Yannick Boddez’s research while affiliated with Ghent University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (91)


Theory-Based Intervention for Transforming Harmful Behavior: The Goal-Directed Predictive Processing Approach
  • Preprint

May 2025

·

13 Reads

·

Yannick Boddez

Human behavior can sometimes cause harm to the behaving individual, other organisms, or their environment. Psychological science plays a crucial role in understanding and addressing these harmful behaviors by providing direction to behavior change interventions. However, the field of psychology encompasses numerous theories, and translating these theories into effective practices can be challenging. In this paper, we introduce the Goal-Directed Predictive Processing (GDPP) framework—an integrative approach that combines key principles from predictive processing and goal-directed theories. The GDPP framework bridges recent theoretical advancements with practical application, offering a user-friendly model to assist practitioners across various domains. It emphasizes assessing inference chains that underlie harmful behaviors in risk situations, mapping out alternative inferential processes in relation to expected surprise, and designing targeted interventions to influence these processes. We illustrate the application of this framework in the context of harmful alcohol consumption and depressive behavior.


Over de rol van associaties in betekenisanalyses: Een kritische evaluatie en mogelijke alternatieven.

February 2025

·

14 Reads

Whereas traditional behavior therapy is grounded in learning principles, cognitive therapy often focuses on meaning. Within cognitive behavioral therapy, attempts were made to reconcile these two approaches by analyzing meaning in terms of learning principles, more specifically classical conditioning. In doing so, classical conditioning has often been conceptualized as the process of the formation of associations in semantic memory. We argue that this conceptualization is not only problematic but also unnecessary. We propose two alternative ways of reconciling learning principles with a focus on meaning: (1) by treating learning principles like classical conditioning as effects rather than mental processes and (2) by conceptualizing meaning analyses in terms of Relational Frame Theory. Research on classical conditioning remains an important source of inspiration also from these alternative perspectives. [Paper in Dutch]


Figure 4. Proportion of outcome expectancy errors in Experiment 3. These figures compare the mean proportion of outcome expectancy errors indicating the original response as correct for remapped stimuli (outcome expectancy action slips) with the mean proportion of outcome expectancy errors indicating an incorrect response as correct for non-remapped stimuli in all time intervals. The top figure displays the errors within timed bins, with error bars representing the standard error of the mean. The bottom figure features continuous lines illustrating the proportion of errors over time. Shading denotes the standard error of the mean. In both figures, the dashed line represents chance level performance of 50%.
Exploring the Role of Goal-Dependent Processes in Action Slips Under Time Pressure
  • Article
  • Full-text available

January 2024

·

120 Reads

·

6 Citations

·

Yannick Boddez

·

·

[...]

·

People sometimes emit frequently practiced responses that were previously effective in achieving desired outcomes but are no longer appropriate in the current context. While dual-process theories attribute these action slips to goal-independent, associative processes, we propose that errors in the expectancies about action outcomes contribute to their occurrence. To investigate this, we first replicated an influential study by Hardwick et al. (2019), demonstrating the occurrence of action slips following extensive stimulus–response training when individuals are required to respond rapidly. Building on this foundation, we conducted two additional experiments using a similar procedure, incorporating a measure of outcome expectancies under time pressure. Our findings provide compelling evidence for errors not just in selected responses but also in expected outcomes of these responses, particularly in early time intervals. These results highlight the possible role of goal-directed processes in action slips under time pressure, advancing our understanding of the cognitive mechanisms underlying suboptimal behavior.

Download

Relating different perspectives on how outcomes of behavior influence behavior

October 2023

·

289 Reads

·

3 Citations

Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior

Many researchers have tackled the question of how behavior is influenced by its outcomes. Some have adopted a non-mechanistic (functional) perspective that aims to describe the impact of outcomes on behavior. Others have adopted a mechanistic (cognitive) perspective that aims to explain the impact of outcomes on behavior. Orthogonal to this distinction, some have focused on the impact of outcomes that a behavior had in the past whereas others take into account also the impact of outcomes that a behavior might have in the future. In the current paper, we relate these different perspectives with the aim of reducing misunderstandings and fostering collaborations between researchers who adopt different perspectives on the common question of how behavior is influenced by its outcomes.


A Roadmap for Future Interactions Between Research on Personality and Learning

October 2023

·

182 Reads

·

1 Citation

In this introduction to the special collection of papers on the relation between learning and personality research, we provide a functional-cognitive framework that can guide interactions between learning and personality researchers. It highlights that learning researchers can treat personality variables as potential (first or second order) moderators of learning effects. They can also examine whether the effects of personality on learning itself depend on other moderators. Personality researchers can benefit from looking for stable individual differences in known learning effects. Together with learning researchers, they can analyze personality as learned and thus malleable behavior. We end by summarizing the papers of the special collection and by situating them within our framework.


