Yilin Yu’s research while affiliated with Education University of Hong Kong and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (4)


The Longitudinal Pathways Linking SES to Reading Comprehension
  • Article

February 2025

·

56 Reads

Journal of Literacy Research

·

·

Yilin Yu

·

[...]

·

Guided by the family investment model, family process model, and componential model of reading, this study examined underlying mechanisms linking family socioeconomic status (SES) to reading comprehension. Participants were 682 Chinese third graders (Wave1, M age = 9.31 years, 338 girls) randomly recruited, and they were followed up after 1 year. Structural equation modeling revealed that higher parents’ education was related to more books in the home and higher levels of positive family relationship, which was associated with better language skills directly or indirectly through stronger reading interest—better language skills ultimately linked to better reading comprehension. No significant indirect pathways were found from family income to reading comprehension. The mediating pathways were similar for children with different parental migration statuses.


Fig. 4. Topographical maps of different conditions in the 120-180 ms time windows for three Grade groups. H-L: the voltage difference between high and low consistency conditions; H-M: the voltage difference o between high and moderate consistency conditions; M-L: the voltage difference between moderate and low consistency conditions.
Fig. 5. Topographical maps of different conditions in the 210-250 ms time windows for three Grade groups. M-H: the voltage difference between moderate and high consistency conditions; M-L: the voltage difference between moderate and low consistency conditions; L-H: the voltage difference between low and high consistency conditions.
Fig. 6. Topographical maps of different conditions in the 320-370 ms time windows for three Grade groups. L-H: the voltage difference between low and high consistency conditions; L-M: the voltage difference between low and moderate consistency conditions; M-H: the voltage difference between moderate and high consistency conditions.
Fig. 7. Time-point-wise ERP effects of consistency. The TANOVA results of low versus high and moderate versus high contrasts showed differences in the P1-N170 component and the other two time ranges (dark grey frames are from 164 to 242 ms, 402-434 ms, and 446-500 ms, respectively. T-maps are displayed on the right).
Reaction Time and Accuracy Rate in the Learning Phase and Recognition Test for Three Conditions among Three Grades.
Corrigendum to “Multiple mechanisms regulate statistical learning of orthographic regularities in school-age children: Neurophysiological evidence” [Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 59C (2023) 101190]
  • Article
  • Full-text available

February 2023

·

122 Reads

Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience

Download

Fig. 4. Topographical maps of different conditions in the 120-180 ms time windows for three Grade groups. H-L: the voltage difference between high and low consistency conditions; H-M: the voltage difference o between high and moderate consistency conditions; M-L: the voltage difference between moderate and low consistency conditions.
Fig. 5. Topographical maps of different conditions in the 210-250 ms time windows for three Grade groups. M-H: the voltage difference between moderate and high consistency conditions; M-L: the voltage difference between moderate and low consistency conditions; L-H: the voltage difference between low and high consistency conditions.
Fig. 6. Topographical maps of different conditions in the 320-370 ms time windows for three Grade groups. L-H: the voltage difference between low and high consistency conditions; L-M: the voltage difference between low and moderate consistency conditions; M-H: the voltage difference between moderate and high consistency conditions.
Fig. 7. Time-point-wise ERP effects of consistency. The TANOVA results of low versus high and moderate versus high contrasts showed differences in the P1-N170 component and the other two time ranges (dark grey frames are from 164 to 242 ms, 402-434 ms, and 446-500 ms, respectively. T-maps are displayed on the right).
Reaction Time and Accuracy Rate in the Learning Phase and Recognition Test for Three Conditions among Three Grades.
Multiple Mechanisms Regulate Statistical Learning of Orthographic Regularities in School-Age Children: Neurophysiological Evidence

December 2022

·

84 Reads

·

7 Citations

Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience

How brains of children of different ages extract and encode relational patterns contained in orthographic input remains elusive. We investigated this question by measuring event-related potentials (ERPs) of 99 Grades 1-3 children while performing an artificial orthography statistical learning task that comprised logographic components embedded in characters with high (100%), moderate (80%), and low (60%) positional consistency. The behavioral results indicated that across grades, children more accurately recognized the characters with high than with low consistency. The neurophysiological results revealed that in each grade, the amplitude of some ERP components differed across the positional consistency, with a larger P1 effect in the high consistency condition and a larger N170 and left-lateralized P300 effect in the low consistency condition The grade effect was significant, i.e., the smaller N170 amplitude in Grade 3 than Grade 1 and the larger P300 amplitude in Grade 1 than either Grade 2 or 3. These findings suggest the dynamic nature of statistical learning by showing that at least two mechanisms, i.e., neural adaptation associated with N170, and attention and working memory related to P1 and P300 regulate different types of structural input, and that children’s abilities to prioritize these mechanisms vary with context and age.


Citations (1)


... Irregular characters, by definition, have pronunciations that cannot be directly derived from their phonetic radicals. This complexity may make them more susceptible to explicit instruction (Conway, 2020;Tong et al., 2023). Our intervention emphasized both regular and irregular radicals, potentially providing more benefit to the more challenging irregular characters. ...

Reference:

The effectiveness of phonological training and morphological training in Chinese children with reading difficulty
Multiple Mechanisms Regulate Statistical Learning of Orthographic Regularities in School-Age Children: Neurophysiological Evidence

Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience