Zoe Steine-Hanson’s research while affiliated with Oregon State University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (9)


Fig. 1. (A) Arch itecture of the Common Model of Cognition, as describe d by Laird et al. (2 01 7) . (B) Theoretical ma pping be tween CMC components and homologous cortical and subcortical regions, as used in this stud y's pipeline to identify the equivalent Regions of Interest (ROIs). (C) Progressive approxima tion of the ROIs, from high-level func tional ma ppings (left) to task-level group results (m iddle, with group-level centroid coordinated ma rked by a color circle) to the individual func tional centroids of the regions in our sample (right; each individual centroid represented by a "+" ma rker; note that hu ndreds of ma rkers are overlapping in each region). Group-level and individual-level data come from the Relational Reasoning task.
Fig. 4. Lateral view of the distribution of the ROI centroids across individual participants and tasks. Each "+" ma rker represents the centroid of an ROI for one participant. Colors represent the components, following the conventions of Fig 1A-C. The background represents the statistical parame tric ma p (in greyscale) of the corresponding group-level analysis used to identify the seed coordinates for each ROI (Step 2 in Fig. 1C).
Fig. 5. (A) Visual representation of the hierarch ical Bayesian modeling procedu re. (B) Visual representation of two arch itectures' probability distributions r k , shown as the two thick grey and black cu rves. The red dashed line represents the expected probability of the winning arch itecture; (C) Visual representation of the winning arch itecture's exceedance probability, that is, the proportion of a probability distribution that is greater than any other. In the case of two possible models (k = 2) , the exceedance probability redu ces to the area to the right of r k = 0.5. Modified from Stephan et al. (2 00 9) .
Fig. 6. Results of the Bayesian model comparisons. In all plots, different colors represent different arch itectures. (A-G) Probability distributions that each of the seven arch itectures is true, given the data within each task and across all tasks combined. Vertical dotted lines represent the me an of each distribution, i.e. the expected probability of each model. (H) Corresponding exceedance probabilities, represented as stacked bars for each task.
Fig. 7. Follow-up Bayesian model comparisons, after the six alternative arch itectures have be en augme nted with bilateral Perception-Ac tion connections. In all plots, different colors represent different arch itectures. (A-G) Probability distributions that each of the seven arch itectures is true, given the data within each task and across all tasks combined. Vertical dotted lines represent the me an of each distribution, i.e. the expected probability of each model. (H) Corresponding exceedance probabilities represented as stacked bars for each task.

+1

Analysis of the Human Connectome Data Supports the Notion of A “Common Model of Cognition” for Human and Human-Like Intelligence Across Domains
  • Article
  • Full-text available

April 2021

·

362 Reads

·

40 Citations

NeuroImage

·

·

Zoe Steine-Hanson

·

[...]

·

The Common Model of Cognition (CMC) is a recently proposed, consensus architecture intended to capture decades of progress in cognitive science on modeling human and human-like intelligence. Because of the broad agreement around it and preliminary mappings of its components to specific brain areas, we hypothesized that the CMC could be a candidate model of the large-scale functional architecture of the human brain. To test this hypothesis, we analyzed functional MRI data from 200 participants and seven different tasks that cover a broad range of cognitive domains. The CMC components were identified with functionally homologous brain regions through canonical fMRI analysis, and their communication pathways were translated into predicted patterns of effective connectivity between regions. The resulting dynamic linear model was implemented and fitted using Dynamic Causal Modeling, and compared against six alternative brain architectures that had been previously proposed in the field of neuroscience (three hierarchical architectures and three hub-and-spoke architectures) using a Bayesian approach. The results show that, in all cases, the CMC vastly outperforms all other architectures, both within each domain and across all tasks. These findings suggest that a common set of architectural principles that could be used for artificial intelligence also underpins human brain function across multiple cognitive domains.

Download


A Common Architecture for Human and Artificial Cognition Explains Brain Activity Across Domains

July 2019

·

496 Reads

·

1 Citation

The Common Model of Cognition (CMC) is a consensus architecture for human and human-like artificial cognition. We hypothesized that, because of its generality, the CMC could be a candidate model of the large-scale functional architecture of the human brain. To this end, we analyzed neuroimaging from N=200 participants across seven tasks that cover the broad range of cognitive domains. The CMC framework was translated into a model of neural connectivity between brain regions homologous to CMC components. After the model was implemented and fitted using Dynamic Causal Modeling, its performance was compared against four alternative large-scale brain architectures that had been previously proposed in the field of neuroscience. The results show that the CMC outperforms the other four architectures within and across all domains. These findings suggest that a common, functional computational blueprint for human-like intelligence also captures the neural architecture that underpins human cognition.


Engineering Gender-Inclusivity into Software: Tales from the Trenches

May 2019

·

75 Reads

Although the need for gender-inclusivity in software itself is gaining attention among both SE researchers and SE practitioners, and methods have been published to help, little has been reported on how to make such methods work in real-world settings. For example, how do busy software practitioners use such methods in low-cost ways? How do they endeavor to maximize benefits from using them? How do they avoid the controversies that can arise in talking about gender? To find out how teams were handling these and similar questions, we turned to 10 real-world software teams. We present these teams experiences "in the trenches," in the form of 12 practices and 3 potential pitfalls, so as to provide their insights to other real-world software teams trying to engineer gender-inclusivity into their software products.


Fixing Inclusivity Bugs for Information Processing Styles and Learning Styles

May 2019

·

37 Reads

Most software systems today do not support cognitive diversity. Further, because of differences in problem-solving styles that cluster by gender, software that poorly supports cognitive diversity can also embed gender biases. To help software professionals fix gender bias "bugs" related to people's problem-solving styles for information processing and learning of new software we collected inclusivity fixes from three sources. The first two are empirical studies we conducted: a heuristics-driven user study and a field research industry study. The third is data that we obtained about a before/after user study of inclusivity bugs. The resulting seven potential inclusivity fixes show how to debug software to be more inclusive for diverse problem-solving styles.


From Gender Biases to Gender-Inclusive Design: An Empirical Investigation

April 2019

·

329 Reads

·

137 Citations

In recent years, research has revealed gender biases in numerous software products. But although some researchers have found ways to improve gender participation in specific software projects, general methods focus mainly on detecting gender biases -- not fixing them. To help fill this gap, we investigated whether the GenderMag bias detection method can lead directly to designs with fewer gender biases. In our 3-step investigation, two HCI researchers analyzed an industrial software product using GenderMag; we derived design changes to the product using the biases they found; and ran an empirical study of participants using the original product versus the new version. The results showed that using the method in this way did improve the software's inclusiveness: women succeeded more often in the new version than in the original; men's success rates improved too; and the gender gap entirely disappeared.


Refining the Common Model of Cognition Through Large Neuroscience Data

December 2018

·

187 Reads

·

19 Citations

Procedia Computer Science

The Common Model of Cognition (CMC) is an effort to highlight the commonalities between multiple studies of human-like intelligent minds and cognition, and bring these architectures and functions together into one common model. The CMC is still relatively new, and while it has substantial theoretical evidence in its favor, there is little empirical evidence confirming the theory. The CMC is fundamentally informed by human cognition, and must at least hold true for human brains. As such, this paper uses a large fMRI dataset from the Human Connectome Project (HCP) to refine the CMC and test it as a reasonable model for human cognition. We tested three models in the CMC family and two alternative models against two tasks from the HCP dataset. We found that one model from the CMC family explained the HCP dataset best, providing further empirical evidence in favor of the CMC, whilst also suggesting a slight modification to the CMC itself that may improve the model’s generalizability.


Pedagogical Content Knowledge for Teaching Inclusive Design

August 2018

·

75 Reads

·

33 Citations

Inclusive design is important in today's software industry, but there is little research about how to teach it. In collaboration with 9 teacher-researchers across 8 U.S. universities and more than 400 computer and information science students, we embarked upon an Action Research investigation to gather insights into the pedagogical content knowledge (PCK) that teachers need to teach a particular inclusive design method called GenderMag. Analysis of the teachers' observations and experiences, the materials they used, direct observations of students' behaviors, and multiple data on the students' own reflections on their learning revealed 11 components of inclusive design PCK. These include strategies for anticipating and addressing resistance to the topic of inclusion, strategies for modeling and scaffolding perspective taking, and strategies for tailoring instruction to students' prior beliefs and biases.


Open source barriers to entry, revisited: a sociotechnical perspective

May 2018

·

101 Reads

·

148 Citations

Research has revealed that significant barriers exist when entering Open-Source Software (OSS) communities and that women disproportionately experience such barriers. However, this research has focused mainly on social/cultural factors, ignoring the environment itself --- the tools and infrastructure. To shed some light onto how tools and infrastructure might somehow factor into OSS barriers to entry, we conducted a field study with five teams of software professionals, who worked through five use-cases to analyze the tools and infrastructure used in their OSS projects. These software professionals found tool/infrastructure barriers in 7% to 71% of the use-case steps that they analyzed, most of which are tied to newcomer barriers that have been established in the literature. Further, over 80% of the barrier types they found include attributes that are biased against women.

Citations (7)


... This finding is also consistent with the classic idea that certain computational principles may be shared across cortical brain areas and may provide an individual fingerprint across various functional levels of hierarchy [88][89][90] . Recent observations of shared activity patterns in the early developing cortex 91 could explain the domainoverarching consistency of representational patterns in individuals 91 as well as other cross-modal correlations of cognitive performance 92,93 and the stability of representational architectures despite fluctuating activity at the single-neuron level [94][95][96] . ...

Reference:

Perceptual and semantic maps in individual humans share structural features that predict creative abilities
Analysis of the Human Connectome Data Supports the Notion of A “Common Model of Cognition” for Human and Human-Like Intelligence Across Domains

NeuroImage

... The SES-facet survey could help recruit participants who cover wide spectra of SES-facet values. Hilderbrand et al. reported an analogous example of the latter, in which a company used the GenderMag survey to recruit a participant pool balanced with respect to the GenderMag facets [Hilderbrand et al. 2020]. ...

Engineering gender-inclusivity into software: ten teams' tales from the trenches
  • Citing Conference Paper
  • June 2020

... To obtain neuronal activity and dynamics, the neuronal circuit needs to be monitored and/or manipulated. Recent approaches to record such activity include brain wide twophoton calcium single neuron imaging in vivo of C. elegans (Kato et al., 2015), wide-field calcium imaging of regions of the brain of behaving mice (Zatka-Haas et al., 2020), twophoton calcium imaging (Villette et al., 2019), Neuropixel recordings of single neurons in the brain of behaving mice (Steinmetz et al., 2018(Steinmetz et al., , 2019, and functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI) recordings of voxels in the human brain as part of the Human Connectome Project (Van Essen et al., 2012;Stocco et al., 2019). It is noteworthy that FC aims to capture statistical dependencies based on neural recordings and does not rely on the underlying anatomical connectivity, thereby FC methods are applicable for different scales of neural data-micro (Hill et al., 2012), meso (Passamonti et al., 2015), and macro (Mumford and Ramsey, 2014). ...

A Common Architecture for Human and Artificial Cognition Explains Brain Activity Across Domains

... Leavy [3] emphasizes the need for more gender-diverse teams in order to avoid bias in the development of artificial intelligence. In software development it is also crucial to aim for diversity in the development teams, as men and women have been found to use software in different ways and thus have different needs [4], [5]. Gender balance in computing education benefits all genders, as pointed out by Lagesen's et al. research: not only women's, but men's drop-out rates were reduced with an increased gender balance [6]. ...

From Gender Biases to Gender-Inclusive Design: An Empirical Investigation
  • Citing Conference Paper
  • April 2019

... The Common Model of Cognition describes several key modules: perceptual and motor modules for interacting with the agent's environment, short-term/working memory buffers for holding the active data in the agent's mind, a (Laird, Lebiere, and Rosenbloom 2017), associated brain areas (Stocco et al. 2021;Steine-Hanson, Koh, and Stocco 2018;Stocco et al. 2018) and our approach to modelling each module. Solid arrows pass data while dashed arrows modulate data passing. ...

Refining the Common Model of Cognition Through Large Neuroscience Data

Procedia Computer Science

... Personas are fictional characters created from data about a specific group of people [78]. Personas typically include background information, goals, or behaviors, and are commonly used in product design, user experience research, and education [39,73,78]. Personas can help users focus their attention on a specific target audience, which can make users feel more empathy towards that group of people [64,78,87]. ...

Pedagogical Content Knowledge for Teaching Inclusive Design
  • Citing Conference Paper
  • August 2018

... Research highlights that information presentation in OSS projects [7], [8]-such as documentation and issue descriptions-often favors specific problem-solving styles (e.g., hands-on learners) over others (e.g., process-oriented learners), contributing to gender biases. While additional studies have further examined gender bias in OSS [9], [10], [11], [12], [13], there is still a need to adapt OSS environments to support diverse contributors effectively [14]. ...

Open source barriers to entry, revisited: a sociotechnical perspective
  • Citing Conference Paper
  • May 2018