ArticlePDF Available

The dopamine receptor D4 7-repeat allele influences neurocognitive functioning, but this effect is moderated by age and ADHD status: An exploratory study

Authors:

Abstract and Figures

Evidence suggests the involvement of the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) in the pathogenesis of ADHD, but the exact mechanism is not well understood. Earlier reports on the effects of DRD4 polymorphisms on neurocognitive and neuroimaging measures are inconsistent. This study investigated the functional consequences of the 7-repeat allele of DRD4 on neurocognitive endophenotypes of ADHD in the Dutch subsample of the International Multicenter ADHD Genetics study. Participants were 350 children (5-11.5 years) and adolescents (11.6-19 years) with ADHD and their 195 non-affected siblings. An overall measure of neuropsychological functioning was derived by principal component analysis from five neurocognitive and five motor tasks. The effects of DRD4 and age were examined using Linear Mixed Model analyses. The analyses were stratified for affected and non-affected participants after finding a significant three-way interaction between ADHD status, age and the 7-repeat allele. Apart from a main effect of age, a significant interaction effect of age and DRD4 was found in non-affected but not in affected participants, with non-affected adolescent carriers of the 7-repeat allele showing worse neuropsychological performance. In addition, carrying the 7-repeat allele of DRD4 was related to a significantly worse performance on verbal working memory in non-affected siblings, independent of age. These results might indicate that the effect of the DRD4 7-repeat allele on neuropsychological functioning is dependent on age and ADHD status.
Content may be subject to copyright.
ORIGINAL INVESTIGATION
The dopamine receptor D4 7-repeat allele infl uences neurocognitive
functioning, but this effect is moderated by age and ADHD status:
An exploratory study
MARIEKE E. ALTINK
1,3 , NANDA N.J. ROMMELSE
4 , DORINE I.E. SLAATS-WILLEMSE
3 ,
ALEJANDRO ARIAS V Á SQUEZ
1,2 , BARBARA FRANKE
1,2 , CATHELIJNE J.M. BUSCHGENS
1 ,
ELLEN A. FLIERS
1 , STEPHEN V. FARAONE
5 , JOSEPH A. SERGEANT
4 ,
JAAP OOSTERLAAN
4 & JAN K. BUITELAAR
1
1 Department of Psychiatry, Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Center, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior,
Nijmegen, The Netherlands,
2 Department of Human Genetics, Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Centre, Nijmegen, The
Netherlands,
3 Karakter Child and Adolescent University Centre Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands,
4 Department of Clinical
Neuropsychology, VU University Amsterdam, Amsterdam, The Netherlands, and
5 Departments of Psychiatry and Neuroscience &
Physiology, SUNY Upstate Medical University, NY, USA
Abstract
Objectives. Evidence suggests the involvement of the dopamine D4 receptor gene ( DRD4 ) in the pathogenesis of ADHD,
but the exact mechanism is not well understood. Earlier reports on the effects of DRD4 polymorphisms on neurocognitive
and neuroimaging measures are inconsistent. This study investigated the functional consequences of the 7-repeat allele of
DRD4 on neurocognitive endophenotypes of ADHD in the Dutch subsample of the International Multicenter ADHD
Genetics study. Methods. Participants were 350 children (5 11.5 years) and adolescents (11.6 19 years) with ADHD and
their 195 non-affected siblings. An overall measure of neuropsychological functioning was derived by principal component
analysis from fi ve neurocognitive and fi ve motor tasks. The effects of DRD4 and age were examined using Linear Mixed
Model analyses. Results. The analyses were stratifi ed for affected and non-affected participants after fi nding a signifi cant three-
way interaction between ADHD status, age and the 7-repeat allele. Apart from a main effect of age, a signifi cant interaction
effect of age and DRD4 was found in non-affected but not in affected participants, with non-affected adolescent carriers of the
7-repeat allele showing worse neuropsychological performance. In addition, carrying the 7-repeat allele of DRD4 was related
to a signifi cantly worse performance on verbal working memory in non-affected siblings, independent of age. Conclusions. These
results might indicate that the effect of the DRD4 7-repeat allele on neuropsychological functioning is dependent on age
and ADHD status.
Key words: Dopamine receptor D4 gene , attention defi cit hyperactivity disorder , neurocognitive functioning , non-affected
siblings , development
Introduction
Family, twin, adoption and molecular genetic studies
have clearly pointed out the role of genetic factors in
the etiology of attention defi cit hyperactivity disorder
(ADHD) and led to an estimated heritability around
76% (Faraone et al. 2005). Pharmacological studies
(Staller and Faraone 2007) and biochemical imaging
studies (Forssberg et al. 2006) support the involvement
of the dopamine system in ADHD. An extensively
studied candidate gene for ADHD is the dopamine
D4 receptor gene ( DRD4 ), located on chromosome
11p15.5. A specifi c variant of the DRD4 gene associ-
ated with ADHD is the functional 48 base pair (bp)
variable number of tandem repeats (VNTR) poly-
morphism in exon 3 (Li et al. 2006). The 7-repeat
allele of this VNTR, identifi ed as the risk allele for
ADHD, produces fewer or less effi cient D4 receptors
resulting in less responsiveness to dopamine stimula-
tion (Asghari et al. 1995). Although association with
Correspondence: Jan K. Buitelaar, Department of Cognitive Neuroscience (204), Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior,
Radboud University Nijmegen Medical Center, PO Box 9101, 6500 HB, Nijmegen, The Netherlands. Tel: 31 24 3610750. Fax: 31
24 3610989. E-mail: j.buitelaar@psy.umcn.nl
(Rece ived 20 October 2010 ; ac cepted 19 May 2011 )
The World Journal of Biological Psychiatry, 2011; Early Online, 1–13
ISSN 1562-2975 print/ ISSN 1814-1412 online © 2011 Informa Healt hcare
DOI : 10.3109 /156229 75.2011.595822
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Yale University on 02/20/12
For personal use only.
2 M.E. Altink et al.
variation in design (e.g., case-control, case-only or
control-only designs), (b) the comparison of groups
with different lengths of repeats of the VNTR (7-
repeat carriers versus non-carriers, 4-repeat homozy-
gotes versus all others, or short (2 5 repeats) allele
carriers versus long (6 9 repeats) allele carriers), (c)
differences in method of sample ascertainment and
collection, and (d) small sample sizes.
An additional issue that has not been addressed is
the importance of a developmental perspective, when
studying the relationship between the DRD4 gene
and neurocognitive functioning. In both healthy and
ADHD children neurocognitive functioning improves
during development (Seidman 2006). Further, the
expression of many genes is differentially regulated
across different stages of development (Elia and
Devoto 2007), as may their infl uence on neurocogni-
tion. A well-known example is the increasing genetic
infl uence on general cognitive ability from infancy to
adolescence (Plomin et al. 1997). Similar effects
have been found for specifi c neurocognitive abilities
(Polderman et al. 2007). A recent twin study using
structural magnetic resonance imaging reported
genetic effects on cortical thickness to be most
prominent in early childhood for early developing
brain regions, such as the motor and sensory regions.
In contrast, genetic effects were most prominent
during adolescence for later developing regions, such
as the dorsolateral prefrontal and temporal areas
(Lenroot et al. 2009). This may be explained by the
involvement of new, additional genes during devel-
opment, which may infl uence ongoing brain matura-
tion. Previously we found nominally signifi cant
ndings of the dopamine transporter gene ( DAT1 )
on neuropsychological functioning predominantly in
adolescents in contrast to children (Rommelse et al.
2008c). With respect to the DRD4 7-repeat allele, a
longitudinal study in a community-based sample
suggested that the effects were more profound at age
11 than at age 4 8 (El-Faddagh et al. 2004). This is
in contrast to other longitudinal analyses that showed
that the association between the DRD4 7-repeat
allele and cortical thickness was most apparent early
in development and resolved around age 16 18
(Shaw et al. 2007).
The present study sought to extend previous work
by including the effect of age, which might explain
the inconsistent results of DRD4 on neurocognitive
functioning. Therefore, we examined the effect of
the DRD4 7-repeat allele on an overall measure of
neurocognitive functioning in a large sample includ-
ing both children and adolescents with combined
subtype ADHD and their non-affected siblings. The
overall-measure of neurocognitive functioning was
based on a broad range of cognitive and motor mea-
sures that met requirements for neuropsychological
ADHD is supported by a recent meta-analysis (Gizer
et al. 2009) data of individual studies on the asso-
ciation between the DRD4 7-repeat allele and ADHD
are not consistent. For example, the large family-
based association study of the International Multi-
center ADHD Genetics (IMAGE) project reported
only a trend towards association of the 7-repeat allele
( P 0.09) and a single nucleotide polymorphism
(SNP) rs9195457 with ADHD (Brookes et al. 2006).
Other fi ndings suggest DRD4 not to be associated with
the full ADHD phenotype, but only with inattentive
(Lasky-Su et al. 2008) or hyperactive-impulsive symp-
toms (Lasky-Su et al. 2007). In addition, among
children with ADHD the presence of the 7-repeat
allele was associated with less severe instead of
more ADHD symptoms (Tahir et al. 2000). A pos-
sible explanation for the heterogeneity of fi ndings
may be that published candidate gene studies have
employed relatively small samples, which makes
interpretation of fi ndings diffi cult. Another explana-
tion is the presence of unmeasured sources of het-
erogeneity that modify the association between the
candidate gene and the disorder , such as different
genetic backgrounds of populations sampled or the
use of different assessment and diagnostic measures
(Gizer et al. 2009).
Therefore, the exact mechanism underlying the
association of the DRD4 7-repeat allele with ADHD
is not well understood. Studies on the functional
consequences of the exon 3 VNTR in DRD4 on neu-
rocognitive, neuroimaging and neurophysiological
measures show inconsistent results (see Table I). The
presence of the DRD4 7-repeat allele was associated
with defi cits on a range of these measures in some
studies (Auerbach et al. 2001; Fossella et al. 2002;
Langley et al. 2004; Kieling et al. 2006; Mill et al.
2006; Shaw et al. 2007; Froehlich et al. 2007;
Herrmann et al. 2007; Boonstra et al. 2008; Loo et al.
2008) whereas others found no relationship (Langley
et al. 2004; Szobot et al. 2005; Barkley et al. 2006;
Genro et al. 2006; Demiralp et al. 2007; Herrmann
et al. 2007; Sonuga-Barke et al. 2008; Monuteaux
et al. 2008). In contrast to expectations, carriers of
at least one 7-repeat allele performed even better than
non-carriers on a verbal working memory task
(Boonstra et al. 2008), inhibitory control (Kramer et
al. 2009) and IQ test (Gornick et al. 2007). In a
case control design, ADHD 7-repeat non-carriers
rather than ADHD 7-repeat carriers performed
worse on attention and inhibition tests and measures
of speed and variability (Swanson et al. 2000). Fur-
ther, in a longitudinal study the DRD4
7-repeat allele
was associated with a normalization of the right pari-
etal cortex thickness, a higher IQ, and with a better
clinical outcome among ADHD patients (Shaw et al.
2007). These inconsistent results may be due to (a)
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Yale University on 02/20/12
For personal use only.
D R D 4 a n d n e u r o c o g n i t i v e f u n c t i o n i n g i n A D H D 3
Table I. Overview of studies reporting on the 48-bp VNTR of the DRD4 in exon 3 and functional (neurocognitive, neuroimaging and neurophysiological) measures.
Authors N Age (years) Functional Measure (s) Test Results
Neurocognitive measures
Swanson et al. 2000 96 ADHD
48 controls
M 11.7
M 11.2
EF, orienting, alerting,
Shifting, maintenance of
attention
7R vs. 7R 7R more cognitive problems (more slowly and
variable), 7R not different from controls.
Fossella et al. 2002 200 normals Adults Attention 4R vs. 4R 4R absent (2/2 and 7/7) have lower (effi cient) executive
attention scores.
Manor et al. 2002 180 ADHD M 10.68 Impulsivity, response
variability
Long (6-8R) vs.
short (2-5 R)
Short allele is related to poorer performance on
impulsivity and response variability. Dose-response
effect with number of repeats, signifi cant difference
2 vs. 7 repeat.
Langley et al. 2004 133 ADHD M 9.2 Sustained attention, impulse
control
7R vs. 7R – 7R greater impulsiveness, faster and less
Accurate control, inhibition
Bellgrove et al. 2005 54 ADHD
39 controls
M 12.3
M 11.3
Sustained attention, response
variability
7R vs. 7R 7R more errors and greater variability.
7R did not differ from controls.
Barkley et al. 2006 122 ADHD followed
longitudinally
67 controls followed
longitudinally
12-20 19-25
12-20 19-25
Attention, EF (vigilance,
problem solving,
impulsiveness)
7R vs. 7R No differences on any measures.
Froehlich et al. 2007 174 normals M 5.5 EF measures (spatial working
memory, rule learning and
reversal, spatial span and
planning)
7R vs. 7R – 7R impaired spatial working memory.
Boonstra et al. 2008 45 ADHD M 39.1 EF (fl uency, planning, working,
memory, set shifting,
inhibition) and non-EF
7R vs. 7R – 7R performed better on verbal short term memory.
7R better performance on visuo-constructive ability
and set shifting.
Loo et al. 2008 540 ADHD affected
sibling pairs
M 12.3,
M 9.7
EF measures (inhibition, working
memory, interference control,
set-shifting) & intelligence
7R vs. 7R-
240 bp allele vs.
120 bp allele
7R lower intelligence functioning, impaired speeded
naming, interference control and working memory.
Homozygous 240-bp allele: slower reaction time.
Kr ä mer et al. 2009 656 students (EEG
paradigm N 20)
M 21.7 Inhibitory control & EEG
paradigm
7R vs. 4R
(EEG paradigm
7/7R vs. 4/4R)
7R more accurate in the Go/Nogo-task than
4R (marginal signifi cance). No genetic effects on
ERPs.
Non-executive measure: IQ
Genro et al. 2006 242 normals
100 normals
220 normals
Children
Children
Adults
IQ 7R vs. 7R No differences in IQ.
Sonuga-Barke et al. 702 ADHD
694 Unaffected siblings
5-17 IQ 7R vs. 7R In both groups, carriers versus non-carriers did not
differ in IQ.
Mill et al. 2006 171 ADHD (sample 1)
1758 controls (sample 1)
49 ADHD (sample 2)
45 controls (sample 2)
5
5
7, 9, 11, 13
7, 9, 11, 13
IQ (combined score of 4
assessments in sample 2)
7R vs. 7R In both independent samples, 7R
lower IQ, but only in ADHD subjects and
not in controls.
(Continued)
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Yale University on 02/20/12
For personal use only.
4 M.E. Altink et al.
Table I. (Continued).
Authors N Age (years) Functional Measure(s) Test Results
Neuroimaging measures
Durston et al. 2005 26 ADHD
26 non-affected siblings
20 controls
Children
Children
Children
MRI volume analyses of
prefrontal gray matter and
caudate nucleus
4/4 versus
others
4/4 less volume for prefrontal gray matter volume, most
signifi cantly in the unaffected siblings.
Shaw et al. 2007 105 ADHD
67 ADHD at follow-up
103 controls
47 controls at follow-up
M 10.1
M 16
M 10.2
M 14.5
MRI, IQ 7R vs. 7R – 7R thinner right orbitofrontal and posterior parieto-
occipital cortex. Possession of 7-repeat affects cortical
thickness similarly in ADHD and controls. ADHD
7R had better clinical outcome and normalization
of right parietal cortex.
Gornick et al. 2007 166 ADHD
282 controls
M 9.0
M 15.9
IQ, inhibition, MRI scan 7R vs. 7R Cases with 7R higher scores on 3 WISC subscales.
Monuteaux et al. 2008 24 ADHD
19 ADHD & BPD
20 controls
M 37.8
M 33.6
M 36
Brain regions (superior frontal,
middle frontal, anterior
cingulate, and cerebellum
cortices) by MRI
7R vs. 7R Only within ADHD group: 7R reduction volume in
the superior frontal cortex and cerebellum cortex.
Neurophysiological measures
Szobot et al. 2005 34 ADHD M 11.6 Vigilance using SPECT 7R vs. 7R No differences in regional cerebral blood fl ow during
the task.
Muller et al. 2006 87 normals M 22.1 EBR refl ecting fl exibility 4/4 vs. 4/7 No main effect of 7R carriership.
Herrmann et al. 2007 40 normals M 22.6 Working memory, fNIRS (brain
activation)
7R vs. 7R No differences for working memory. 7R higher/
ineffective brain activity in dorsolateral PFC.
Demiralp et al. 2007 50 normals M 21.5 Auditory target detection 7R vs. 7R No differences on 2 of 2 variables. Main effect of
evoked and induced gamma response activity in
subjects with the 7-repeat.
ADHD, attention-defi cit/hyperactivity disorder; R , 7-repeat carrier; R , 7-repeat non-carrier; IQ, Intelligence Quotient; EF, Executive Functioning; EBR, Eye Blink Rate; (f)MRI, (functional)
magnetic resonance imaging; (f)NIRS, near-infrared spectroscopy; EEG, ElectroEncephaloGram; ERP, event related potential; SPECT, single photon emission computed tomography; PFC,
prefrontal cortex; BPD, bipolar disorder.
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Yale University on 02/20/12
For personal use only.
D R D 4 a n d n e u r o c o g n i t i v e f u n c t i o n i n g i n A D H D 5
all raw PACS data to yield diagnosis based on oper-
ational 18 DSM-IV criteria for ADHD (Brookes
et al. 2006). These were combined with items that
were scored 2 (pretty much true) or 3 (very much
true) in the teacher-rated Conners ADHD subscales
(L, M, and N) to generate the total number of hyper-
active-impulsive and inattentive symptoms of the
DSM-IV symptom list. Situational pervasiveness
was defi ned as at least one symptom occurring in
two or more different situations, as indicated by the
parents in the PACS interview, as well as the pres-
ence of at least one symptom scoring 2 or 3 from
the ADHD subscales (L, M, and N), as indicated by
the teachers on the Conners questionnaire. For pur-
poses of analysis here, siblings were regarded as non-
affected if they obtained scores in the nonclinical
range on both the parent and teacher questionnaires
(Conners N scale: T score 63, SDQ: 90th per-
centile) and had no formal or suspicion of ADHD
diagnosis. No PACS interview was administered
concerning non-affected siblings. The proband
and affected sibling both have a formal clinical
ADHD diagnosis, passed screening procedures and
fulfi lled the PACS-ADHD diagnosis (probands had
the combined type).
This study reports on a Dutch part of the larger
IMAGE sample of which neuropsychological data
also was obtained. A total of 238 families partici-
pated. This resulted in the participation of 238
ADHD affected probands and 112 affected siblings
(64 with combined subtype, 28 with inattentive sub-
type and 20 with hyperactive-impulsive subtype) and
195 non-affected siblings. Two groups were formed:
one group of affected participants ( N 350, mean
age 12.0, %boys 75.7) and one group of non-
affected participants ( N 195, mean age 11.5,
%boys 45.6).
Neuropsychological measures
To be selected as a cognitive endophenotype, mea-
sures should have satisfi ed several criteria outlined
in the literature, such as being associated with
ADHD, showing familiality, and being observable in
non-affected fi rst-degree relatives of an affected
individual (Gottesman and Gould 2003). Variables
of 10 neuropsychological tasks (fi ve cognitive and
ve motor tasks) satisfi ed these criteria and were
selected (Rommelse et al. 2008a). For a description
of these tasks, see Table II. The cognitive tests were:
Time Test (time reproduction; Barkley 1998), Visuo-
Spatial Sequencing (visuo-spatial working memory),
Digit Span (verbal working memory; Wechsler 2002),
Stop Task (motor inhibition of an ongoing response;
Logan 1994) and Shifting Attentional Set (motor
inhibition and cognitive fl exibility). The fi ve motor
endophenotypes of ADHD in earlier reports
(Rommelse et al. 2008a). We tested the hypotheses
that the effect of the DRD4 7-repeat allele on
neurocognitive functioning would differ between
children and adolescents, and between ADHD
affected and non-affected participants, since -accord-
ing to earlier studies by our group (Altink et al. 2008;
Boonstra et al. 2008) the impact of the DRD4
7-repeat allele might differ depending on affected
status.
Materials and methods
Participants
Families with at least one child with the combined
subtype of ADHD (proband) and at least one addi-
tional sibling (regardless of possible ADHD status)
were recruited from clinics in order to participate in
the International Multi-Centre ADHD Genetics
(IMAGE) project. This is an international collabora-
tive study in eight European countries (Belgium,
Germany, Ireland, Israel, Spain, Switzerland, The
Netherlands and the United Kingdom) that aims to
identify genes that increase the risk for ADHD using
linkage and association strategies (Brookes et al.
2006). Ethical approval was obtained from National
Institutes of Health recognized local ethical review
boards, and all families gave written informed con-
sent prior to participation. All participants were aged
5 19 and of European Caucasian descent. Exclusion
criteria included IQ 70, presence of autism, epi-
lepsy, brain disorders and any genetic or medical
disorder associated with externalizing behaviours
that might mimic ADHD.
Details of the screening procedures and measures
for phenotyping are described elsewhere (Brookes
et al. 2006). In short, both the children already clin-
ically diagnosed with ADHD and their siblings were
similarly screened with the following rating scales:
parent and teacher Conners long version rating
scales (Conners 2003) and the Strength and Diffi -
culties Questionnaire (SDQ; Goodman 2001). Only
clinical cases with an average T score of the DSM-IV
total symptom score (N-scale) 63 on the Conners
scales and scores 90th percentile on the SDQ-hy-
peractivity scale were recruited into IMAGE. Subse-
quently, all children in the family with clinical scores
on any of these questionnaires, the clinical diagnosis
(of the probands and affected siblings) was verifi ed
with the Parental Account of Childhood Symptoms
(PACS; Taylor et al. 1986). PACS is a semi-struc-
tured, standardized, investigator-based, parent-in-
formed interview developed as an instrument to
provide an objective measure of children’s behaviour.
A standardized algorithm for PACS was applied to
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Yale University on 02/20/12
For personal use only.
6 M.E. Altink et al.
Table II. Description of the neuropsychological tasks.
Task Aim of measurement Performance measures
Cognitive tasks
Stop task
a Motor inhibition of an ongoing response Stop signal reaction time (SSRT)
Shifting attentional set
b Motor inhibition and cognitive fl exibility Percentage of errors across blocks
Time test
c Time reproduction Accuracy in seconds (absolute discrepancy)
Visuo-spatial Sequencing
b Visuo-spatial working memory Total number of correct targets in the
correct order
Digit span
d Verbal working memory Maximum span backwards
Motor tasks
Pursuit
b Motor control under continuous adaptation Precision (mean distance cursor and target
in mm)
Tracking
b Motor control without continuous
adaptation required
Precision (mean distance to midline in mm)
Tapping
b Self-generated motor output Variability (SD in ms) in tapping rate
Baseline speed
b Motor output as response to an external
cue
Variability (SD in ms) in reaction times
Motor timing
e Timing of motor output Variability (SD in ms) in reaction times
SD, standard deviation; mm, millimeter; ms, milliseconds.
a Logan 1994.
b Subtests of the Amsterdam Neuropsychological Tasks (ANT; De Sonneville 1999).
c Barkley 1998.
d Subtest WISC-III (Wechsler 2002).
e Van Meel et al. 2005.
tasks were: Tapping (self-generated motor output),
Pursuit (motor control under continuous adapta-
tion), Tracking (motor control without continuous
adaptation required), Baseline Speed (motor output
as response to an external cue; De Sonneville 1999)
and Motor Timing (timing of motor output; van
Meel et al. 2005). To normalize the dependent vari-
ables, a Van der Waerden transformation was applied
(Statistical Package for the Social Sciences [SPSS]
version 16). A principal component analysis was per-
formed on the 10 task measures, to reduce error
variance and to create a stronger familial neuropsy-
chological composite of the individual task measures.
Although fi ve of the tasks were theoretically judged
as belonging to the cognitive domain and the other
ve as belonging to the motor domain, all 10 task
measures were found to load on one major compo-
nent, explaining 46.5% of the variance in the task
measures (see Table III), which is in concordance
with an earlier report (Rommelse et al. 2008a).
DNA collection
Directly after collection, blood samples were sent to
Rutgers University Cell and DNA Repository, NJ,
USA, where DNA was extracted from part of the
blood or from immortalized cell lines. DNA stocks
were collated at the Social, Genetic and Develop-
mental Psychiatry Center (SGDP) laboratories in
London where they were stored, organized and
plated out for further analysis. Geneservice Ltd.,
Table III. Component matrix with factor loadings (principal
component analysis).
Neurocognitive variables Factor 1
Time test 0.734
Visuo-spatial sequencing 0.789
Digit span 0.651
Stop task 0.652
Shifting attentional set 0.650
Tapping 0.543
Pursuit 0.792
Tracking 0.591
Motor timing 0.742
Baseline speed 0.634
Cambridge (UK) performed whole genome amplifi -
cation on all samples with 100 μ g stock DNA, using
the REPLI-g kit (Quiagen Ltd, Crawley, UK).
Statistical analyses
Hardy Weinberg equilibrium (HWE) proportions
were estimated from parental DRD4 genotype infor-
mation using the Markov Chain Monte-Carlo
approximation of the exact test implemented in the
GENEPOP package V 3.3. (Raymond and Rousset
1995). Genotyping errors leading to Mendelian incon-
sistencies were detected using PedCheck (O’Connell
and Weeks 1998) and inconsistencies that could not
be resolved were excluded (nine participants). The
DRD4 48 bp VNTR is a multiallelic polymorphism,
for which we identifi ed seven different alleles in our
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Yale University on 02/20/12
For personal use only.
D R D 4 a n d n e u r o c o g n i t i v e f u n c t i o n i n g i n A D H D 7
sample. The most common genotype was the one
with two copies of the 4-repeat (39.7%), followed by
the 4/7 genotype (23.7%), 2/4 genotype (12.5%), 3/4
genotype (7%), 7/7 genotype (3.1%) and 2/2 geno-
type (2.2%). Other genotypes were even less fre-
quent. In order to maximize the power of our
analysis, we recoded the genotypes according to
absence or presence of the 7-repeat allele into carri-
ers (one or two 7-repeat alleles) and non-carriers
(zero 7-repeat alleles) of the allele. No genotypic
information was available for 47 participants, due to
refusal of blood extraction.
The effects of DRD4 and age on neuropsycho-
logical functioning were examined using linear mixed
model (LMM) analyses, which accounted for within
family correlation. Age was recoded into two cate-
gories using median age as a threshold, children
(age 11.5 years) and adolescents (age 11.5
years). The reason for the median as a cut-off score
is to ensure that we the age groups are similar in size.
Another reason is that at age 11.5 years children go
to high school. In high school more independence of
the child is asked for (such as homework, planning)
which may refl ect a different developmental phase.
The LMM included the fi xed factors 7-repeat allele
(present versus absent), and age (children versus ado-
lescents) and the interaction between both factors.
The overall neurocognitive composite score was used
as dependent measure. The model was adjusted for
IQ and gender, given the difference in gender ratio in
the affected and non-affected children.
After an omnibus test, analyses were separately
conducted for affected and non-affected participants,
given our hypotheses, and because previous studies
suggest that genetic effects may be different in
these groups (Altink et al. 2008). Correction for
multiple comparisons according to the false discov-
ery rate (FDR) controlling procedure was applied to
the analyses with a P value setting of 0.05.
Results
No deviations from HWE were detected for the DRD4
7-repeat allele. More than one-third of the total sam-
ple ( N 168; 34.4%) was carrier of the 7-repeat
allele (ADHD children: 31.4%; non-affected: 39.7%).
The 7-repeat carriers and non-carriers did not sig-
nifi cantly differ in age, gender or IQ ( F (1,498) 2.16,
χ ² (1,489 0.01, F (1,498) 1.02, all P 0.05). The
association of ADHD diagnosis with the 7-repeat allele
was not signifi cant ( χ ² (1,489) 3.36, P 0.067), as
previously also found in the IMAGE sample (Brookes
et al. 2006). Inspired by fi ndings in a larger IMAGE
sample (Lasky-Su et al. 2007; Altink et al. 2008) we
also examined the association between the 7-repeat
allele and ADHD symptoms , measured as the averaged
teacher and parent rating on the Conners scales.
ADHD affected participants carrying the 7-repeat
allele showed signifi cantly lower scores on the hyper-
activity scale compared to ADHD participants with-
out the 7-repeat allele (60.8 vs. 63.9; F (1,487) 7.54,
P 0.05). No signifi cant differences were detected
for inattention and the total ADHD scale
( F (1,487) 3.32, F (1,488) 4.93; P 0.05).
Effects of DRD4 7-repeat allele and age on
neuropsychological functioning
The omnibus analysis of the overall neuropsycho-
logical component in the whole sample showed
main effects of age ( F (1,402.61) 294.22, P 0.001)
and ADHD status ( F (1,365.21) 7.62, P 0.006).
Older children performed better than younger chil-
dren, and children with ADHD performed worse than
their non-affected siblings. These variables also sig-
nifi cantly interacted ( F (1,396.50) 8.31, P 0.004).
The 7-repeat allele had no signifi cant main effect
( F (1,327.98) 0.286, P 0.593). The allele did sig-
nifi cantly interact with age ( F (1,402.98) 4.78,
P 0.029), but not with ADHD ( F (1,388.22) 0.20,
P 0.655). Importantly, we found a signifi cant
three-way interaction between age, ADHD and the
7-repeat allele ( F (2,401.35) 4.62, P 0.032).
These results showed that the effects of the 7-repeat
allele and age were different in ADHD affected chil-
dren and their non-affected siblings. This gave us
enough evidence to further perform stratifi ed analy-
ses in affected and non-affected participants.
As expected, a main effect of age was found in the
separate analysis for both ADHD affected and non-
affected participants, with adolescents performing
better than children on the overall neuropsycholo-
gical component. There was no main effect of the
DRD4 7-repeat allele on the overall neuropsycho-
logical component in the separate analysis for either
affected or non-affected participants. Further, the
7-repeat allele was found to interact signifi cantly
with age in the non-affected (
F (1,140.00) 5.95,
P 0.016) but not in the affected participants. To
further localize this large modifying effect of age, we
stratifi ed the sample in four groups: affected and
non-affected children (age 11.5 years; N 125,
N 77 respectively), and affected and non-affected
adolescents (age 11.5 years; N 137, N 63 respec-
tively). Post-hoc analyses showed that non-affected
adolescents carrying the 7-repeat allele performed
signifi cantly worse on overall neuropsychological
functioning than non-affected adolescents without
the risk allele ( P 0.008; Figure 1), whereas this
effect was not apparent in affected adolescents or
children (affected or non-affected).
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Yale University on 02/20/12
For personal use only.
8 M.E. Altink et al.
Figure 1. Z scores and standard error of the mean of the effect of
the DRD4 7-repeat allele on the overall neurocognitive component
stratifi ed according to ADHD status and age. A higher score
refl ects a worse performance. Non-affected adolescents carrying
the 7-repeat allele performed signifi cantly worse on the overall
neurocognitive component compared to non-affected adolescent
non-carriers ( P 0.008).
To further identify the single neurocognitive mea-
sures contributing to the interaction effect of DRD4
with age, the LMM analyses were repeated for all
10 neurocognitive measures separately. Again, age
showed signifi cant effects on all neurocognitive mea-
sures in affected and non-affected participants. In
addition, the 7-repeat allele was associated with a
signifi cantly worse performance on Digit Span in the
non-affected siblings ( F (1,157.16) 7.24, P 0.008,
N 172), independent of age. The signifi cant inter-
action between 7-repeat allele carriership and age in
the non-affected group was found on four tasks,
namely Visuo-Spatial sequencing ( F (1,172.00) 6.19,
Figure 2. For explanation, see Figure 1. Non-affected adolescents
carrying the 7-repeat allele performed signifi cantly worse on
Visuo-Spatial Sequencing compared to non-affected adolescent
noncarriers ( P 0.019). An opposite effect was found in
ADHD affected children, with a signifi cantly better performance
for 7-repeat allele carriers compared to the non-carriers
( P 0.041).
Figure 3. For explanation, see Figure 1. Non-affected adolescents
carrying the 7-repeat allele performed signifi cantly worse on
Digit Span compared to non-affected adolescent non-carriers
( P 0.001).
Figure 4. For explanation, see Figure 1. Non-affected adolescents
carrying the 7-repeat allele performed signifi cantly worse on
Shifting Attentional Set compared to non-affected adolescent
non-carriers ( P 0.023). An opposite effect was found in
ADHD affected children, with a signifi cantly better performance
for 7-repeat allele carriers compared to the non-carriers
( P 0.034).
P 0.014), Digit Span ( F (1,169.89) 6.04, P
0.015), Stop Task ( F (1,172.00) 6.38, P 0.012)
and Shifting Attentional Set ( F (1,146.47) 4.30,
P 0.040; nominally signifi cant). The latter result
did not survive multiple testing. Within the ADHD
affected group no signifi cant main or interaction
effects of 7-repeat allele carriership were found.
Post-hoc analyses showed that non-affected
adolescents carrying the 7-repeat allele performed
signifi cantly worse than those without the risk allele
on Visuo-Spatial Sequencing ( P 0.019, N 78;
Figure 2), Digit Span ( P 0.001, N 78; Figure
3) and Shifting Attentional Set ( P 0.023, N 78;
Figure 4). In contrast, affected participants carrying
the 7-repeat allele appeared to perform better than
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Yale University on 02/20/12
For personal use only.
D R D 4 a n d n e u r o c o g n i t i v e f u n c t i o n i n g i n A D H D 9
of the brain during development (Polderman et al.
2007). Genetic effects on later developing regions,
such as the dorsolateral prefrontal and temporal
areas, are most prominent during adolescence. These
are the regions that express the highest levels of
DRD4 (Meador-Woodruff et al. 1996). Differences
in the weight of genetic infl uences over age on neuro-
cognitive performance might therefore explain why
DRD4 effects are restricted to adolescents in our
study. Another related and partly overlapping pos-
sibility are age-related changes of the dopamine sys-
tem (Spear 2000). Whereas the density of dopamine
receptors appears to decline over late childhood and
adolescence, at the same time the dopamine activity
in the prefrontal cortex tends to increase while
improving neurocognitive performance, such as
working memory (Thompson et al. 2004). Carrying
the DRD4 7 repeat allele may interfere with a sub-
optimal functioning of the dopamine system in the
prefrontal cortex. Thus normally developing adoles-
cents may be much more sensitive than children
to this effect of carrying the DRD4 7-repeat allele.
The differential effects of the gene in the adolescents
and children could explain why earlier studies
have reported inconsistent results with regard to
the association between DRD4 and neurocognitive
functioning.
The effect of DRD4 was not comparable in affected
participants and non-affected siblings, with signifi -
cant effects only found in the non-affected siblings.
If anything, the direction of effects was reversed in
the affected individuals, i.e. those with the 7-repeat
allele had less hyperactive-impulsive symptoms and
better neurocognitive functioning. This remains a
puzzle in need of solving. A possible explanation why
DRD4 effects were seen in the non-affected siblings
only may be given by the fact that ADHD is a
multifactorial disorder. Multiple genes and environ-
mental risk factors, each of small effect, are thought
to underlie these complex disorders (Crosbie et al.
2008). When these multiple factors together reach a
certain threshold, pathology occurs, in our case
ADHD. Therefore, in the affected individuals, most
of whom were affected with the most severe, com-
bined subtype of ADHD, the dopamine neurotrans-
mission might be rather impaired. Hence, the effects
of a single gene on neurocognitive functioning
can be distinguished less easily. In the non-affected
siblings, less genetic load and/or gene gene and
gene environmental interactions might be present,
and single gene effects might therefore be easier to
observe. Anyhow, the developmentally expected
greater sensitivity to suboptimal functioning of
the dopaminergic system as discussed above is obvi-
ously absent in adolescents with ADHD. This would
lead to the conclusion that DRD4 may serve as a
those without the 7-repeat allele on Shifting Atten-
tional Set ( P 0.034, N 166; Figure 4) and
Visuo-Spatial Sequencing ( P 0.041, N 166;
Figure 2), but these effects were not signifi cant upon
correction for multiple testing. For raw means, stan-
dard deviations of each task see Table IV.
To summarize, the results show a clear effect of
age and ADHD: older children and children without
an ADHD diagnosis perform better on the overall
neurocognitive construct. The effect of DRD4 is not
straightforward and appears to be dependent on age
and ADHD diagnosis. In other words, the effect of
DRD4 differs for younger children compared to
older children, and children with the disorder versus
without. The signifi cant effects are most pronounced
in the non-affected adolescents; carrying the 7-repeat
allele results in a worse performance on the overall
neurocognitive construct, Visuo-Spatial Sequencing
task, Digit Span and Shifting attentional Set. Indepen-
dent of age the results show an effect of DRD4 on Digit
Span, but only in the non-affected participants.
Discussion
Studies on the functional consequences of DRD4 on
neurocognitive, neuroimaging and neurophysiologi-
cal measures show inconsistent results. Apart from
variation in sample size, ascertainment and study
design, the heterogeneity of fi ndings might be
explained by ignoring a development perspective.
Therefore this study tested the hypothesis that the
effect of the DRD4 7-repeat allele on neurocognitive
functioning would differ between children and ado-
lescents, and also between ADHD affected and non-
affected participants, since the impact of the DRD4
7-repeat allele might differ depending on affected
status within families. Apart from a main effect of
age on neurocognitive functioning, the effect of the
DRD4 on neurocognitive functioning indeed dif-
fered between children and adolescents. Among ado-
lescents, carriers of the 7-repeat allele performed
worse than non-carriers, but this was not observed
in children. This effect was not due to a greater vari-
ability of neurocognitive performance in children.
Possible explanations for the main effect of age
remain speculative but should be sought in brain
maturation processes taking place during adolescence.
Decrease of grey matter density and increase of white
matter density occurs globally in the brain during
adolescence, with specifi c increase in the frontal, pari-
etal and occipital lobes (Krain and Castellanos 2006).
This is refl ected by improvements in neurocognitive
functioning, as also observed in our study.
As mentioned in the introduction, the heritability
of cortical thickness changes across different regions
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Yale University on 02/20/12
For personal use only.
10 M.E. Altink et al.
Table IV. Non-transformed multiple testing means, standard deviations and number of each task and the overall neuropsychological
component for the affected, non-affected children stratifi ed according to age and 7-repeat carriership.
7-repeat non carrier 7-repeat carrier
11.5 11.5 11.5 11.5
MSD N MSD N MSD N MSD N
ADHD participants
Main component ( Z score) 0.67 0.74 82 0.48 0.74 99 0.66 0.81 43 0.45 0.72 38
Time reproduction (s) 3.56 1.96 92 2.32 2.16 120 3.42 1.82 51 2.28 1.73 46
Visual spatial sequencing ( N ) 80.99 13.05 92 94.20 8.26 120 82.80 12.80 51 92.07 9.84 46
Digit span backwards ( N ) 3.41 0.87 92 4.33 1.21 120 3.41 0.92 51 4.28 1.15 46
Stop task (SSRT) 328.56 77.80 82 255.42 62.27 105 351.73 105.65 43 259.69 54.53 40
Shifting attentional set (%) 16.78 9.99 92 10.26 8.26 120 13.86 8.80 51 9.71 8.03 46
Tapping (ms) 78.18 34.39 92 56.01 19.73 120 74.11 26.42 51 58.11 27.79 46
Pursuit (mm) 7.84 5.34 92 4.24 1.36 120 8.16 5.41 51 4.25 1.61 46
Tracking (mm) 3.58 2.40 92 2.39 1.73 120 3.45 3.13 51 2.14 0.94 46
Time estimation (ms) 541.78 428.00 92 304.19 216.86 120 612.67 518.18 51 329.46 193.22 46
Baseline speed (ms) 177.57 107.35 92 123.81 100.81 120 188.25 119.08 51 101.56 79.02 46
Unaffected siblings
Main component ( Z score) 0.74 0.93 43 1.16 0.58 40 0.49 0.77 34 0.80 0.68 23
Time reproduction (s) 3.33 2.13 54 1.52 0.62 50 2.86 1.33 40 1.78 1.33 28
Visual spatial sequencing ( N ) 80.19 17.02 54 98.74 5.28 50 86.55 11.04 40 96.12 5.69 28
Digit span backwards ( N ) 3.28 1.04 54 4.90 0.93 50 3.23 0.77 40 4.11 0.88 28
Stop task (SSRT) 348.14 116.50 43 223.31 35.52 45 313.73 102.90 34 241.53 41.41 27
Shifting attentional set (%) 16.99 10.03 54 6.62 5.08 50 14.82 10.43 40 8.78 7.06 28
Tapping (ms) 90.51 63.36 54 48.93 16.91 50 86.36 88.12 40 49.62 22.24 28
Pursuit (mm) 8.26 4.13 54 3.69 1.10 50 7.18 4.38 40 3.83 1.19 28
Tracking (mm) 618.14 563.12 54 1.64 1.53 50 2.99 1.58 40 1.85 1.11 28
Time estimation (ms) 3.27 2.26 54 258.10 304.28 50 593.24 522.51 40 231.65 73.76 28
Baseline speed (ms) 183.50 114.45 54 99.09 81.83 50 184.50 130.60 40 124.30 103.24 28
M, mean; SD, standard deviation; s, second; N , number; SSRT, stop signal reaction time; ms, millisecond; mm, millimeter.
modifying gene, acting against a background of
other genetic and environmental etiological factors,
rather than as a gene merely increasing the risk for
ADHD.
Post-hoc analyses suggested that the effect of
DRD4 on overall neurocognitive functioning in
non-affected adolescents was related to effects on
neurocognitive rather than to motor functioning
tests. For four of fi ve neurocognitive tasks the same
effect of DRD4 was observed that non-affected ado-
lescents carrying the 7-repeat allele performed worse
than non-affected adolescents not carrying the allele.
This interpretation would be in keeping with evi-
dence showing that DRD4 is mainly expressed in
the frontal lobe which subserves neurocognitive
functions, and not in subcortical structures like the
striatum which are involved in motor functions
(Durston et al. 2005). However, other explanations
for this fi nding cannot be ruled out, such as measure-
ment error or neurocognitive load differing between
the single tasks.
This is the fi rst study to report a main effect of the
DRD4 7-repeat allele on verbal working memory in
children and adolescents. Carrying the 7-repeat was
associated with a worse performance of verbal work-
ing memory in non-affected siblings. Working memory
is one of the core defi cits of ADHD (Klingberg et al.
2005), and genetic factors were found to explain
36% of the variance of working memory perfor-
mance according to a recent twin study (Kuntsi
et al. 2006). Importantly, in one of our previous
studies non-affected siblings displayed verbal work-
ing memory defi cits, whereas they did not manifest
clinical ADHD symptoms (Rommelse et al. 2008b).
Other evidence also suggests that DRD4 plays a role
in working memory in healthy ADHD non-affected
individuals. Working memory is regulated by the pre-
frontal cortex and DRD4 is highly expressed in this
brain region (Ariano et al. 1997). Indeed, the exon
3 VNTR polymorphism of DRD4 was associated
with the volume of the prefrontal cortex (Durston
et al. 2005). Dopamine plays a crucial role in work-
ing memory performance (Diamond 2007). Several
animal studies have also indicated the importance of
the D4 receptor in working memory using specifi c
agonists and antagonists for the receptor (Arnsten
et al. 2000; Zhang et al. 2004; Browman et al. 2005).
Working memory is often regarded as a foundation
for other executive functions (Klingberg et al. 2005).
That we did not observe main effects of the 7-repeat
allele for the other cognitive tasks associated with the
prefrontal cortex (e.g., inhibition, cognitive fl exibil-
ity, visuo-spatial working memory) may most likely
be due to these other cognitive processes being less
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Yale University on 02/20/12
For personal use only.
D R D 4 a n d n e u r o c o g n i t i v e f u n c t i o n i n g i n A D H D 11
using LMM analyses. Another limitation is the
cross-sectional design of our study and the lack of
longitudinal measures of neurocognitive function-
ing. Other potential limitations are the assessment
of only one polymorphism at the DRD4 gene and
the use of a statistically derived global measure of
neurocognitive functioning.
In conclusion, our results indicate that the effect
of the DRD4 7-repeat allele on neurocognitive func-
tioning may be dependent on age and ADHD status
and may relate to neurocognitive rather than to
motor functioning. Increased activity of the dop-
amine system in adolescence may make adolescents
more vulnerable to carrying the DRD4 7-repeat
allele which interferes with optimal functioning of
dopamine transmission in the prefrontal cortex. This
study thus may give new insights into how a devel-
opmental perspective is inextricably bound up with
genetics. As such, our study may be viewed as explor-
atory, with the results presented to be considered a
valuable dataset for replication.
Acknowledgements
The authors thank all of the parents, teachers, and
children who participated. We thank Keeley Brookes
and Xiaohui Xu for genotyping. This study was
partly funded by a grant assigned to Stephen Fara-
one by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIH
grant # R01 MH62873-01A1).
Statement of interest
None to declare.
References
Altink ME, Arias-Vasquez A, Franke B, Slaats-Willemse DI,
Buschgens CJ, Rommelse NN, et al. 2008. The dopamine
receptor D4 7-repeat allele and prenatal smoking in ADHD-
affected children and their unaffected siblings: no gene-environ-
ment interaction. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Disciplines
49:1053 1060.
Ariano MA, Wang J, Noblett KL, Larson ER, Sibley DR. 1997.
Cellular distribution of the rat D4 dopamine receptor protein in
the CNS using anti-receptor antisera. Brain Res 752:26 34.
Arnsten AF, Murphy B, Merchant K. 2000. The selective
dopamine D4 receptor antagonist, PNU-101387G, prevents
stress-induced cognitive defi cits in monkeys. Neuropsycho-
pharmacology 23:405 410.
Asghari V, Sanyal S, Buchwaldt S, Paterson A, Jovanovic V, Van
Tol HH. 1995. Modulation of intracellular cyclic AMP levels
by different human dopamine D4 receptor variants. J Neuro-
chem 65:1157 1165.
Auerbach JG, Benjamin J, Faroy M, Geller V, Ebstein R. 2001.
DRD4 related to infant attention and information processing:
a developmental link to ADHD? Psychiatr Genet 11:31 35.
Time Perception Application 1998. Version 1.0. University of
Massachussetts Medical Center: Chesapeake Technology.
dependent on dopamine and strongly infl uenced
by additional neurotransmitters systems (Diamond
2007).
It is challenging to relate our fi ndings to previous
studies on neural effects of DRD4 in ADHD. A
recent report on the effect of the DRD4 7-repeat
allele on cortical thickness in participants with
ADHD and healthy controls found overlapping main
effects of affected status and DRD4 genotype, and
similar effects of carrying the 7-repeat allele in ADHD
participants and healthy controls (Shaw et al. 2007).
Further, all these effects were more prominent early
in development, below age 12, than thereafter. These
discrepant fi ndings should be considered in the per-
spective of key differences in design between the two
studies, such as a focus on neuroanatomical struc-
ture versus neurocognitive function, and the inclu-
sion of healthy controls versus non-affected but
genetically related siblings. Cross-study variation in
sample composition may further be relevant, since
our sample of ADHD participants had a higher pro-
portion of males, a higher rate of comorbid conduct
disorder, and a lower average IQ. Further, in our
sample, 7-repeat carriers had a similar IQ as non-
carriers. Nonetheless, our post-hoc fi ndings suggest
a protective effect of carrying the 7-repeat allele
among ADHD affected participants in performing
better than non-carriers on Shifting Attentional Set
and Visuo-Spatial Sequencing, and having lower
hyperactivity ratings. Future work should try to resolve
discrepancies by integrating measures of cortical thick-
ness and neurocognitive functioning in the same sam-
ple and aggregating data in multisite collaborations.
Our study should be viewed in the context of some
strengths and limitations. Strengths of this study
include the large sample size that increased power to
detect an effect of the DRD4 7-repeat allele on neu-
rocognitive functioning. Second, the data-collection
of the ADHD families and the diagnostic procedure
of the probands in the IMAGE study were done in
a very structured and thorough manner. Third, a
broad range of neurocognitive tasks was used, tap-
ping different neurocognitive functions known to be
defi cient in ADHD. Our non-affected participants
were siblings of ADHD probands. This had the
advantage to control for various shared environmen-
tal and family factors, such as ADHD status of the
parents, parenting issues and family climate (Bieder-
man et al. 1995). Non-affected siblings however can-
not be equated to healthy controls because their
higher genetic risk for ADHD and higher rate of neu-
rocognitive performance defi cits (Rommelse et al.
2008a). Please note that we included unrelated as
well as related individuals in the same analyses, which
may have reduced the variance of our cognitive tests.
However, we corrected for relatedness of siblings by
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Yale University on 02/20/12
For personal use only.
12 M.E. Altink et al.
Barkley RA, Smith KM, Fischer M, Navia B. 2006. An examina-
tion of the behavioral and neuropsychological correlates of
three ADHD candidate gene polymorphisms (DRD4 7 ,
DBH TaqI A2, and DAT1 40 bp VNTR) in hyperactive and
normal children followed to adulthood. Am J Med Genet Part
B: Neuropsychiatr Genet 141:487 498.
Biederman J, Milberger S, Faraone SV, Kiely K, Guite J, Mick E,
et al. 1995. Impact of adversity on functioning and comorbid-
ity in children with attention-defi cit hyperactivity disorder.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 34:1495 1503.
Boonstra AM, Kooij JJ, Buitelaar JK, Oosterlaan J, Sergeant JA,
Heister JG, Franke B. 2008. An exploratory study of the rela-
tionship between four candidate genes and neurocognitive per-
formance in adult ADHD. Am J Med Genet Part B:
Neuropsychiatr Genet 147:397 402.
Brookes K, Xu X, Chen W, Zhou K, Neale B, Lowe N, et al. 2006.
The analysis of 51 genes in DSM-IV combined type attention
defi cit hyperactivity disorder: association signals in DRD4,
DAT1 and 16 other genes. Mol Psychiatry 11:934 953.
Browman KE, Curzon P, Pan JB, Molesky AL, Komater VA,
Decker MW, et al. 2005. A-412997, a selective dopamine D4
agonist, improves cognitive performance in rats. Pharmacol
Biochem Behav 82:148 155.
Conners CK. 2003. Conner’s Rating Scales, Revised. 6th ed.
Toronto: MHS.
Crosbie J, Perusse D, Barr CL, Schachar RJ. 2008. Validating psy-
chiatric endophenotypes: Inhibitory control and attention defi -
cit hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 32:40 55.
De Sonneville LMJ. 1999. Amsterdam Neuropsychological Task:
A computer-aided assessment program. In: Den Brinker
BPLM, Beek PJ, Brand N, Maarse SJ, Mulder LJM, editors.
Cognitive ergonomics, clinical assessment and computer-
assisted learning: Computers in psychology. Lisse, The Neth-
erlands: Swets & Zeitlinger. p. 204 217.
Demiralp T, Herrmann CS, Erdal ME, Ergenoglu T, Keskin YH,
Ergen M, Beydagi H. 2007. DRD4 and DAT1 polymorphisms
modulate human gamma band responses. Cerebral Cortex
17:1007 1019.
Diamond A. 2007. Consequences of variations in genes that affect
dopamine in prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex 17(Suppl
1):i161 170.
Durston S, Fossella JA, Casey BJ, Hulshoff Pol HE, Galvan A,
Schnack HG, et al. 2005. Differential effects of DRD4 and
DAT1 genotype on fronto-striatal gray matter volumes in a sam-
ple of subjects with attention defi cit hyperactivity disorder, their
unaffected siblings, and controls. Mol Psychiatry 10:678 685.
El-Faddagh M, Laucht M, Maras A, Vohringer L, Schmidt MH.
2004. Association of dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) gene with
attention-defi cit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in a high-risk
community sample: a longitudinal study from birth to 11 years
of age. J Neural Trans 111:883 889.
Elia J, Devoto M. 2007. ADHD genetics: 2007 update. Curr Psy-
chiatry Rep 9:434 439.
Faraone SV, Perlis RH, Doyle AE, Smoller JW, Goralnick JJ,
Holmgren MA, Sklar P. 2005. Molecular genetics of attention-
defi cit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry 57:1313 1323.
Forssberg H, Fernell E, Waters S, Waters N, Tedroff J. 2006.
Altered pattern of brain dopamine synthesis in male adoles-
cents with attention defi cit hyperactivity disorder. Behav Brain
Funct 2:40.
Fossella J, Sommer T, Fan J, Wu Y, Swanson JM, Pfaff DW, Posner
MI. 2002. Assessing the molecular genetics of attention net-
works. BMC Neurosci 3:14.
Froehlich TE, Lanphear BP, Dietrich KN, Cory-Slechta DA,
Wang N, Kahn RS. 2007. Interactive effects of a DRD4 poly-
morphism, lead, and sex on executive functions in children.
Biol Psychiatry 62:243 249.
Genro JP, Roman T, Zeni CP, Grevet EH, Schmitz M, de Abreu
PB, et al. 2006. No association between dopaminergic poly-
morphisms and intelligence variability in attention-defi cit/
hyperactivity disorder. Mol Psychiatry 11:1066 1067.
Gizer IR, Ficks C, Waldman ID. 2009. Candidate gene studies of
ADHD: a meta-analytic review. Hum Genet 126:51 90.
Goodman R. 2001. Psychometric properties of the strengths and
diffi culties questionnaire. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry
40:1337 1345.
Gornick MC, Addington A, Shaw P, Bobb AJ, Sharp W, Green-
stein D, et al. 2007. Association of the dopamine receptor D4
(DRD4) gene 7-repeat allele with children with attention-
defi cit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): an update. Am J Med
Genet Part B: Neuropsychiatr Genet 144:379 382.
Gottesman II, Gould TD. 2003. The endophenotype concept in
psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry
160:636 645.
Herrmann MJ, Walter A, Schreppel T, Ehlis AC, Pauli P, Lesch
KP, Fallgatter AJ. 2007. D4 receptor gene variation modulates
activation of prefrontal cortex during working memory. Eur J
Neurosci 26:2713 2718.
Kieling C, Roman T, Doyle AE, Hutz MH, Rohde LA. 2006.
Association between DRD4 gene and performance of children
with ADHD in a test of sustained attention. Biol Psychiatry
60:1163 1165.
Klingberg T, Fernell E, Olesen PJ, Johnson M, Gustafsson P,
Dahlstrom K, et al. 2005. Computerized training of working
memory in children with ADHD a randomized, controlled
trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 44:177 186.
Krain AL, Castellanos FX. 2006. Brain development and ADHD.
Clin Psychol Rev 26:433 444.
Kramer UM, Rojo N, Schule R, Cunillera T, Schols L, Marco-
Pallares J, et al. 2009. ADHD candidate gene (DRD4 exon III)
affects inhibitory control in a healthy sample. BMC Neurosci
10:150.
Kuntsi J, Rogers H, Swinard G, Borger N, van der MJ, Rijsdijk
F, Asherson P. 2006. Reaction time, inhibition, working mem-
ory and delay aversion performance: genetic infl uences and
their interpretation. Psychol Med 36:1613 1624.
Langley K, Marshall L, van den BM, Thomas H, Owen M,
O’Donovan M, Thapar A. 2004. Association of the dopamine
D4 receptor gene 7-repeat allele with neuropsychological test
performance of children with ADHD. Am J Psychiatry 161:
133 138.
Lasky-Su J, Banaschewski T, Buitelaar J, Franke B, Brookes K,
Sonuga-Barke E, et al. 2007. Partial replication of a DRD4
association in ADHD individuals using a statistically derived
quantitative trait for ADHD in a family based association test.
Biol Psychiatry 62:985 990.
Lasky-Su J, Lange C, Biederman J, Tsuang M, Doyle AE, Smoller
JW, et al. 2008. Family-based association analysis of a statisti-
cally derived quantitative traits for ADHD reveal an association
in DRD4 with inattentive symptoms in ADHD individuals. Am
J Med Genet Part B: Neuropsychiatr Genet 147B:100 106.
Lenroot RK, Schmitt JE, Ordaz SJ, Wallace GL, Neale MC, Lerch
JP, et al. 2009. Differences in genetic and environmental infl u-
ences on the human cerebral cortex associated with develop-
ment during childhood and adolescence. Hum Brain Mapp
30:163 174.
Li D, Sham PC, Owen MJ, He L. 2006. Meta-analysis shows
signifi cant association between dopamine system genes and
attention defi cit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Hum Mol
Genet 15:2276 2284.
Logan GD. 1994. On the ability to inhibit thought and action: a
users guide to the stop signal paradigm. In: Dagenbach D, Carr
TH, editors. Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and
language. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. p. 189 239.
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Yale University on 02/20/12
For personal use only.
D R D 4 a n d n e u r o c o g n i t i v e f u n c t i o n i n g i n A D H D 13
Loo SK, Rich EC, Ishii J, McGough J, McCracken J, Nelson
S, Smalley SL. 2008. Cognitive functioning in affected
sibling pairs with ADHD: familial clustering and dopamine
genes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Disciplines
49:950 957.
Meador-Woodruff JH, Damask SP, Wang J, Haroutunian V,
Davis KL, Watson SJ. 1996. Dopamine receptor mRNA
expression in human striatum and neocortex. Neuropsy-
chopharmacology 15:17 29.
Mill J, Caspi A, Williams BS, Craig I, Taylor A, Polo-Tomas
M, et al. 2006. Prediction of heterogeneity in intelligence
and adult prognosis by genetic polymorphisms in the
dopamine system among children with attention-defi cit/
hyperactivity disorder: evidence from 2 birth cohorts. Arch
Gen Psychiatry 63:462 469.
Monuteaux MC, Seidman LJ, Faraone SV, Makris N,
Spencer T, Valera E, et al. 2008. A preliminary study of
dopamine D4 receptor genotype and structural brain alter-
ations in adults with ADHD. Am J Med Genet Part B:
Neuropsychiatr Genet 147B:1436 1441.
O’Connell JR, Weeks DE. 1998. PedCheck: a program for
identifi cation of genotype incompatibilities in linkage anal-
ysis. Am J Hum Genet 63:259 266.
Plomin R, Fulker DW, Corley R, Defries JC. 1997. Nature,
nurture, and cognitive development from 1 to 16 years: a
parent-offspring adoption study. Psychol Sci 8:442 446.
Polderman TJ, Posthuma D, De Sonneville LM, Stins JF,
Verhulst FC, Boomsma DI. 2007. Genetic analyses of the
stability of executive functioning during childhood. Biol
Psychology 76:11 20.
Raymond M, Rousset F. 1995. GENEPOP (Version 1.2):
Population genetics software for exact tests and ecumeni-
cism. J Heredity 86:248 249.
Rommelse NN, Altink ME, Martin NC, Buschgens CJ,
Buitelaar JK, Sergeant JA, Oosterlaan J. 2008a. Neuropsy-
chological measures probably facilitate heritability research
of ADHD. Arch Clin Neuropsychol 23:579 591.
Rommelse NN, Altink ME, Oosterlaan J, Buschgens CJ,
Buitelaar J, Sergeant JA. 2008b. Support for an independ-
ent familial segregation of executive and intelligence endo-
phenotypes in ADHD families. Psychol Med 38:
1595 1606.
Rommelse NN, Altink ME, Arias-Vasquez A, Buschgens CJ,
Fliers E, Faraone SV, et al. 2008c. A review and analysis
of the relationship between neuropsychological measures
and DAT1 in ADHD. Am J Med Genet Part B: Neuropsy-
chiatr Genet 147B:1536 1546.
Seidman LJ. 2006. Neuropsychological functioning in people with
ADHD across the lifespan. Clin Psychol Rev 26:466 485.
Shaw P, Gornick M, Lerch J, Addington A, Seal J, Greenstein D, et al.
2007. Polymorphisms of the dopamine D4 receptor, clinical outcome,
and cortical structure in attention-defi cit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch
Gen Psychiatry 64:921 931.
Sonuga-Barke EJ, Brookes KJ, Buitelaar J, Anney R, Bitsakou P,
Baeyens D, et al. 2008. Intelligence in DSM-IV combined type
attention-defi cit/hyperactivity disorder is not predicted by either
dopamine receptor/transporter genes or other previously identifi ed
risk alleles for attention-defi cit/hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med
Genet Part B: Neuropsychiatr Genet 147:316 319.
Spear LP. 2000. The adolescent brain and age-related behavioral
manifestations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 24:417
463.
Staller JA, Faraone SV. 2007. Targeting the dopamine system in the
treatment of attention-defi cit/hyperactivity disorder. Expert Rev
Neurother 7:351 362.
Swanson J, Oosterlaan J, Murias M, Schuck S, Flodman P,
Spence MA, et al. 2000. Attention defi cit/hyperactivity disorder
children with a 7-repeat allele of the dopamine receptor D4
gene have extreme behavior but normal performance on critical
neuropsychological tests of attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA
97:4754 4759.
Szobot C, Roman T, Cunha R, Acton P, Hutz M, Rohde LA. 2005.
Brain perfusion and dopaminergic genes in boys with attention-
defi cit/hyperactivity disorder. Am J Med Genet Part B: Neuropsy-
chiatr Genet 132:53 58.
Tahir E, Yazgan Y, Cirakoglu B, Ozbay F, Waldman I, Asherson PJ.
2000. Association and linkage of DRD4 and DRD5 with attention
defi cit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in a sample of Turkish chil-
dren. Mol Psychiatry 5:396 404.
Taylor E, Schachar R, Thorley G, Wieselberg M. 1986. Conduct dis-
order and hyperactivity: I. Separation of hyperactivity and antisocial
conduct in British child psychiatric patients. Br J Psychiatry
149:760 767.
Thompson JL, Pogue-Geile MF, Grace AA. 2004. Developmental
pathology, dopamine, and stress: a model for the age of onset of
schizophrenia symptoms. Schizophr Bull 30:875 900.
van Meel CS, Oosterlaan J, Heslenfeld DJ, Sergeant JA. 2005. Moti-
vational effects on motor timing in attention-defi cit/hyperactivity
disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 44:451 460.
Wechsler D. 2002. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children
Derde Editie NL. Handleiding. London: The Psychological Cor-
poration.
Zhang K, Grady CJ, Tsapakis EM, Andersen SL, Tarazi FI, Bald-
essarini RJ. 2004. Regulation of working memory by dopamine D4
receptor in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 29:1648 1655.
World J Biol Psychiatry Downloaded from informahealthcare.com by Yale University on 02/20/12
For personal use only.
... At the behavioral level, 7-R allele carriers showed slower RT (Szekely et al., 2011) and a sample of children/adolescents with ADHD performed worse in working memory and executive tasks (Loo et al., 2008). However, in another study, adult 7-R allele carriers with ADHD performed better in a working memory task as compared to those without the 7-R allele (Boonstra et al., 2008) suggesting that the effects of DRD4 48 bp might depend on age and ADHD status (Altink et al., 2012). Additionally, the DRD4 48 bp 7-R allele polymorphism has been associated with noveltyseeking (Schinka et al., 2002), impulsivity (Eisenberg et al., 2007;Congdon et al., 2008;Varga et al., 2012), risk behavior (Dreber et al., 2009;Kuhnen and Chiao, 2009;Roussos et al., 2009) andADHD (LaHoste et al., 1996;Faraone et al., 1999). ...
... Third, overall recognition memory performance was higher in short allele carriers of the DRD4 48 bp polymorphism as compared to long allele carriers (at least one 7-R allele, Figure 6). This is partly consistent with previous research demonstrating decreased working memory performance in children carrying the 7-R allele (Froehlich et al., 2007;Altink et al., 2012). In contrast, others found better performance in cognitive tasks, including short-term working memory, in 7-R allele carriers (Boonstra et al., 2008), which might be mediated by ADHD status and age (Altink et al., 2012). ...
... This is partly consistent with previous research demonstrating decreased working memory performance in children carrying the 7-R allele (Froehlich et al., 2007;Altink et al., 2012). In contrast, others found better performance in cognitive tasks, including short-term working memory, in 7-R allele carriers (Boonstra et al., 2008), which might be mediated by ADHD status and age (Altink et al., 2012). Alternatively, the DRD4 gene might be relevant for attention selection of highly relevant information. ...
Article
Full-text available
In humans, monetary reward can promote behavioral performance including response times, accuracy, and subsequent recognition memory. Recent studies have shown that the dopaminergic system plays an essential role here, but the link to interindividual differences remains unclear. To further investigate this issue, we focused on previously described polymorphisms of genes affecting dopaminergic neurotransmission: DAT1 40 base pair (bp), DAT1 30 bp, DRD4 48 bp, and cannabinoid receptor type 1 (CNR1). Specifically, 669 healthy humans participated in a delayed recognition memory paradigm on two consecutive days. On the first day, male vs. female faces served as cues predicting an immediate monetary reward upon correct button presses. Subsequently, participants performed a remember/know recognition memory task on the same day and 1 day later. As predicted, reward increased accuracy and accelerated response times, which were modulated by DAT 30 bp. However, reward did not promote subsequent recognition memory performance and there was no interaction with any genotype tested here. Importantly, there were differential effects of genotype on declarative long-term memory independent of reward: (a) DAT1 40 bp was linked to the quality of memory with a more pronounced difference between recollection and familiarity in the heterozygous and homozygous 10-R as compared to homozygous 9-R; (b) DAT1 30 bp was linked to memory decay, which was most pronounced in homozygous 4-R; (c) DRD4 48 bp was linked to overall recognition memory with higher performance in the short allele group; and (d) CNR1 was linked to overall memory with reduced performance in the homozygous short group. These findings give new insights into how polymorphisms, which are related to dopaminergic neuromodulation, differentially affect long-term recognition memory performance.
... 12 Study in Jordan children showed that the 10-repeat allele of DAT1 associated with ADHD and the 7-repeat allele of DRD4 associated with ADHD in the children in European countries. 12,20 Instead of meta analysis studies in ADHD showed that a VNTR polymorphism in DAT1 and DRD4 gene had a small association to ADHD, Faraone et al. and Yang et al. suggested that these gene may be had role in the risk of ADHD. 21,22 On the other hand, conflicting results were reported in the studies in the Omani children, Han Chinese children, Iranian population and Turkish population which showed no association significant between a VNTR polymorphism with ADHD. ...
Article
Full-text available
Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most common neurobehavioural in the children. Genetic factor is known one of the factors which contributed in ADHD development. VNTR polymorphism in 3’UTR exon 15 of DAT1 gene and exon 3 of DRD4 gene are reported to be associated in ADHD. In this study we examine the association of ADHD with VNTR polymorphism of DAT1 and DRD4 gene in Indonesian children. Sixty-five ADHD children and 70 normal children (6- 13 years of age), were included in the study, we matched by age and gender. ADHD was diagnosed by DSM-IV. We performed a casecontrol study to found the association between ADHD and VNTR polymorphism of DAT1 and DRD4 genes. The 10-repeat allele of DAT1 and 2-repeat allele of DRD4 were higher in Indonesian children. Although the frequency of these allele was higher, but it was similar both in ADHD and control groups. Neither DAT1 nor DRD4 gene showed showed significant difference in genotype distribution and frequency allele between both groups (p > 0.05). No association between ADHD and VNTR polymorphism of DAT1 and DRD4 genes found in Indonesian children. This data suggest that DAT1 and DRD4 do not contribute to etiology of ADHD in Indonesian children. Further studies are needed to clarify association between VNTR polymorphism of DAT1 and DRD4 genetic with ADHD of Indonesian children in larger sample size and family based study.
... Regarding the results from Egger's test, for the 7R case, we observed presence of publication bias in European populations with a CC model (P = 0.018), but the P value becomes smaller (P = 0.0004) when CC model is merged with TDT study design. We observed that, when we eliminated from the analyses Sonuga-Barke and colleagues 47 along with Altink and colleagues 48 , the values are less significant and the P value for the total sample was 0.83. This could further mean the importance of studying this kind of polymorphism in samples where there are not mixed populations. ...
Article
Full-text available
The identification of biomarkers to support the diagnosis and prediction of treatment response for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is still a challenge. Our previous works highlighted the DRD4 (dopamine receptor D4) as the best potential genetic marker for childhood diagnosis and methylphenidate (MPH) response. Here, we aimed to provide additional evidence on biomarkers for ADHD diagnosis and treatment response, by using more specific approaches such as meta-analytic and bioinformatics tools. Via meta-analytic approaches including over 3000 cases and 16,000 controls, we demonstrated that, among the different variants studied in DRD4 gene, the 48-base pair, Variable Tandem Repeat Polymorphism, VNTR in exon 3 showed an age/population-specificity and an allelic heterogeneity. In particular, the 7R/“long” allele was identified as an ADHD risk factor in European-Caucasian populations (d = 1.31, 95%CI: 1.17–1.47, Z = 4.70/d = 1.36, 95%CI: 1.20–1.55, Z = 4.78, respectively), also, from the results of last meta-analysis, linked to the poor MPH efficacy. The 4R/“short” allele was a protective factor in European-Caucasian and South American populations (d = 0.83, 95%CI: 0.75–0.92, Z = 3.58), and was also associated to positive MPH response. These results refer to children with ADHD. No evidence of such associations was detected for adults with persistent ADHD (data from the last meta-analysis). Moreover, we found evidence that the 4R allele leads to higher receptor expression and increased sensitivity to dopamine, as compared with the 7R allele (d = 1.20, 95%CI: 0.71–1.69, Z = 4.81), and this is consistent with the ADHD protection/susceptibility effects of the respective alleles. Using bioinformatics tools, based on the latest genome-wide association (GWAS) meta-analysis of the Psychiatry Genomic Consortium (PGC), we demonstrated that the 48 bp VNTR is not in Linkage Disequilibrium with the DRD4 SNPs (Single Nucleotide Polymorphisms), which were not found to be associated with ADHD. Moreover, a DRD4 expression downregulation was found in ADHD specific brain regions (Putamen, Z score = −3.02, P = 0.00252). Overall, our results suggest that DRD4 48 bp VNTR variants should be considered as biomarkers to support the diagnosis of ADHD and to predict MPH response, although the accuracy of such a biomarker remains to be further elucidated.
... Genetic polymorphisms found on the dopamine type 2 receptor gene (DRD2 gene, rs1799732 [−141delC]) or its regulators (rs1800497 [Taq1A]) were linked to cognitive performance [40] and to ADHD core traits and co-morbidity [41]. Similarly, DRD4 exon III VNTR has been implicated in the development of ADHD and impulsivity [42,43], and dopamine transporter (DAT1 gene) VNTR has been related to cognitive flexibility [44] and risk taking [45]. In this study, we aimed at using a multilocus approach, in which the genetic variation of these five important polymorphisms (rs1800497 [Taq1A], COMT Val158Met [rs4680], DRD2 rs1799732 [−141delC], DAT1 and DRD4 VNTRs) was considered at the same time. ...
Article
Full-text available
Background Fetal adversity, evidenced by poor fetal growth for instance, is associated with increased risk for several diseases later in life. Classical cut-offs to characterize small (SGA) and large for gestational age (LGA) newborns are used to define long term vulnerability. We aimed at exploring the possible dynamism of different birth weight cut-offs in defining vulnerability in developmental outcomes (through the Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development), using the example of a gene vs. fetal adversity interaction considering gene choices based on functional relevance to the studied outcome. Methods 36-month-old children from an established prospective birth cohort (Maternal Adversity, Vulnerability, and Neurodevelopment) were classified according to birth weight ratio (BWR) (SGA ≤0.85, LGA >1.15, exploring a wide range of other cut-offs) and genotyped for polymorphisms associated with dopamine signaling (TaqIA-A1 allele, DRD2-141C Ins/Ins, DRD4 7-repeat, DAT1-10- repeat, Met/Met-COMT), composing a score based on the described function, in which hypofunctional variants received lower scores. Results There were 251 children (123 girls and 128 boys). Using the classic cut-offs (0.85 and 1.15), there were no statistically significant interactions between the neonatal groups and the dopamine genetic score. However, when changing the cut-offs, it is possible to see ranges of BWR that could be associated with vulnerability to poorer development according to the variation in the dopamine function. Conclusion The classic birth weight cut-offs to define SGA and LGA newborns should be seen with caution, as depending on the outcome in question, the protocols for long-term follow up could be either too inclusive—therefore most costly, or unable to screen true vulnerabilities—and therefore ineffective to establish early interventions and primary prevention.
... hD4.7), with the global frequency of 64% and 21%, respectively (Chang et al., 1996). Clinical studies have found a high prevalence of the hD4.7 variant in children diagnosed with ADHD (Grady et al., 2003;Altink et al., 2012;Berry et al., 2013;Trejo et al., 2015), and in people with personality traits of novelty seeking, risk taking, substance abuse and impulsivity (Ebstein et al., 1996;Benjamin et al., 1996;Li et al., 2004;Reiner and Spangler, 2011). ...
Article
Full-text available
The human dopamine D4 receptor (hD4R) variants with long tandem repeats in the third intracellular loop have been strongly associated with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and risk taking behaviors. To understand the potential molecular mechanism underlying the connection, we have investigated the synaptic function of human D4R polymorphism by virally expressing the ADHD-linked 7-repeat allele, hD4.7, or its normal counterpart, hD4.4, in the prefrontal cortex (PFC) of D4R knockout mice. We found that hD4R bound to the SH3 domain of PSD-95 in a state-dependent manner. Activation of hD4.7 caused more reduction of NR1/PSD-95 binding and NR1 surface expression than hD4.4 in PFC slices. Moreover, the NMDAR-mediated excitatory postsynaptic currents (NMDAR-EPSC) in PFC pyramidal neurons were suppressed to a larger extent by hD4.7 than hD4.4 activation. Direct stimulation of NMDARs with the partial agonist d-cycloserine prevented the NMDAR hypofunction induced by hD4.7 activation. Moreover, hD4.7-expressing mice exhibited the increased exploratory and novelty seeking behaviors, mimicking the phenotypic hallmark of human ADHD. d-cycloserine administration ameliorated the ADHD-like behaviors in hD4.7-expressing mice. Our results suggest that over-suppression of NMDAR function may underlie the role of hD4.7 in ADHD, and enhancing NMDAR signaling may be a viable therapeutic strategy to ADHD humans carrying the D4.7 allele.
... nasilona, jeżeli w miejscu polimorficznym dochodzi do siedmiokrotnego powtórzenia 48 nukleotydowej sekwencji [8,18,28,56] . Polimorfizm ten pogarsze wyniki w testach oceniających pamięć roboczą [4]. Mill i wsp. ...
Article
Full-text available
Intelligence as an ability to reason, think abstractly and adapt effectively to the environment is a subject of research in the field of psychology, neurobiology, and in the last twenty years genetics as well. Genetical testing of twins carried out from XX century indicated heritebility of intelligence, therefore confirmed an influence of genetic factor on cognitive processes. Studies on genetic background of intelligence focus on dopaminergic (DRD2, DRD4, COMT, SLC6A3, DAT1, CCKAR) and adrenergic system (ADRB2, CHRM2) genes as well as, neutrofins (BDNF) and oxidative stress genes (LTF, PRNP). Positive effect of investigated gene polymorphism was indicated by variation c.957C>T DRD2 gene (if in polymorphic site is thymine), polymorphism c.472G>A COMT gene (presence of adenine) and also gene ADRB2 c.46A> G (guanine), CHRM2 (thymine in place c.1890A>T) and BDNF (guanine in place c.472G>A)Obtained results indicate that intelligence is a feature dependent not only on genetic but also an environmental factor.
... nasilona, jeżeli w miejscu polimorficznym dochodzi do siedmiokrotnego powtórzenia 48 nukleotydowej sekwencji [8,18,28,56] . Polimorfizm ten pogarsze wyniki w testach oceniających pamięć roboczą [4]. Mill i wsp. ...
Article
Full-text available
Intelligence as an ability to reason, think abstractly and adapt effectively to the environment is a subject of research in the field of psychology, neurobiology, and in the last twenty years genetics as well. Genetical testing of twins carried out from XX century indicated heritebility of intelligence, therefore confirmed an influence of genetic factor on cogniti-ve processes. Studies on genetic background of intelligence focus on dopaminergic (DRD2, DRD4, COMT, SLC6A3, DAT1, CCKAR) and adrenergic system (ADRB2, CHRM2) genes as well as, neutrofins (BDNF) and oxidative stress genes (LTF, PRNP). Positive effect of investigated gene polymorphism was indicated by variation c.957C>T DRD2 gene (if in polymorphic site is thy-mine), polymorphism c.472G>A COMT gene (presence of adenine) and also gene ADRB2 c.46A->G (guanine), CHRM2 (thymine in place c.1890A>T) and BDNF (guanine in place c.472G>A) Obtained results indicate that intelligence is a feature dependent not only on genetic but also an environmental factor. intelligence • gene • polymorphism • DRD2 • DRD4 • COMT • SLC6A3 • DAT1 • CCKAR • ADRB2 • CHRM2 • BDNF • LTF • PRNP Streszczenie Inteligencja jako zdolność do postrzegania, analizy i adaptacji do zmian otoczenia jest przed-miotem wielu badań z zakresu psychologii, neurobiologii, a w ostatnim dwudziestoleciu także z zakresu genetyki. Badania nad bliźniętami, zapoczątkowane w XX w., wskazały na dziedzi-czenie inteligencji, a wiec dowiodły wpływu czynnika genetycznego na procesy poznawcze. Badania nad genetycznym podłożem inteligencji dotyczyły m.in. genów układu dopaminer-gicznego (DRD2, DRD4, COMT, SLC6A3, DAT1, CCKAR), adrenergicznego (ADRB2, CHRM2), genów neutrofin (BDNF) oraz genów, których produkty są zaangażowane w neutralizację wolnych rodników tlenowych (LTF, PRNP). Korzystny związek z inteligencja wykazywał polimorfizm c.957C>T genu DRD2 (jeśli w miejscu polimorficznym była tymina), polimorfizm genu COMT c.472G>A (obecność adeniny), a także genu ADRB2 c.46A>G (guanina), CHRM2 (tymina w po-zycji c.1890A>T) i BDNF (guanina w pozycji c.472G>A). Uzyskane wyniki dowodzą, że inteli-gencja jest cechą złożoną, zależną nie tylko od czynników genetycznych, ale także czynni-ków środowiskowych. inteligencja • gen • polimorfizm • DRD2 • DRD4 • COMT • SLC6A3 • DAT1 • CCKAR • ADRB2 • CHRM2 • BDNF • LTF • PRNP
Article
The high affinity dopamine D4 receptor ligand APH199 and derivatives thereof exhibit bias toward the Gi signaling pathway over β-arrestin recruitment compared to quinpirole. Based on APH199, two novel groups of D4 subtype selective ligands were designed and evaluated, in which the original benzyl phenylsemicarbazide substructure was replaced by either a biphenylmethyl urea or a biphenyl urea moiety. Functional assays revealed a range of different bias profiles among the newly synthesized compounds, namely, with regard to efficacy, potency, and GRK2 dependency, in which bias factors range from 1 to over 300 and activation from 15% to over 98% compared to quinpirole. These observations demonstrate that within bias, an even more precise tuning toward a particular profile is possible, which─in a general sense─could become an important aspect in future drug development. Docking studies enabled further insight into the role of the ECL2 and the EPB in the emergence of bias, thereby taking advantage of the diversity of functionally selective D4 agonists now available.
Book
Full-text available
We have studied important differences between the urbanization of "wild" species of birds and mammals and the domestication of domestic ones, together with the difference in stressors to which they adapt. In the first case, these are the most general characteristics of the urban environment - an extreme level of heterogeneity, instability and variability compared to any non-urban landscapes. It requires "urban" birds to quickly change nesting sites, feeding habitats, feeding methods and other features of biology in the wake of habitat changes, local and citywide, with the development of the ability to predict them, live, constantly "jumping from ice floe to ice floe" as opposed to sustainability existence in "rural" or "forest" populations. In the second, it is simply a change in the reaction to people, equipment, and animal care from an anxious-defensive to a friendly-interesting one. In the process of urbanization of "wild" species of birds, the brain increases, as in other variants of extreme habitats. Cognitive progress is achieved by each "urban" individual independently, due to the developing impact of the urban environment on its psyche, thanks to the growth of the possibilities for evaluating and predicting its dynamics using precursor signals. Therefore, it is preceded by an increase in the courage of individuals, a better differentiation of stimuli by them, a separation of significant ones from all the others (to which indifference is growing). On the contrary, during domestication, the brain decreases, cognitive progress in the new environment is achieved due to the “cooperative thinking”, social “prompts” of people and relatives. Behavioral changes during the urbanization of "wild" species are also sharply different from domestic ones. An analysis of aggression, defensive, exploratory behavior, attitudes towards the novelty of "urban" birds and mammals shows that they do not become "kind" or "trustful" (as happened with domestic animals). On the contrary: their aggression increases, along with courage and a better response to potential danger, a more accurate differentiation of it from “just anxiety”, to which they become more indifferent. As the species urbanizes, the behavior of each of the individuals becomes more diverse. Aggression, courage, flight from potential danger or vice versa, taking risks, exploring new places and objects no longer characterize individuals, but situations. All behavior is made as flexible and contextual as possible, with better recognition of the specifics of the situation, better choice of the mode of action, more accurate “dosing” of reactions according to the goal. This is the developing role of the urban environment. Urbanization destroys the behavioral syndrome: the correlations existing in the original populations between exploration, courage, aggression, risk taking, and in some cases other parameters. The launches of the forms of behavior characterized by each of these features in "urban" birds are mutually independent, in contrast to "rural" individuals. This maximizes the accuracy of the choice of behavior in a problem situation and its switching to another according to the situation, which is opposite to the changes associated with domestication and inconsistent with their explanation based on the D.K. Belyaev model. Urbanization changes the life strategy in a completely different way than domestication. The strategy of the newly formed urban population in the r-K-continuum shifts towards a “more pronounced K” compared to the initial one, due to a set of changes that mutually determine and reinforce each other: 1) Population growth occurs to a greater extent due to the lengthening of the average life expectancy, while reducing the reproduction of individuals (partly similar to the demography of Homo sapiens); 2) The primacy of future reproduction in the best conditions, with a directed movement to search for them “following the dynamics of the urban environment”, compared with the maximum reproductive effort “here and now”; 3) Greater “fractionality” of the reproductive potential of “urban” individuals, subdivided into a greater number of breeding attempts compared to the original population, with greater mobility and contextuality of each of them. Domesticated species behave in the opposite way and shift the strategy towards the "r-pole" of the continuum. A partly similar result during urbanization and domestication is achieved by opposite changes in the life strategy, behavior, cognitive characteristics, attitudes towards humans, and other aspects of the "natural history" of the species. The “individuality genes” DRD4 and SERT, which are under positive selection during urbanization, do not participate in domestication-related changes in the genome. On this basis, an evolutionary scenario for the urbanization of "wild" species is proposed, explaining on the same basis both differences and similarities with domestication. Modern cities, their expansion and association into groups (urbanization) are very interesting as arenas of the fastest microevolutionary processes that "separate" the urban population from the original and adapt it mainly to the most general features of the urban environment, but only gradually, then, and not completely - to specific influences that it "dumps" on individuals of this species. This happens in each region separately: the newly formed populations in different cities change parallel to each other (the same is true for urbanization changes in different, completely unrelated species), but do not approach each other in any way, remaining genetically closer to the local initial ones than to urban populations in other regions. Human-modified ("man-made") landscapes create extreme habitats, cities are its quintessence. We consider the adaptation of "wild" species of birds, partly mammals, to such changes, occurring through directed invasion into it and rapid changes in the population structure, ecology and behavior of individuals, restoring viability in new conditions and facilitating their even greater development, with penetration into areas, all more and more modified by man. The settlement of the urban environment is the culmination of all processes of this kind. Microevolution goes faster here than in natural landscapes (the faster, the stronger the transformation, with a maximum in the urban environment), but "gets stuck" at the stage of adaptation. Form formation - the appearance of "urban" subspecies, species, etc. - does not happen, despite the growing separation of "urban" populations from the original ones.
Article
Full-text available
There are several sources that determine school abilities: endogenous factors, i.e. abilities, intelligence, personality, internal motivation, and exogenous factors, such as environment and external motivation. The causes of school failures are found most often in the level of fluid or crystallised intelligence. In particular in the fluid intelligence, which is genetically conditioned. The type of personality also affects the achievement in learning, as well as the pleasure derived from it. The motives (for which students take action) can also have an impact on achievements – school abilities. There are two types of motivation: internal and external. The internal, exogenous motivation has an extremely strong positive impact and it should be stimulated by mentors – teachers. Furthermore, there are several types of general abilities that also affect school competences. Abilities should be noticed and developed during school education. Undoubtedly, the educational system should develop creativity and support the development of all individual characteristics, providing opportunity for self-fulfilment.
Article
A polyclonal antiserum was generated against a unique peptide fragment in the rat D4 dopamine (DA) receptor. The titer was monitored using solid-phase ELISA and once it was established, specificity was assessed using Chinese Hamster Ovary (CHO) cells, stably transfected with the full-length cDNA for the rat D4 DA receptor. Immunofluorescent staining produced by incubation with the anti-D4 DA receptor antiserum was selective for D4 DA receptor-transfected CHO cells, and was expressed at their cell membranes and cytoplasm. Attenuated staining for D4 DA receptor protein was visible in untransfected, Kl CHO cells, and in D2 or D3 DA receptor-transfected CHO cells. The regional and cellular CNS distribution patterns for the D4 DA receptor subtype were examined, and illustrated significant protein levels within the frontal (FCx) and parietal cortices. Lesser amounts of receptor protein staining occurred in the thalamus, globus pallidus, hippocampus, cerebellar vermis, and very low expression was detected in the striatum (CPu). D4 DA receptor protein staining was correlated with the cellular expression of its mRNA transcripts in these same brain regions using concurrent fluorescent analyses. The homologous coincidence in staining patterns for the D4 DA receptor transcripts and encoded proteins in identified neurons of the FCx and CPu showed variations in receptor expression in these identified basal ganglia pathways.
Article
Children increasingly resemble their parents in cognitive abilities from infancy through adolescence. Results obtained from a 20-year longitudinal adoption study of 245 adopted children and their biological and adoptive parents, as well as 245 matched nonadoptive (control) parents and offspring, show that this increasing resemblance is due to genetic factors. Adopted children resemble their adoptive parents slightly in early childhood but not at all in middle childhood or adolescence. In contrast, during childhood and adolescence, adopted children become more like their biological parents, and to the same degree as children and parents in control families. Although these results were strongest for general cognitive ability and verbal ability, similar results were found for other specific cognitive abilities - spatial ability, speed of processing, and recognition memory. These findings indicate that, within this population, genes that stably affect cognitive abilities in adulthood do not all come into play until adolescence and that environmental factors that contribute to cognitive development are not correlated with parents' cognitive ability.
Article
presents a users' guide to the stop signal paradigm / explains the paradigm and the situations in the natural and artificial environment the paradigm is intended to capture / explains how the inhibition that is apparent in the stop signal paradigm is similar to and different from the kinds of inhibition seen in other paradigms / reviews theory and data in the experimental literature on stopping / explains the race model, which is the commonly accepted theory of performance in the stop signal paradigm, and it explains how to apply the model to data [in this paradigm] people are engaged in a primary task, and occasionally, they are presented with a signal that tells them to stop their response to the primary task / the primary task is typically a visual choice reaction time task . . . and the stop signal is usually a tone / the main datum is whether or not Ss withhold their response to the primary task when the stop signal occurs (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)
Article
A major goal of genetic studies of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is to identify individual characteristics that might help segregate the disorder's inherent heterogeneity. [Mill et al. (2006); Arch Ger Psychiatry 63:462–469] recently reported a potentially important association between two dopamine-related risk polymorphisms (DRD4 variable number tandem repeat (VNTR) in exon 3 and DAT1 VNTR in the 3′ UTR) and lowered IQ in ADHD. The objective of the current study was to replicate the [Mill et al. (2006); Arch Ger Psychiatry 63:462–469] findings in a clinical sample and to extend the analysis to a large range of alternative SNP markers of putative ADHD risk alleles identified in a recent study [Brookes et al. (2006); Mol Genet 11:934–953]. Participants were 1081 children and adolescents with a research-confirmed combined type ADHD diagnosis and 1300 unaffected siblings who took part in the International Multi-centre ADHD Genetics (IMAGE) project. They were recruited from multiple settings from across Europe: Belgium, Britain, Germany, Ireland, Israel, Netherlands, Spain and Switzerland. The results were that ADHD was associated with reduced IQ. However, there was no association between the two dopamine-related risk polymorphisms and IQ in either the probands or their siblings. Furthermore, other selected genetic markers previously demonstrated to be associated with ADHD in this sample were not associated with IQ. This large scale study with a clinically ascertained and regorously diagnosed sample failed to replicate the association between genetic polymorphisms in the dopamine system and IQ in ADHD. We also observed no association of other SNPs with IQ in ADHD. © 2007 Wiley-Liss, Inc.