Figure 1. The Summary Statistics and the Density of the CSs Evaluations for Each Condition Note. The violin plots present the summary statistics and the density of the evaluations for the CSs paired with ambivalent USs (the first plot), negative USs (the second plot), neutral USs (the third plot), and positive USs (the fourth plot). In each plot, there is a boxplot representation of the evaluative response distributions. The black point from each violin plot represents the mean of the evaluative responses for each condition, while the grey point represents the median of the evaluative responses for each condition.
Figure 2. The Main Effect of Neuroticism on the CSs Evaluations Note. Each slope becomes more abrupt as the level of neuroticism increases, reflecting the general negative ratings received by CSs, independent of the condition. The interaction effect between neuroticism and the evaluations of the CSs paired with ambivalent USs, relative to the neutral condition, is also visible (see the second and third lines).
Figure 3. The Summary Statistics and the Density of the CS Evaluations for Each Condition Note. The violin plots present the summary statistics and the density of the evaluations for the CSs from the ambiguous condition (the first plot), negative condition (the second plot), neutral condition (the third plot), and positive condition (the fourth plot). In each plot, there is a boxplot representation of the evaluative response distributions. The black point from each violin plot represents the mean of the evaluative responses for each condition, while the grey point represents the median of the evaluative responses for each condition.
Figure 4. The Simple Effect of Neuroticism Factor on the CS Evaluations Note. Figure 4 presents the slopes of CS evaluations for each condition. Each slope becomes more abrupt as the level of neuroticism increases (except the neutral one-first line), reflecting the main effect as generally more negative ratings received by CSs, independent of condition. The interaction effect between neuroticism and the ambiguous condition (relative to the neutral one) over the evaluations of the CSs is also visible (see the second and third lines).
The Moderating Role of Neuroticism on Evaluative Conditioning: Evidence From Ambiguous Learning Situations

August 2023

·

159 Reads

·

5 Citations

Social Psychological and Personality Science

Numerous studies have demonstrated a link between neuroticism and negative biases. Although some studies suggest that people with high neuroticism give more weight to negative information, others suggest that they respond more strongly to both positive and negative information. We investigated whether neuroticism is related to the evaluation of conditioned stimuli (CSs) in evaluative conditioning procedures that involve ambiguous learning conditions. We created ambiguous situations where CSs were paired with unconditioned stimuli (USs) consisting of both positive and negative pictures (Experiment 1) or paired alternatingly with positive and negative USs (Experiment 2). In addition to CSs consistently paired with positive and negative USs, we introduced neutral USs as a control condition. Our findings revealed that neurotic individuals negatively evaluated the CSs from ambiguous conditions relative to neutral conditions. In addition, participants with high neuroticism scores generally rated CSs more negatively. Theoretical and clinical implications of these results are discussed.


Reinstatement of thought conditioning following extinction and counterconditioning

April 2023

·

29 Reads

·

1 Citation

Behaviour Research and Therapy

Pairing a cue (e.g., an image of a geometrical figure) with an outcome (e.g., an image with aversive content) can result in the cue eliciting thoughts of that outcome (i.e., thought conditioning). Previous research suggests an advantage of counterconditioning over extinction in reducing thoughts of (aversive) outcomes. However, it is unclear how robust this effect is. This study aimed to (1) replicate the previously observed advantage of counterconditioning over extinction and (2) test whether counterconditioning leads to less reinstatement of thoughts of an aversive outcome relative to extinction. Participants (N = 118) underwent a differential conditioning procedure and were then assigned to one of three conditions: extinction (i.e., the aversive outcome was no longer presented), no extinction (i.e., the aversive outcome continued to be presented) and counterconditioning (i.e., the aversive outcome was replaced with positive images). After three unsignaled outcome presentations, participants indicated in a return of fear test the extent to which they thought of the aversive outcome. As predicted, counterconditioning was more successful in reducing thinking of the aversive outcome than extinction. Yet, there were no differences in return of thoughts of the aversive outcome between the two conditions. Future research should consider other return of fear procedures.


Fear Conditioning Biases in Anxiety Disorders: A Matter of Interpretation?

March 2023

·

63 Reads

Biases in fear conditioning and interpretation biases have both been linked to the onset, maintenance, and treatment of anxiety. This chapter starts with summarizing empirical evidence on the link between fear conditioning biases and pathological anxiety. We discuss how biased interpretational processing might mediate biased behavior in fear conditioning tasks. In particular, stimuli might become ambiguous as a result of fear conditioning procedures (e.g., generalization and extinction). A tendency to interpret these ambiguous stimuli as threatening might result in increased fear responding in anxiety-prone individuals. We make a distinction between biases as observable behaviors (e.g., in experimental tasks) and biases as underlying (latent) cognitive processes or deficits. A mediation model is proposed that explains how a combination of genetic factors and learning experiences can give rise to observable biases and how this association can be (partially) driven by biases as a latent cognitive process (including interpretation biases). Finally, we discuss the role of (fear conditioning) biases in interventions for anxiety. Remediating biases might enhance the efficacy of cognitive-behavioral treatment and exposure interventions. However, it is predicted that biased individuals might be less sensitive to corrective experiences (e.g., due to a stagnated error reduction process).KeywordsFear conditioningAnxietyInterpretation biasExposureExtinctionGeneralization


Let’s play pretend: Towards effective modelling in experimental psycho(patho)logy

March 2023

·

45 Reads

·

1 Citation

Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry

Background and objectives: Models are omnipresent in science. We introduce a novel framework to achieve more effective modelling practices in general psychology and experimental psychopathology. Method: We analyze three types of models that are common in psychology: laboratory models, computational models, and cognitive models. We then evaluate two common ways to assess the translational value of models (phenomenological similarity and deep similarity) as well as an arguably underappreciated way (functional similarity). Functional similarity is based on an assessment of whether variables (e.g., the administration of a pharmacological substance) have a similar effect (a) in the model (e.g., in a fear conditioning procedure) and (b) on the real-life target phenomenon (e.g., on real-life anxiety complaints). Conclusions: We argue that the assessment of functional similarity is a powerful tool to assess the translational value of models in the field of experimental psychopathology and beyond.


Figure 1. Flow chart. Note. PCL-5 = PTSD checklist for the DSM-5.
Figure 2. Acquisition and extinction learning curves. Note. a1-a8 represent acquisition trials, e1-e8 represent extinction trials.
Regression analyses predicting PTSD symptom severity at follow-up, with corresponding model information and beta weights.
Pearson correlations among psychometric variables.
Extinction learning as pretrauma vulnerability factor of posttraumatic stress: a replication study

December 2022

·

150 Reads

·

10 Citations

Background: Learning tasks have been used to predict why some, and not others, develop posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) after exposure to a traumatic event. There is some evidence from prospective studies in high risk profession samples that reduced extinction learning might represent a marker or even a vulnerability factor for PTSD development. Objective: Since the evidence is scarce, the aim of this study was to perform a conceptual replication of an earlier prospective study, testing whether pretrauma extinction learning predicts later PTSD symptom severity. Method: A sample of 529 fire fighters performed a conditioning task at baseline and filled out questionnaires to assess PTSD symptom severity and neuroticism. At six and 12 months follow-up, exposure to stressful events and PTSD symptom severity were measured. Results: Results indicate that previous findings were not replicated: although reduced extinction learning was associated with higher PTSD symptom severity at baseline, extinction learning did not predict PTSD symptom severity at follow-up. Only PTSD symptom severity at baseline and stressor severity predicted PTSD symptom severity at follow-up. Conclusions: Since earlier findings on the predictive value of pre-trauma extinction learning on PTSD symptom severity were not replicated, extinction learning might not be a general risk factor PTSD for all individuals. More prospective studies including multiple factors seem needed to unravel the complex relationships of these factors influencing PTSD development. Highlights: Reduced extinction learning correlated with higher PTSD symptom severity at baseline.Reduced extinction learning did not predict PTSD symptom severity at follow-up.The predictive effect of pre-trauma extinction learning on PTSD was not replicated.


Citations (77)


... This suggests that action slips could be driven by the persistence of rigid, outdated goal-directed knowledge (see also [50]). Recently, this goal-directed explanation received additional support from a study by Van Dessel et al. [51]. These observations, moreover, challenge one of the key methods used to detect habit processes. ...

Reference:

Critical Review of the Habit Theory in Substance Use Disorder and Application of Moors' Goal-Directed Theory
Exploring the Role of Goal-Dependent Processes in Action Slips Under Time Pressure

... To test whether GD or SR processes, or a combination of both, are involved in determining behavior, a classical approach consists in manipulating the outcome value of a response (e.g., by outcome devaluation 37 ) or the action-outcome contingency (e.g., by contingency degradation 37 ). Indeed, while GD behavior is sensitive to outcome values and actionoutcome contingencies, SR behavior is not affected by learning or environmental feedback 37,38 . It has been proposed that the controllability of expected action-outcomes governs the balance between SR and GD processes 39 . ...

Relating different perspectives on how outcomes of behavior influence behavior

Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior

... In selecting personality traits that could moderate EC and feature transformation more generally, we relied on the existing literature linking EC and impression formation to personality (see De Houwer et al., 2023). For instance, in a study of EC, Vogel et al. (2019) concluded that neuroticism and agreeableness were moderators of the EC effect. ...

A Roadmap for Future Interactions Between Research on Personality and Learning

... For instance, in a study of EC, Vogel et al. (2019) concluded that neuroticism and agreeableness were moderators of the EC effect. Subsequent studies provided more evidence for the correlation with agreeableness (Ingendahl & Vogel, 2023) and revealed a more complex picture with regard to the correlation with neuroticism (e.g., Bunghez et al., 2023;Casini et al., 2023). In the impression formation literature, agreeableness is one of the most evaluative personality traits within the Big Five taxonomy (Rau et al., 2021) that organises information about others and is considered a central dimension of interpersonal judgment (Ames & Bianchi, 2008). ...

The Moderating Role of Neuroticism on Evaluative Conditioning: Evidence From Ambiguous Learning Situations

Social Psychological and Personality Science

... In behavior analysis, the primary approach which advocates for this relational view is Relational Frame Theory (RFT; Hayes et al., 2001). RFT states that the ability to relate stimuli in arbitrary and complex ways can be considered a general operant response class, typically referred to as arbitrarily applicable relational responding (AARR;De Houwer et al., 2023;. This perspective is unique to RFT: most theoretical accounts from other fields tend to situate relational responding as a mental mechanism, rather than as an operant behavior (McLoughlin et al., 2020). ...

Thinking of Learning Phenomena as Instances of Relational Behavior

Learning & Behavior

... While some routine behaviour might be unintentional or habitualthat is, not guided by people's goals -many others certainly are (for a recent debate on this issue, see De Houwer et al., 2023;Wood et al., 2022). Specifically, from a goal-directed perspective, people's routine behaviours are guided by maintenance goals (Ecker & Gilead, 2018;Railton, 2017). ...

Reasons to Remain Critical About the Literature on Habits: A Commentary on Wood et al. (2022)

Perspectives on Psychological Science

... Learned helplessness is a key dimension of depression that has been confirmed by numerous studies and is associated with both negative emotions and ineffective response to stressors (Abramson et al., 2014;Liu et al., 2015;Pryce et al., 2011;Vollmayr & Gass, 2013). Additionally, learned helplessness is correlated to burnout both in general (Boddez et al., 2022;Burisch & Schaufeli, 2017) and especially in teachers, leading them to reduced involvement in teaching activities (Kumcagiz et al., 2014;Qutaiba, 2011). ...

Learned helplessness and its relevance for psychological suffering: a new perspective illustrated with attachment problems, burn-out, and fatigue complaints
  • Citing Article
  • September 2022

... Some researchers refer to S-R associations as habit [17]. However, we reserve the term 'habit' for the observable behavior only, to more clearly delineate the multiple cognitive processes that contribute to whether or not a habit is expressed [18]. Importantly, from a dual system perspective, as we describe in detail in this review, modifying S-R associations is not the only pathway to promote or curtail the expression of habits. ...

Reasons to remain critical about the literature on habits: A commentary on Wood et al. (2021)

... Recently, Smeets et al. [54] failed to replicate these findings in two studies. Similarly, Buabang et al. [55] were unable to conceptually replicate Schwabe and Wolf's [53] results, instead providing evidence for goal-directed control under stress. Other studies [56,57] have also not been able to observe differences between stress and control groups, although they have shown that stressed individuals show more insensitive behavior when moderating variables were invoked. ...

The Role of Goal-Directed and Habitual Processes in Food Consumption Under Stress After Outcome Devaluation With Taste Aversion

... We also share memories with other people in order to create and maintain relationships with them based on shared, relatable or sympathetic experiences (Alea & Bluck, 2003). The ability to retrieve and share such memories, and to do so in ways that are detailed and specific can play an important role in eliciting emotional and instrumental support from other people (Barry, Boddez, Chiu, & Raes, 2022;Barry, Vinograd, et al., 2019). ...

The Sharing of Autobiographical Memories Elicits Social Support