William F. Brewer’s research while affiliated with University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (23)


Discourse structure and mental models
  • Article

March 1985

·

35 Reads

·

22 Citations

J. Kathryn Bock

·

William F. Brewer

What children know about discourse and its conventions before they start to read may influence their acquisition of reading skill.


The Nature and Functions of Schemas. Technical Report No. 325

January 1984

·

18 Reads

·

8 Citations

Defining schemas as higher order cognitive structures that serve a crucial role in providing an account of how old knowledge interacts with new in perception, language, thought, and memory, this paper offers an analytic account of the nature and functions of schemas in psychological theory and organizes some of the experimental evidence dealing with the operation of schemas in memory. The paper is organized into six sections, the first of which provides a detailed examination of the schema concept as formulated by F. C. Bartlett in 1932. The second section relates Bartlett's theory to the larger issue of the conflict in psychological theory between ideas from British Empiricism and ideas from Continental philosophy. The third section briefly outlines some of the basic theoretical assumptions of information processing psychology to serve as a background for an analysis of schema theory, and the fourth examines modern schema theory, and contrasts it with Bartlett's theory and with the information processing approach. The fifth section discusses the nature of schemas, specifically mentioning ontological assumptions, modularity, ecological validity, and phenomenal experience, while the final section develops a framework for analyzing the functions of schemas in the memory process, and then examines a number of recent experiments in terms of this framework. A 16-page bibliography is included. (FL)



The Structure of Human Memory

December 1983

·

255 Reads

·

54 Citations

Psychology of Learning and Motivation

This chapter presents an analysis of the structure of human memory. It focuses on the process of recall of information from long-term memory. Common forms of memory such as personal memory, semantic memory, and rote linguistic skills are described in the chapter. A skill is the ability to carry out a practiced motor performance or cognitive operation. There is typically no experience of mental imagery when skilled actions are carried out. Three important types of skills are motor skills, cognitive skills, and rote linguistic skills. Data from a phenomenal experience is given equal status with the other forms of data typically gathered in experiments on human memory. In working out the analysis of the structure of memory, a constant tension between a view of memory as the reliving of earlier perceptions and a view of memory as a schema-based reconstructive process is felt. Many of the classic theories of human memory have achieved simplicity by ignoring the actual complexity of the phenomena and by attempting to give a simple image account, a simple interference account, or a simple propositional account.



Use of plan schemata in the recall and recognition of goal-directed actions
  • Article
  • Publisher preview available

January 1983

·

76 Reads

·

112 Citations

Compared videotaped actions that were embedded in a plan schema with actions not embedded in a plan schema (e.g., opening a drawer vs opening a drawer in order to take out a stapler). 102 college students were tested for their memory of actions seen on tape; the actions were embedded or not in a schema. Exp I showed that actions in schema were recalled better than were the actions not in a schema, but high recognition scores were found for both types in a visual recognition task. Findings are interpreted as showing that plan schemata are used in the retrieval process. Exp II showed that after 48 hrs, actions seen embedded in a schema were recognized better than were items not in a schema. This finding is interpreted as resulting from the plan schema either (a) serving to preserve the representation or (b) enabling Ss to recall overall goal and give high recognition responses to subordinate actions in an in-order-to relation with the remembered goal. Exp III studied recognition memory of new subordinate actions embedded in old schemata. Ss made false recognition responses, suggesting that plan schemata were not serving to preserve surface information. Data on loss of memory over time suggest that information about actions is lost from the hierarchical plan structure from the bottom up, leaving successively more abstract information about plans and goals in memory. (16 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

View access options

Stories Are to Entertain: A Structural-Affect Theory of Stories

December 1982

·

273 Reads

·

382 Citations

Journal of Pragmatics

This paper proposes that theory construction in the area of stories must distinguish between theories of plan comprehension, theories of narrative comprehension, and theories of the story schema. Evidence is provided which suggests that a number of theoretical and empirical findings that have been taken to contribute to theories of the story schema are better interpreted as relating to plan comprehension and narrative comprehension. The paper suggests that theory evaluation in this area must take into account the discourse force of the genre being investigated. The authors propose that stories are a subclass of narratives that have entertainment as their primary discourse force. Finally, a structural-affect theory is outlined and evidence is given to suggest that this theory gives a partial account of the reader's story schema.


