey capture and general mobility when light levels
become limiting for the immediate visual guidance of flight and other behaviour.
To
275
stray out of the territory (for example in response to a shortage
of
optimal sized prey)
is of no advantage since
it
is specific knowledge of landmarks and the regularly used
perches that is essential for prey capture and movement under restricted sensory
input. For similar reasons invasion of an adjacent territory is
of
little value. The use
of the ‘perch-and-pounce’ hunting technique, employing a limited number of
perches, is also comprehensible, since the use of these regular perches will facilitate
the accumulation of the topographical knowledge required to mediate prey capture
using audition alone.
I
986
SENSORY CAPACITIES
OF
OWLS
The value
of
topographical knowledge
The adaptive function of avian territoriality is not generally understood, This is
because the proximate and ultimate factors underlying territoriality are not the same
in all species and indeed more than one factor may be operating simultaneously
(Wilson 1975, Davies 1978). Hinde (1956: 349) proposed that ‘familiarity with food
sources and refuges from predators’, could be regarded
as
one of the functions
of
territorial behaviour. However, most recent work on territoriality has tended to
favour analyses using metaphores drawn from economics in which emphasis has
been upon the costs and benefits involved in the defence of resources within the
territory.
Familiarity with food sources and refuges from predators are likely to be benefits
which accrue as the result of protracted residence in any one site. However, in the
case of the Tawny Owl it would seem that to be an habitual nocturnal species,
detailed topographical knowlege, gained through protracted residence in a restricted
area, is essential for survival. If an economical metaphor is to be employed, then
rather than regard the high degree of territorality in the Tawny Owl as involving only
the defence of a resource (average territory size in Tawny Owls is related primarily to
average abundance of prey within an area (Southern 1970, Hirons 1976, Hardy 1977,
Hirons 1985), and nest sites may sometimes be
a
limited resource (Lundberg 1979,
Wardhough 1984)), it might be convenient to view the situation as also involving the
‘protection of an investment’.
Defence of a resource implies that some commodity within the territory has a
value which can be readily appropriated by other individuals. However, topographi-
cal knowledge of a territory has value to the resident individual only. It cannot be
transferred to another individual nor can it be transferred by the resident to another
site. However, topographical knowledge, and hence the value of holding a particular
territory
is
likely to increase with length of residence in the same way that an
investment increases its value over time. (In the Tawny Owl Southern
&
Lowe
(1
986) have argued that continued occupancy of a territory results in greater hunting
skill. This
is
reflected in the data of Southern (1970) which suggest that the
probability of successful breeding increases with the length
of
time that the territory
has been occupied.) At some point the value of this investment may come to
outweigh that of the defended resources. Whether it does
so
for a particular species
or individual will depend upon many factors concerning the quality of the defended
resources and the value of topographical knowledge in the natural behaviour of the
species.
In the Tawny Owl, because of the necessity
of
detailed topographical knowledge
for nocturnal mobility and prey capture thoughout the year, the point at which the
value of this knowledge exceeds the value of the defended resources may be reached
frequently. The observations that resident Tawny Owls remain on territory and
accept a diversity of prey items, many of which are below optimal size, e.g.,
earthworms and beetles (Southern 1970) and/or use less favoured nest sites (e.g., a
276
G.
R.
MARTIN
IBIS
128
scrape on the ground rather than
a
hollow
tree
(Mikkola
1983)
may both be explained
in
these
terms.
I thank the following for their most helpful discussions and comments: J. Cohen, H. Kruuk, H. N.
Southern, K. Voous and
S.
R. Young.
References
BARLOW, H.B.
1981.
Critical limiting factors in the design of the eye and visual cortex. Proc.
R.
SOC. Lond.
BEVEN,
G.
1956.
The food of Tawny Owls in London. London Bird Rep.
29: 57-72.
BLOUGH, D.S.
1955.
Method for tracing dark adaptation in the pigeon. Science
121: 703-704.
BLOUGH,
P.M.
1971,
The visual acuity of the pigeon for distant targets. J. exp. Anal. Behav.
15:
57-68.
BOWMAKER, J.K.
&
DARTNALL, H.J.A.
1980.
Visual pigments of rods and cones in a human retina.
J.
BOWMAKER, J.K. &MARTIN, G.R.
1978.
Visual pigments and colour vision in a nocturnal bird,
Strix
aluco
BURTON, J.A.
1985.
Owls of the World (Second Edition). London: Lowe.
CRAMP,
S.
(ED.).
1985.
The birds of the Western Palearctic, Vol.
4.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
DAVIES, N.B.
1978.
Ecological questions about territorial behaviour.
In
Krebs, J.R.
&
Davies, N.B. (eds),
DICE, D.L.
1945.
Minimum intensities
of
illumination under which owls can find dead prey by sight. Am.
DIERCKS, K. J.
&
JEFFRESS, L.A.
1962.
Interaural phase and the absolute threshold for tone. J. Acoust.
SOC.
FITE, K.V.
1973.
Anatomical and behavioural correlates of visual acuity in the Great Horned Owl. Vision
FRISBY,
J.
1979.
Seeing, illusion, brain and mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
GREGORY, R.L.
1974.
Concepts and mechanisms of perception. London: Duckworth.
GRIFFIN, D.R.
&
SUTHERS, R.A.
1970.
Sensitivity of echolocation in cave swiftlets. Biol. Bull.
139: 495-
HARDY, A.R.
1977.
Hunting ranges and feeding ecology of owls in farmland. PhD Thesis, Aberdeen
HARRIS,
G.G.
1967.
Brownian motion in the cochlear partition. J. Acoust. SOC. Am.
44: 176-186.
HECHT,
S.
&
PIRENNE, M.H.
1940.
The sensitivity of the nocturnal LongEared
Owl
in the spectrum.
J.
HILLS, B.L.
1980.
Vision, visibility and perception in driving. Perception
9: 183-216.
HINDE, R.A.
1956.
The biological significance
of
the territories of birds.
Ibis
98: 34C-369.
HIRONS, G.J.M.
1976.
A population study of the Tawny Owl
(Stvzx
aluco)
and its main prey species in a
HIRONS, G.J.M.
1985.
The effects
of
territorial behaviour on the stability and dispersion of Tawny Owl
HIRONS, G., HARDY,
A.
&
STANLEY, P.
1979.
Starvation in young Tawny Owls. Bird Study
26: 59-63.
HIRSCH, J.
1982.
Falcon visual sensitivity to grating contrast. Nature (Lond).
300: 57-58.
HODOS,
W.
&
LEIBOWITZ,
R.W.
1977.
Near-vield visual acuity of pigeons: effects of scotopic adaptations
HODOS,
W., LEIBOWITZ, R.W.
&
BONBRIGHT, J.C.
1976.
Near-field visual acuityof pigeons: effects ofhead
KENWARD,
R.E.
1982.
Goshawk hunting behaviour, and range size as a function of food and habitat
KNUDSEN, E.I.
&
KONISHI, M.
1979.
Mechanisms of sound localization in the Barn Owl
(Tyto
alba).
J.
KNUDSEN, E.I., BLASDEL, G.G.
&
KONISHI, M.
1979.
Sound localization by the Barn
Owl
(Tyto
alba)
KONISHI, M.
1973.
How the
owl
tracks its prey. Am. Sci.
61: 414424.
KONISHI, M.
&
KNUDSEN, E.I.
1979.
The oil bird: hearing and echolocation. Science
204: 425427.
KRINOV, E.L.
1947.
Spectrol, naye otrazhatel ‘naya sposobnost’ prirodynykh obrazovanii. Izadaltel’sto
Akad.
U.S.S.R.
(translation by G. Belkov, National Research Council, Canada. Technical
Translation
TT-439, 1953).
LUNDBERG, A.
1979.
Residency, migration and a compromise: adaptations to nest-site scarcity and food
specialization in three Fenoscandian owl species. Oecologia. (Berl).
41: 273-281.
B.
212: 1-34.
Physiol. (Lond).
298:
501-511.
(Tawny Owl). Vision Res.
18:
1125-1 130.
Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach:
31 7-350.
Oxford: Blackwell.
Nat.
79: 384416.
Am.
34: 981-984.
Res.
13:
219-230.
501.
University.
Gen. Physiol.
23: 709-717.
woodland. D Phil Thesis, Oxford University.
(Stiix
aluco)
populations. J.
2001.
Lond. (in press).
and wavelength. Vision Res.
17: 463467.
position and stimulus. J. Exp. Anal. Behav.
25: 129-141.
availability. J. Anim. Ecol.
51: 69-80.
Comp. Physiol.
133: 13-21.
measured with the search coil technique. J. Comp. Physiol.
133:
1-11.
277
1986
SENSORY
CAPACITIES OF
OWLS
MARTIN, G.R.
1974.
Color vision in the Tawny Owl
(Szrix
aluco).
J.
Comp. Physiol. Psychol.
86:
133-
MARTIN, G.R.
1977.
Absolute visual threshold and scotopic spectral sensitivity in the tawny owl,
Strix
MARTIN, G.R.
1982.
An owl’s eye: schematic optics and visual performance in
Strix aluco.
L.
J.
Comp.
MARTIN, G.R.
1983.
Schematic eye models in vertebrates.
In:
Ottoson D. (ed). Progress in Sensory
MARTIN, G.R.
1984a.
The visual fields
of
the Tawny Owl,
Strix aluco
L. Vision Res.
24: 1739-1751.
MARTIN, G.R.
1984b.
An environmental limit on absolute auditory sensitivity in non-aquatic vertebrates.
MARTIN, G.R.
&
GORDON, I.E.
1974a.
Increment-threshold spectral sensitivity in the Tawny Owl
(Strix
MARTIN, G.R.
&
GORDON, I.E.
1974b.
Visual acuity in the Tawny Owl
(Strix aluco).
vision Res.
14: 1393-
MARTIN,
G.R.,
GORDON, I.E.
&
CADLE, D.R.
1975.
Electroretinographically
determined spectral
MARQUIS, M.
&
NEWTON,
I.
1981.
A radio-tracking study of the ranging behaviour and dispersion of
MASTERTON, B., HEFFNER, H.
&
RAVIZZA, R.
1969.
The evolution of human hearing.
J.
Acoust. Soc. Am.
MATTHEWS, L.H.
&
MATTHEWS, B.H.C.
1939.
Owls and infra-red radiation. Nature (Lond).
143: 983.
MIKKOLA, H.
1983.
Owls of Europe. Calton: Poyser.
MILLS, A.W.
1958.
On the minimum audible angle.
J.
Acoust.
SOC.
Am.
30:
237-246.
NEWTON,
I.
1979.
Population Ecology
of
Raptors. Berkhampstead: Poyser.
NILSSON,
I.N.
1978.
Hunting in flight by Tawny Owl
Strix
alum.
Ibis
120: 528-531.
NORBERG,
R.A.
1977.
Occurrence and independent evolution of bilateral ear assyrnetry in owls and
implications on owl taxonomy. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B.
280: 375408.
NORBERG, R.A.
1978.
Skull asymmetry, ear structure and function, and auditory localization in
Tengmalm’s owl,
Aegolius funereus
(Linne). Phil. Trans. Roy.
SOC.
Lond. B.
282: 325410.
PAYNE, R.S.
1971.
Acoustic location
of
prey by Barn Owls
(Tyto
ah).
J.
Exp. Biol.
54: 535-573.
PIRENNE, M.H.
1962.
Absolute thresholds and quantum effects:
In
Davson
H
(ed) The Eye, Vol
2: 123-
PIRENNE, M.H., MARRIOTT, F.H.C.
&
O’DOHERTY,
E.F.
1957.
Individual differences in night vision
ROEFFLER,
S.K.
&BUTLER, R.A.
1968.
Factors that influence the localization of sound in the vertical plane.
SCHULTZE,
M.
1867.
Uber Stabchen und Zapfen der Retina. Archiv
fur
mikroskopische anatomie und
SHLAER,
S.
1937.
The relation between visual acuity and illumination.
J.
Gen. Physiol.
21: 165-188
SIVIAN, L.J.
&
WHITE, S.D.
1933.
On minimal audible sound fields.
J.
Acoust. SOC. Am.
4: 288-321.
SNYDER,
A.W., LAUGHLIN, S.B.
&
STAVENGA,
D.G.
1977.
Information capacity of eyes. Vision Res.
17:
SOUTHERN, H.N.
1970
The natural control of a population of tawny owls
(Strzx
aluco).
J.
2001.
Lond.
162:
SOUTHERN, H.N.
&
LOWE, V.P.
1968.
The pattern and distribution of prey and predation in Tawny Owl
SOUTHERN, H.N., VAUGHAN, R.
&
MUIR, R.C.
1954.
The behaviour of young Tawny Owls after fledging.
TANSLEY,
K.
1965.
Vision in vertebrates. London: Chapman and Hall.
VANDERPLANCK, F.L.
1934.
The effect
of
infra-red waves on Tawny Owls
(Strix
alum).
Proc. Zool. Soc.
WALLS, G.L.
1942.
The vertebrate eye and its adaptive radiation. Michigan: Cranbrook Institute of
WARDHOUGH, A.A.
1984.
Wintering strategies of British Owls. Bird Study
31:
76-77.
WILSON, E.O.
1975.
Sociobiology. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard Univ. Press.
Wood, C.A.
1917.
The fundus oculi of birds. Chicago: Lakeside Press.
WRIGHT, A.A.
1979.
Color-vision psychgophysics: a comparison of pigeon and human.
In
Granda, A.M.
&Maxwell, J.H. (eds). Neural mechanisms of Behaviour in the Pigeon:
89-127.
London: New York:
Plenum.
142.
aluco.
Nature (Lond).
268: 636-638.
Physiol.
145: 341-349.
Physiology, Vol.
4: 43-81.
Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer.
Behav. Process.
9: 205-221.
aluco).
Vision Res.
14: 615-620.
1397.
sensitivity in the Tawny Owl
(Strix aluco).
J.
Comp. Physiol. Psychol.
89: 72-78.
European Sparrowhawks
Accipiter
nisus.
J.
Anim. Ecol.
51:
111-133.
45: 966-985.
140.
New York: Academic Press.
efficiency. Med. Res. Counc. GB. Spec. Rep. Ser.
294.
J.
Acoust.
SOC.
Am.
43: 1255-1259.
entrwicklungsmechanik
3:
21 5-247.
1163-117s.
197-285.
territories.
J.
Anim. Ecol.
37: 75-97.
Bird Study
1:
101
-1
10.
Lond.
505-507.
Science.
... Because species living in dense habitats experience lower light intensities [61], these species might also need larger binocular fields. Large binocular fields are in part associated with larger eyes in owls [62,63], suggesting that species living in dense habitats may have larger eyes relative to body mass. This is in part confirmed by this study, with the ratio of eye axial length and body mass differing among species from different habitats (0.075 ± 0.021, 0.055 ± 0.022 and 0.055 ± 0.029 for dense (n = 5), semi-open (n = 6) and open (n = 3) habitats, respectively). ...
... Compared to other birds, owls have numerous binocularly driven neurons [73], which may be a result of their combined use of vision and hearing, using their uniquely elaborate outer ears for accurate auditory localization of prey [11,14,62]. The more frontal position of owl eyes (even though they are still laterally placed [2]) likely results from selection for large eyes with high visual sensitivity [62,63] combined with selection for large outer ear structures [14]. ...
... Compared to other birds, owls have numerous binocularly driven neurons [73], which may be a result of their combined use of vision and hearing, using their uniquely elaborate outer ears for accurate auditory localization of prey [11,14,62]. The more frontal position of owl eyes (even though they are still laterally placed [2]) likely results from selection for large eyes with high visual sensitivity [62,63] combined with selection for large outer ear structures [14]. Such elaborate outer ear structures are not found in other nocturnal birds (such as plovers and oilbirds), whose eyes are placed more laterally and therefore have narrower binocular fields [2]. ...
Article
Full-text available
The binocular field of vision differs widely in birds depending on ecological traits such as foraging. Owls (Strigiformes) have been considered to have a unique binocular field, but whether it is related to foraging has remained unknown. While taking into account allometry and phylogeny, we hypothesized that both daily activity cycle and diet determine the size and shape of the binocular field in owls. Here, we compared the binocular field configuration of 23 species of owls. While we found no effect of allometry and phylogeny, ecological traits strongly influence the binocular field shape and size. Binocular field shape of owls significantly differed from that of diurnal raptors. Among owls, binocular field shape was relatively conserved, but binocular field size differed among species depending on ecological traits, with larger binocular fields in species living in dense habitat and foraging on invertebrates. Our results suggest that (i) binocular field shape is associated with the time of foraging in the daily cycle (owls versus diurnal raptors) and (ii) that binocular field size differs between closely related owl species even though the general shape is conserved, possibly because the field of view is partially restricted by feathers, in a trade-off with auditory localization.
... Fowl have extremely laterally placed eyes, resulting in pronounced visual fields. In fact, the mallard duck has been reported to have total panoramic vision in the horizontal plane, meaning a 360-degree field of view (Martin 1986)! Other ducks have similarly been shown to have wide visual fields (Martin et al. 2007;Guillemain et al. 2002), and Canada geese were reported to have 334 degrees of total visual coverage in the horizontal plane (Fernandez-Juricic et al. 2011). ...
... Other ducks have similarly been shown to have wide visual fields (Martin et al. 2007;Guillemain et al. 2002), and Canada geese were reported to have 334 degrees of total visual coverage in the horizontal plane (Fernandez-Juricic et al. 2011). As a result, small binocular fields are noted (20-22 degrees) (Martin 1986;Fernandez-Juricic et al. 2011;Martin et al. 2007;Guillemain et al. 2002). Eye movement has also been shown to be limited in these species, with the Canada goose having an average of about 13 degrees of eye movement across all directions around the head (Fernandez-Juricic et al. 2011). ...
... Kiwis are cursorial nocturnal birds that forage for earthworms and other surface-dwelling invertebrates, grubs, fallen fruit, and native plants. Their visual fields are among the smallest reported in birds and have features found in birds whose foraging is guided by nonvisual cues (Martin 1986;Martin et al. 2007;Corfield et al. 2015). Kiwis present a very small frontal binocular area, a small monocular area (125 degrees) and a very large blind sector behind the head. ...
Chapter
The chapter will focus on the ophthalmology of members of the superorder Galloanserae (fowl), which includes both those that are strictly terrestrial and those that spend time in water. Landfowl (Galliformes) include the brushturkey and scrubfowl (Megapodiidae), the guans and curassows (Cracidae), guineafowl (Numididae), quail (Odontophoridae), and the chickens, turkeys, pheasants, partridges, and grouse (Phasianidae). Waterfowl (Anseriformes) include the screamers (Anhimidae), the magpie goose (Anseranatidae), and the ducks, geese, and swans (Anatidae). The term “poultry” refers to members of the Galloanserae that have potential commercial use for its meat, eggs, offal, feathers, and manure, directly or indirectly entering the human food chain, regardless of the actual use of individual birds of each species. Poultry largely consist of chickens (Gallus gallus), quails (Coturnix coturnix), turkeys (Meleagris gallopavo), pheasants (Phasianus colchicus), and ducks (Anas platyrhynchos). The largest part of this chapter is about chickens (Gallus gallus domesticus), a subspecies of the red jungle fowl, a Southeast Asian cousin of pheasants that was domesticated more than 7,000 years ago. Chickens are technically considered domestic animals, although there are places where wild (or feral) chickens are common, such as in Hawaii and parts of the world where jungle fowl are still wild. When these patients are kept as pets and are brought to the veterinarian, usually avian veterinarians will examine them, not poultry or farm animal veterinarians.
... Perhaps a most obvious example is revealed in their activity patterns, where most raptors are considered diurnal, except the owls, which are considered nocturnal, a habit they share with two other avian orders for which we have genome sequences (Caprimulgiformes and Apterygiformes) (Martin 1990;Mikkola 1983;Martin 2017;Zhan et al. 2013). However, the ferruginous pygmy owl Glaucidium brasilianum maintains some activity during the day and the snowy owl Bubo scandiacus hunts during the daytime, the American barn owl Tyto furcate is most active in twilight, the burrowing owl Athene cunicularia is cathemeral, and the stripped owl Asio clamator is crepuscular and nocturnal (del Hoyo et al. 1999;Martins and Egler 1991;Motta-Junior 2006;Motta-Junior & de Arruda Bueno 2004;Sarasola and Santillan 2014;Martin 1986a). Overall, as a group, owls actually exhibit a broad range of activity patterns and habitats (Bowmaker and Martin 1978;Braga 2006). ...
... For the owls, despite having large eyes, spatial resolution is comparably poor (Howland et al. 2004;Ghim and Hodos 2006;Wathey and Pettigrew 1989;Harmening et al. 2007b;Martin and Gordon 1974a). The optics of an owl eye is designed, rather, to enhance retinal image brightness, using a rod-dominated retina to maximize retinal information convergence and thus sensitivity (Martin 1986a;Walls 1942;Bohórquez Mahecha and Aparecida de Oliveira 1998;Lisney et al. 2012, Fite 1973, Martin 1990, Oehme 1961, Walls 1942, Schaeffel and Wagner 1996. ...
Chapter
Birds of prey, also collectively known as raptors, consist of the Falconiformes (falcons and caracaras), Accipitriformes (eagles, buzzards, hawks, kites, and Old World vultures), Cathartiformes (New World vultures), Cariamiformes (seriemas), and Strigiformes (true owls Strigidae, and barn owls Tytonidae) (del Hoyo et al. 1999; Jarvis et al. 2014; McClure et al. 2019). Although grouped together as key apex predators, raptors are phylogenetically heterogenous assimilation with many morphological and ecological differences. Perhaps a most obvious example is revealed in their activity patterns, where most raptors are considered diurnal, except the owls, which are considered nocturnal, a habit they share with two other avian orders for which we have genome sequences (Caprimulgiformes and Apterygiformes) (Martin 1990; Mikkola 1983; Martin 2017; Zhan et al. 2013). However, the ferruginous pygmy owl Glaucidium brasilianum maintains some activity during the day and the snowy owl Bubo scandiacus hunts during the daytime, the American barn owl Tyto furcate is most active in twilight, the burrowing owl Athene cunicularia is cathemeral, and the stripped owl Asio clamator is crepuscular and nocturnal (del Hoyo et al. 1999; Martins and Egler 1990; Motta-Junior 2006; Motta-Junior et al. 2004; Sarasola and Santillan 2014; Martin 1986). Overall, as a group, owls actually exhibit a broad range of activity patterns and habitats (Bowmaker and Martin 1978; Braga 2006). Additionally, nearly one-third of falconiform species, as well as some accipitriformes species, maintain activity during the crepuscular period (Mitkus et al. 2018).
... We are not aware of a single study on eyes or vision of the two species of cariamiforms, but many studies have focused on owls ( [4,5], and references therein) as well as falconiform, accipitriform and cathartiform raptors (examples in [6]). Although some owl species hunt during daytime (e.g. the Snowy owl (Bubo scandiacus), Burrowing owl (Athene cunicularia) and some Barn owls (Tyto spp.); [7]) and a third of falconiform and some accipitriform species may have crepuscular habits (see [6] for a review), owls are generally considered a clade with predominantly nocturnal activity [5,8,9], whereas most species in the other orders are mainly diurnal. ...
... The high sensitivity of owl eyes is partly due to their low F-numbers [14,15] or, anatomically, large corneal diameters relative to axial lengths ( Fig. 2; [13,16]). In diurnal raptors, smaller corneal diameters and large axial lengths lead to large retinal images and thus, high spatial resolution, but lower sensitivity ( Fig. 2; [4,17]). ...
Article
Full-text available
Raptors have always fascinated mankind, owls for their highly sensitive vision, and eagles for their high visual acuity. We summarize what is presently known about the eyes as well as the visual abilities of these birds, and point out knowledge gaps. We discuss visual fields, eye movements, accommodation, ocular media transmittance, spectral sensitivity, retinal anatomy and what is known about visual pathways. The specific adaptations of owls to dim-light vision include large corneal diameters compared to axial (and focal) length, a rod-dominated retina and low spatial and temporal resolution of vision. Adaptations of diurnal raptors to high acuity vision in bright light include rod- and double cone-free foveae, high cone and retinal ganglion cell densities and high temporal resolution. We point out that more studies, preferably using behavioural and non-invasive methods, are desirable.
... For nocturnal owls, limited light means listening for auditory cues given off by prey is often the primary sensory modality by which an owl hunts (Martin, 1990). For nocturnal prey, such as a mouse, detecting and responding to auditory cues from an approaching owl may be the best means of escape (Payne, 1971;Martin, 1986). Therefore, there are two hypotheses for the function of quiet flight: stealth and reduced self-masking. ...
Article
Owls have evolved sensitive hearing facilitated by a facial disc, and flight that is quieted in part by a leading-edge comb on their wing. This comb is a series of modified barbs, or serrations, which project up from the outermost primary feathers on the leading edge of the wing. Here we explore the evolution of comb and facial disc morphology. We measured leading-edge comb morphology on museum skins of 147 owl species, and facial disc morphology from photos, as well as ecological traits, on 66 species. The first principal component of comb morphology loaded on serration length, which varied between 0 and ~6 mm long in the species we sampled. Comb size (PC1) was correlated with relative facial disc size, suggesting that owls with good hearing also tend to have quiet flight. Two non-exclusive hypotheses for why quiet flight evolved are for stealth, allowing the owl to approach prey undetected; or to reduce self-masking, enabling the owl to hear prey better midflight. We examined whether ecological variables (prey type, active period and habitat) suggest whether stealth or self-masking better explain the evolution of comb size. Phylogenetic analyses suggested support for both the stealth and the self-masking hypotheses for the evolution of quiet flight.
... Kiwi behavior differs significantly from other paleognaths as well. They are almost entirely nocturnal (Heather and Robertson 2005), a behavior that is found in less than 3% of all avian species and none of the other paleognaths (Le Duc et al. 2015;Martin 1986). They are also fossorial, meaning they are adapted to digging and nest and shelter in underground burrows (Sales 2005). ...
Chapter
Kiwi (Apteryx spp.) are highly threatened flightless birds endemic to New Zealand. They are members of the most basal extant avian lineage, the paleognaths, and exhibit a suite of traits that are unusual in birds. Despite their iconic and imperiled status, there have been only four genomic studies of kiwi to date with only two of these aimed at improving conservation. There is, therefore, massive opportunity to use genomic techniques to elucidate the genetic basis and consequences of the strange ecology and evolution of kiwi and to inform their intensive management. In this chapter, we review genomic studies in paleognaths, assess prospects for the future of kiwi genomics, and define some lessons for population genomics and conservation of at-risk taxa generally. We also present an analysis of genomic signatures associated with the evolution of Apterygidae and the genes involved in diversification of kiwi via comparison of 3,774 orthologous protein coding genes among 28 avian species. We found strong signals of selection in genes associated with dwarfism, neurogenesis, retinal development, and temperature regulation. Our results provide clues as to why kiwi have such small body size (relative to other paleognaths), large egg size (relative to their body size), excellent olfaction, and poor vision. The data further suggest that coping with highly divergent temperature regimes may be a defining feature of the spotted kiwi clade which includes the only kiwi species that inhabits the alpine zone. Considerable genomic resources are now available for kiwi, including whole-genome sequences, transcriptome assemblies, thousands of SNP markers, and numerous candidate genes. There is also a myriad of outstanding questions about kiwi that genomic studies can inform. The challenge now is to bring these new genomic tools to bear on conservation and management of kiwi.
... Strictly nocturnal owls hunting under the forest canopy may mitigate this by employing a sit and wait hunting strategy (Martin 2017). By utilizing a small number of perches in a small territory, owls may rely on spatial memory of their environment to avoid collisions with large objects (Martin 1986;. Nevertheless, owls collide with environmental objects such as adhesive vegetation (Palmer and others 2009;Rodríguez and others 2009), or anthropogenic structures such as fences (Allen and Ramirez 1990), and electrical lines (Ii 2005). ...
Article
Full-text available
Synopsis We raise and explore possible answers to three questions about the evolution and ecology of silent flight of owls: (1) do owls fly silently for stealth, or is it to reduce self-masking? Current evidence slightly favors the self-masking hypothesis, but this question remains unsettled. (2) Two of the derived wing features that apparently evolved to suppress flight sound are the vane fringes and dorsal velvet of owl wing feathers. Do these two features suppress aerodynamic noise (sounds generated by airflow), or do they instead reduce structural noise, such as frictional sounds of feathers rubbing during flight? The aerodynamic noise hypothesis lacks empirical support. Several lines of evidence instead support the hypothesis that the velvet and fringe reduce frictional sound, including: the anatomical location of the fringe and velvet, which is best developed in wing and tail regions prone to rubbing, rather than in areas exposed to airflow; the acoustic signature of rubbing, which is broadband and includes ultrasound, is present in the flight of other birds but not owls; and the apparent relationship between the velvet and friction barbules found on the remiges of other birds. (3) Have other animals also evolved silent flight? Wing features in nightbirds (nocturnal members of Caprimulgiformes) suggest that they may have independently evolved to fly in relative silence, as have more than one diurnal hawk (Accipitriformes). We hypothesize that bird flight is noisy because wing feathers are intrinsically predisposed to rub and make frictional noise. This hypothesis suggests a new perspective: rather than regarding owls as silent, perhaps it is bird flight that is loud. This implies that bats may be an overlooked model for silent flight. Owl flight may not be the best (and certainly, not the only) model for “bio-inspiration” of silent flight.
Article
Background: In Iraq have been collected 10 Falcon (Falco columbarius) and 10 Owl (Bubo bubo ) to obtain their brain (optic lobe) and histological comparative between them. Method: In recent study have been collected specimens and used hematoxylin and eosin stain and used silver stain to compare between them. Result: Our result show weight of brain of falcon was less than owl (4.2± 0.04830 , 4.7± 0. 05164) respectively while weight of optic lobe of falcon was more than owl (1.2±0.05270 , 1.1± 0.05164) respectively. Optic lobe of falcon was consist of six layers with thickness (770.7 ±0.48305 ) while owl was consist of three layers only with (707.7±0.48305). Conclusion: brain weight of falcon is less than owl because of long distance which be fly and weight of optic lobe of falcon is more than owl because it has high vision more than owl
Article
In this study we investigated the eye morphology and retinal topography of two owl species in relation to their visual environment. Although Short-eared Owl Asio flammeus is larger and weighs more than Japanese Scops Owl Otus semitorques, its eye dimensions (weight, corneal diameter, and axial length) are all smaller than the scops owl's. Owl retinas were examined in Nissl-stained whole-mount preparations. The total number of retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) was greater in the Japanese Scops Owl (4,703.0103 cells) than in the Short-eared Owl (2,346.8103 cells). The eye morphology of, and the number of RGCs in the Japanese Scops Owl indicate that it is more adapted to a nocturnal habit. An area of high-density RGCs was horizontally distributed in the temporal retina of the Short-eared Owl, with a peak density of 17.4103 cells/mm2. In the Japanese Scops Owl's temporal retina, there was an oval-shaped arrangement with a peak density of 23.1103 cells/mm2. These distributions indicate that whereas Short-eared Owl is adapted to open habitats, Japanese Scops Owl is adapted to enclosed habitats. The RGCs of both species were classified into three categories (small, medium, and large) based on the size and appearance of somas. In both species, medium-sized cells predominated, and the proportion of large-sized cells was smallest. The distribution and high-density areas differed among these groups, suggesting adaptation to the visual environment. The high-density areas of these groups cover the nasal visual fields, which include the overlap for binocular vision.
Chapter
This chapter reviews and summarizes the ophthalmological knowledge of the Palaeognathae, an avian clade that includes ostriches, rheas, emus, cassowaries, tinamous, and kiwis. It starts with a detailed description of their ocular anatomical and physiological particularities. Then, it explains how to perform a complete ocular examination and the diagnostic tests and procedures already reported. Finally, it describes the most common ophthalmic infectious and noninfectious disorders, how to diagnose them and, when possible, how to treat them.
Article
Full-text available
(1) The ranging behaviour of sparrowhawks was studied by radio-tracking in two areas 12 km apart, one rich in prey and the other poor. (2) In both areas, ranges of adults centered around nesting areas (and in winter around nearby roosts) in woodland. (3) Range size was smallest when hawks were attending the nest, from pre-lay through to incubation/brooding, and largest outside the breeding season; it was most variable at seasons when range size was rapidly changing, in early spring and (for hens) in the late nestling and post-fledging periods. The ranges of cocks were on average smaller than those of hens, and for the same sex and season, ranges were greater in the area with low prey density than in the area with high prey density. (4) In their first winter, both cocks and hens were less faithful to any particular roost than were adults; they had more roost sites and used the main site less often. There was a suggestion that adults used their main roost less often when their ranges were large. (5) When hunting, sparrowhawks used woodland more than expected from its proportion in their ranges, particularly broadleaved woodland, and hens used open country (especially farmland) more than cocks. Adults mostly roosted in thick conifer woodland near nesting areas, and returned repeatedly to the same sites, whereas first-year birds in winter roosted in a variety of small woods in open country, both during the day and overnight. (6) At some stages of breeding, range parameters could be related to breeding performance. In the pre-lay period, cocks which had small ranges and spent much time near the nesting area became successfully mated, whereas cocks which had large ranges and spent little time near the nesting area remained unmated. In the late nestling period, the range size of hens was negatively correlated with the growth of their young. Provision by us of supplementary food in both periods resulted in hens becoming more sedentary at the nest. (7) Between members of a pair in summer, and adjacent birds of the opposite sex in winter, a high proportion (74%) of their locations occurred in the overlapping part of their ranges. Much less overlap occurred between adjacent birds of the same sex, and for adults the overlap was relatively greater between larger ranges. For any given altitude, the ranges of adjacent breeding cocks in the prelay period were mutually exclusive at approximately the level predicted from a previously established relationship between nest density and altitude. For any given range size and altitude, first-year cocks overlapped their range with adjacent birds more than did adults.
Article
Full-text available
The ear apertures in the skin of Tengmalm's owl, Aegolius funereus (Linne) (Strigiformes), are slit-like and ca. 24 mm long. This equals the height of the skull. The ear opening in the skin is bounded by a continuous fold of skin that is developed into a preaural and a postaural flap. The preaural flap carries the facial disk feathers that are structurally specialized to be sound transparent. They form a multi-layered, but sparse and delicate web over the ear opening. The postaural flap carries very densely packed feathers that form an anteriorly concave facial ruff. The ear folds (flaps) and the ear slit in the skin of one ear exhibit bilateral symmetry relative to those of the other ear, but because of asymmetry of underlying skull bones the postaural folds come to be oriented in somewhat different ways in the left and right ear. The skull bones, their constellation, and their role in the bilateral skull asymmetry are described in skulls at various stages of development (12-25 days post-hatching). Inside the ear aperture in the skin lies the smaller ear aperture in the skull. This is ca. 11 mm high. The ear and skull asymmetry reaches its maximum at the ear apertures of the skull. Hence the right ear aperture of the skull lies ca. 6.5 mm higher than the left one. Viewed from in front, a line connecting the centres of the ear apertures deviates 12^circ from the horizontal. The asymmetry then decreases towards the posterior parts of the external auditory meatuses, and the flattened meatus parts extended over the eardrum exhibit complete bilateral symmetry. As projected on the vertical median plane of the head, an axis through the centre of the eardrum and the centre of the ear aperture of one ear gives an angle of vertical divergence of ca. 40^circ with the corresponding, projected, axis of the other ear. The tympanic ring, the eardrum, the middle ear, the stapedial complex, and the bony cochlea and semicircular canals exhibit complete bilateral symmetry. However, one pair of the three pairs of air spaces communicating with the middle ear, namely the superior air space, is of different shape on the two sides. The skull bones participating in the asymmetry are: the orbitosphenoid, squamosal, parietal, frontal, and the squamoso-occipital wing. Of the jaw muscles the M. depressor mandibulae and the aponeurosis 1 portion of M. adductor mandibulae externus exhibit pronounced bilateral asymmetry. Both muscles are related to the highly asymmetrical squamoso-occipital wings. The combined volume of the middle ear cavity and the air spaces communicating with it is ca. 730 mm^3 in one ear. This is almost as much as the volume of the external auditory meatus, which is about 830 mm^3. The large volume of air inside the eardrum should result in (1) a lowering of the resonant frequency of the middle ear, and (2) a lowering of impedance in the stiffness controlled frequency region below the resonance frequency, with a corresponding increase in transmission of these frequencies to the cochlea. An ecological consequence to the owl of lowered middle ear impedance, and hence threshold of hearing at low frequencies, is an improvement of the owl's ability in far range detection of sounds containing low frequencies, such as rustling sounds made by prey moving about in vegetation. While high frequencies are potentially more useful for sound localization than low ones, low frequencies are less attenuated by air and less diffracted and reflected by vegetation, and therefore travel farther and are more useful for detection of sound at some distance. The area ratio between the eardrum and the footplate of stapes is 35.3, which is a high value for a bird. The stapedial complex consists of two functional units that perform two different, but interrelated, movement patterns. One unit is formed by the extracolumella and the Ligamentum ascendens. This unit is rigid and rotates about its axis of rotation at the rim of the tympanic ring. It is the functional equivalent to the mammalian ear ossicles malleus and incus. The other unit is the bony stapes. It performs a pistonlike motion and corresponds to the mammalian stapes. Because of the oblique orientation of the stapedial complex relative to the plane of the tympanic ring, the force lever arm becomes longer than the resistance arm. The maximum transformer ratio attainable by the stapedial complex amounts to 1.6. The combined transformer action, due to the area ratio and the transformer action of the stapedial complex, thus becomes 56 (the possible curved-membrane effect not included). The middle ear transformer ratio thus seems to be close to the optimal one (ca. 65), i.e. that resulting in maximum pressure transfer to the inner ear. The external ears are bilaterally asymmetrical in the vertical plane. This strongly suggests that the asymmetry is linked to vertical directional hearing. The mere fact that there is a bilateral asymmetry of the external ears strongly suggests that vertical directional hearing is based on binaural comparison of signals from the two ears. Indeed, the entire asymmetry would seem meaningless as to auditory localization, if the information processing at the neural level were not based on binaural comparison. It is suggested that the remarkably large height of the symmetrical ear slits in the skin serves the purpose of extending the effect of ear asymmetry to lower frequency domains. Data from acoustical measurements show that the vertical sensitivity pattern is different between the left and right ear for frequencies above 6000 Hz. This demonstrates that the ear asymmetry in Aegolius is capable of producing excellent physical cues to vertical localization of sound. A hypothesis on localization of complex noise is given; it suggests that the owl performs a binaural comparison of spectral pattern. Low frequencies (below 6000 Hz) provide intensity cues to azimuth, high frequencies intensity cues to elevation angle. In theory, the median plane ambiguity, inherent to symmetrical ears, is removed by the bilateral asymmetry. This is because the asymmetry, at some frequencies, causes interaural intensity differences at most elevation angles in the median plane. The use of interaural differences in spectral pattern in auditory localization, would remove also the problem of distinguishing 'what' from 'where', i.e. the uncertainty as to whether a specific spectral pattern should be attributed to the sound source or to a direction-dependent spectral transformation imposed by head and ear. The ear asymmetry provides the cue in the vertical plane. The hypothesis on auditory localization is summarized in a simple mathematical expression.
Article
Full-text available
On the basis of the literature and my own examination of living and/or dead but fresh owls of 16 species, bilateral asymmetry of external ears in owls is surveyed and ear structure briefly described. Consideration of the probability of origin of various structural similarities and dissimilarities in the ear leads to the conclusion that ear asymmetry has evolved independently in at least five lines, represented by the respective genera (1) Tyto, (2) Phodilus, (3) Bubo, Ciccaba, Strix, (4) Rhinoptynx, Asio, Pseudoscops, and (5) Aegolius. Bubo, Ciccaba, and Strix probably represent more than one line of origin of ear asymmetry. Available evidence suggests that bilateral ear asymmetry in owls serves to make the vertical directional sensitivity patterns different between the two ears for high frequencies, thus making possible vertical localization based on binaural comparison of intensity and spectral composition of sound. When an owl localizes prey by hearing, the direction of the source usually forms a shallow angle with the ground. Therefore, a certain angle of error usually converts into a longer distance along the ground for a vertical error than for a horizontal error. This is a crucial factor that calls for good vertical localization ability of owls which rely on hearing for localization of food. Selection pressure for improvement of the ability of vertical localization of sound is believed to lie behind the evolution of all types of bilateral ear asymmetry in owls. On the basis of comparative ear structure the current subdivision of family Strigidae into subfamily Buboninae and Striginae is rejected. The external ears of Rhinoptynx and Pseudoscops are described for the first time and shown to be very similar to those of Asio otus, demonstrating affinity between these three genera.
Article
Five descriptive parameters of hearing—high‐frequency and low‐frequency sensitivity, lowest threshold. best frequency, and area of the audible field—are compared statistically, first, among mammals in general, and then, among seven animals selected to approximate a phylogenetic sequence of man's ancestors. Three potentially explanatory parameters body size, maximum binaural time disparity, and recency of common ancestry with man—are also explicitly included in the analysis. The results show that: high‐frequency hearing (above 32 kHz) is a characteristic unique to mammals, and, among members of this class, one which is commonplace and primitive. Being highly correlated with functionally close‐set ears, it is probably the result of selective pressure for accurate sound localization. Low‐frequency hearing improved markedly in mankind's line of descent, but the kind and degree of improvement are not unique among mammalian lineages. High sensitivity developed in the earliest stages of man's lineage and has remained relatively unchanged since the simian level. The frequency of the lowest threshold has declined in Man's lineage—the greatest drop probably occurring during the Eocene. The total area of the audible field increased until the Eocene and has decreased since then.
Article
(1) Four released goshawks were radio-tracked for up to 29 days at a time in Oxfordshire. Their hunting flights were mostly at 3--4 min intervals, for an average of 200 m in open country. They remained in woodland for 50% of the time although only 12% of their ranges was wooded. This preference resulted not from hawks flying less frequently in woodland, but because they flew half the distance between perches and doubled back twice as often in woodland as in open country. (2) Most attacks were initiated from perches, and only 3% were at prey already in flight. Six percent of observed attacks were successful, but hawks were most successful when hunting out of sight. They killed, on average, once in every 262 minutes of hunting. Seventy percent of prey was taken in or from woodland, a higher proportion than expected from the time spent there. (3) There were no sex or age differences in the preference for woodland of twenty-two wild goshawks radio-tracked in Sweden. Woodland within 200 m of open country was the most preferred habitat, and the majority of kills were made there. Range size was related to the proportion of a range that was woodland edge, and to prey availability. It is suggested that hawks covered the amount of woodland edge which gave adequate kills at the prevailing prey density, range size then being the area which happened to include that amount of woodland edge.
Article
1. Parallel population studies were done on a predator, the tawny owl (Strix aluco), and on its two main prey species, the wood mouse (Apodemus sylvaticus) and the bank vole (Clethrionomys glareolus) in deciduous woodland on the Wytham Estate, near Oxford, from October 1954 to August 1956. 2. The study area of 260 ac (104 ha), in Wytham Great Wood, contained a trapping grid of 120 ac (49 ha) on which, approximately every 2 months, mice and voles were caught, marked with numbered metal leg-rings and released. On this area the tawny owl population, consisting of eleven pairs, was censused and their breeding success recorded; only two of them had territories entirely within the trapping grid. 3. The territorial boundaries of these owls were mapped from observations made at dusk. A check on the observations was rendered possible by searching for pellets regurgitated by the owls and containing the indigestible parts of their prey, including marking rings. Of 1992 rodents marked, 160 were traced subsequently in pellets. 4. Since the home ranges of the rodents are small, the distribution of the locations where the recovered rings had originally been put on the prey delimits the areas in which each pair of owls hunted. The territory boundaries indicated from these data show very close agreement with the boundaries plotted from observations. This confirmed the validity of the observational method, which was being used for long-term census work on the whole estate (1000 ac, 405 ha). 5. A map of the trapping grid was prepared in terms of four categories of density of ground vegetation, ranging from almost bare ground under heavy canopy, where hunting is easy, to ground covered with a dense and high growth of bracken and bramble where hunting would be very difficult. 6. The trapping results showed that about five wood mice per ac were caught in each of the cover categories. The catches of bank voles were of this order in the barest covertype but increased with density of vegetation until they were three times as numerous in the thick bracken-bramble cover. 7. The recovery of marking rings from owl pellets showed that the owls caught bank voles in the four cover-types in the same proportions as did the traps. However, of wood mice they caught proportionately many more than the traps on the bare areas and many less than the traps in the densely vegetated areas. There was no evidence of selection by the owls between the sexes or size classes of the prey. 8. These results suggest that owl territories with plenty of bare ground will provide conditions for the most successful hunting when wood mice are abundant, whereas the territories in which the intermediate cover types predominate will be better when bank voles are abundant. 9. During the period of study both species of prey were scarce in the 1955 breeding season, whereas in the 1956 season the voles had increased in numbers markedly, the wood mice only slightly. 10. In 1955 only two pairs of tawny owls on the area studied even attempted to breed, though both fledged young successfully. In 1956 seven pairs attempted to breed, of which three fledged young. Breeding performance is a sensitive index to availability of prey in spring. 11. A comparison of breeding performance in these 2 years with features of each territory suggests that, in addition to the level of prey density, other characteristics, viz. territory size, the distribution of ground cover, and the previous experience of territory owners all contribute significantly to breeding success.
Article
The present study agrees with earlier ones that the binaural absolute threshold is about 3 dB lower than the monaural. It also finds that reversing the interaural phase of the signal lowers the threshold still further. The findings are shown to indicate the likelihood that so‐called absolute thresholds are really masked thresholds, with the masking noise present internally and exhibiting a small positive correlation. The close relation of our results to the earlier work of Hirsh on binaural masking phenomena is discussed.
Article
All four species of owls studied in the laboratory found their dead prey mostly by sight and there was no evidence of their employment for this purpose of infra-red rays or of any senses other than sight and physical contact. The barred, long-eared, and barn owls are able, under the most favorable conditions, to see and to approach dead prey directly from a distance of six feet or more under an illumination calculated to be as low as 0.000,000,73 foot-candle. When the illumination is reduced to 0.000,000,53 foot-candle, however, all these owls seem to have some difficulty in seeing prey that is more than a foot or so away. There is some evidence that sight may be of some value to the barred owl in finding dark-colored dead prey on nearly white soil at an illumination as dim as 0.000,000,15 foot-candle. The burrowing owl was unable to find dead prey regularly under illuminations dimmer than about 0.000,026 foot-candle. This species, therefore, has much less ability to see in very dim light than the other three species of owls studied. The burrowing owl, however, is more diurnal in habit than these other owls and also it lives in more open situations. From measurements of the incident light in nature it is calculated that in the natural habitats of all these owls the intensity of illumination must often fall below the minimum at which the birds can see their prey.