Sequence and Principles in Article System Use: An Examination of "A, The", and "Null" Acquisition

October 1982

·

17 Reads

·

32 Citations

A new classification of English article (a, the, null) usage was developed. On the basis of this classification scheme, data on article usage were obtained from both adults and children, including children in the initial period of article acquisition (2-3 years). In the results, an early acquisition sequence was found of no article use, a use only, essentially correct a and the patterns of use, and overextended the use. This overuse of the definite article, found only for the more mature children in this study, has also been reported in previous studies on the articles. However, with the more complete usage data and the younger age range used in this study, the overuse was shown to be selective, occurring in 1 usage category and after a period of essentially correct use. From these findings it is argued that an explanation based on egocentrism is not adequate. It is suggested here that the incorrect usage of the more advanced children results from an overextension of a principle of shared knowledge found in adult article use.


Role of schemata in memory for places

April 1981

·

1,689 Reads

·

803 Citations

Cognitive Psychology

A study of memory for places was carried out to examine five hypotheses about the use of schemata in memory performance: (a) that schemata determine what objects are encoded into memory; (b) that schemata act as frameworks for episodic information; (c) that schema-based information is integrated with episodic information; (d) that schemata facilitate retrieval; and (e) that schemata influence what is communicated at recall. Subjects were taken into what they thought was a graduate student's office and later were tested for memory of the room with either drawing recall, written recall, or verbal recognition. Memory scores for objects were correlated with schema expectancy and saliency ratings. Schema expectancy was positively correlated with recall and recognition. Expected objects were inferred in recall, supporting the integration hypothesis. Comparison of recall and recognition data supported the retrieval hypothesis. Analysis of the written descriptions supported the communication hypothesis. Saliency was positively correlated with recall and recognition for present objects, but was unrelated to retrieval. Saliency was negatively correlated with recognition for nonpresent objects, suggesting a metacognitive strategy in recognition of high-salient objects.


Memory for goal-directed events

July 1980

·

46 Reads

·

313 Citations

Cognitive Psychology

A series of experiments on comprehension and memory of common goal-directed events was carried out. Empirically based Plan schemata for two videotaped events were derived by asking subjects which actions were done “in order to” do other actions. An experiment on memory for videotaped events found a hierarchical pattern of recall and that the recall of goal-directed actions was superior to that of non-goal-directed actions, supporting the hypothesis that Plan schemata are used in the recall of events. An additional experiment showed that the temporal order of actions in observed events is reconstructed from the underlying Plan schema. A parallel series of recall experiments was carried out using prose descriptions of the videotaped events. Results replicated the earlier studies, suggesting that the recall of narrative prose is largely determined by the Plan schema of the underlying events.


Citations (23)


... The term false memory subsumes various phenomena in memory psychology relating to erroneously remembering an event or detail that the person believes to be true, but that did not actually happen or occurred differently than remembered (Bernstein et al., 2018). One such memory error is remembering events that were implied or could be inferred from a sentence, but were not explicitly stated (Brewer, 1977). For example, after reading the sentence "The karate champion hit the cinder block" participants may recall that the karate champion broke the cinder block, even though the original sentence did not mention whether the block actually broke. ...

Reference:

A Statistical Experimentum Crucis: Competing-Contrast Analysis with the R Package cofad
Memory for pragmatic implications of sentences
  • Citing Article
  • November 1977

Memory & Cognition

... Both models differ concerning how the negation tag and the core supposition are processed. In the fusion model, they are spontaneously fused (e.g., Brewer and Lichtenstein, 1974;Horn, 1989;MacDonald and Just, 1989;Gannon and Ostrom, 1996;Lea and Mulligan, 2002;Mayo et al., 2004), and in the schema-plus-tag model, they are separately processed (e.g., Fiedler et al., 1996;Rubaltelli and Slovic, 2008). Put differently, the dual-step model assumes that a negation such as The window is not open is processed by the negation-incongruent schema and an open window by the core supposition. ...

Memory for marked semantic features versus memory for meaning
  • Citing Article
  • April 1974

Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior

... For example, the number of segments or units identified at perception correlates with the information verbally recalled (Hanson & Hirst, 1989;Newtson & Engquist, 1976;Zacks, 2020). These units are organised around the agent's goals and changes in spatiotemporal properties such as travel paths and locations (Lichtenstein & Brewer, 1980;Zacks et al., 2009). ...

Memory for goal-directed events
  • Citing Article
  • July 1980

Cognitive Psychology

... Moreover, it is important to emphasize that not every study dealing with the acquisition of articles ensures that an alleged new referent is indeed hearer-new. For example, the elicitation of articles in the sentence completion tasks performed by Zehler and Brewer (1982) as well as van Hout et al. (2010) did not provide a situation in which children had to assume that the experimenter as a hearer is uninformed about the answertherefore, there is no real discrepancy between the knowledge states of the participant and the hearer. There is also no necessity to mark a referent as hearer-new if the relevant referent is present in the shared visual context, as in Schaeffer and Matthewson's (2005) study. ...

Sequence and Principles in Article System Use: An Examination of "A, The", and "Null" Acquisition
  • Citing Article
  • October 1982

... When categorizing consumption-related information, they might develop a network of nodes in memory, storing information in hierarchical order ( Brewer and Nakamura 1984 ;Halkias 2015 ;Meyers-Levy and Tybout 1989 ), such that attributes related to finer information (e.g., operating hours) get stored at a subordinate level ( Meyers-Levy and Tybout 1989 ). Broader information, such as overall quality judgments, instead is installed at a superordinate or higher level ( Meyers-Levy and Tybout 1989 ). According to schema theory, customers use category-driven, top-down processes to evaluate experiences quickly and with little cognitive effort ( Alba and Hutchinson 1987 ;Fiske and Pavelchak 1986 ;Meyers-Levy and Tybout 1989 ), but they engage in slower, effortful, attribute-by-attribute, bottom-up information processing if existing category knowledge is inadequate to understand the information provided by their current experiences ( Fiske and Pavelchak 1986 ;Sujan and Bettman 1989 ). ...

The Nature and Functions of Schemas. Technical Report No. 325
  • Citing Article
  • January 1984

... Using a free recall method, Stafford and Daly (1984) reported that subjects only remembered an average of 10% of what was said in a conversational exchange five minutes after. It is also thought that memory representations of discourse more strongly reflect the overall meaning or "gist" of the discourse rather than verbatim words and phrases (Bock & Brewer, 1974;Sachs, 1967). However, our findings from the present experiments did not support our hypothesis. ...

Reconstructive recall in sentences with alternative surface-structures

Journal of Experimental Psychology

... In contrast to what we found for recall, we found no evidence that orienting attention to segmentation with the overt task improved recognition memory, and there was very weak evidence that segmentation agreement predicted recognition memory performance. 12 SMITH, HALL, MEMBRENO, QUINTERO, AND ZACKS One possibility is that recognition tests are less sensitive than recall tests to the event models constructed during movie watching (Brewer & Dupree, 1983). We elaborate further on this possibility in the General Discussion section. ...

Use of plan schemata in the recall and recognition of goal-directed actions

... It has been discovered that cognitive engagement of the reader will increase if he or she must predict or anticipate what will happen next (Kintsch, 1980; Schank, 1979). Creating suspense (Rabkin, 1973) or introducing surprise (Brewer, 1983; Jose and Brewer, 1990) can also heighten interest. The presence of mystery-can turn the reading process in to a problem-solving task where the reader can match the unfolding events against his or her expectations (Black and Bowler, 1980). ...

Form, content, and affect in the theory of stories
  • Citing Article
  • December 1983

Behavioral and Brain Sciences

... If young children between 4 and 7 are sensitive to discourse factors in their choice of referring expression, but still tend to overuse either pronouns or lexical NPs, the question is what their discourse models look like. One possibility is that young children's discourse models are impoverished versions of adults' discourse models (e.g., Bock & Brewer, 1985). For taking into account local discourse factors such as givenness, recency and topicality, working memory needs to be continuously updated to reflect the current status of the discourse referents (Whitely & Colozzo, 2013). ...

Discourse structure and mental models
  • Citing Article
  • March 1985

... Our overall findings are in line with previous literature which demonstrated a close relationship between global coherence and memory measures (Drummond et al., 2015;Kim et al., 2019). In addition, the close link between deictic elements of a language (which contain limited meaning in sentences) and memory seemed to also help the formation of utterances to a particular time, place, speaker, or discourse context (Brewer and Harris, 1974). The current study in a Chinese sample adds to a growing literature of speakers of different languages (Fleming and Harris, 2008;March et al., 2009;Kim et al., 2019), including Western studies showing significant associations between working memory and discourse measures (Almor et al., 1999;Youse and Coelho, 2005;Cahana-Amitay and Jenkins, 2018). ...

Memory for deictic elements in sentences
  • Citing Article
  • June 1974

Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior