Timothy C. Brock’s research while affiliated with The Ohio State University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (68)


Test of Schema Correspondence Theory of Persuasion: Effects of Matching an Appeal to Actual, Ideal, and Product “Selves”
  • Chapter

October 2019

·

38 Reads

·

16 Citations

·

Timothy C. Brock

Delayed Recall of Copytest Responses: The Temporal Stability of Listed Thoughts
  • Article
  • Full-text available

May 2013

·

66 Reads

·

13 Citations

Data from a large-scale advertising contesting study were analyzed to assess the overall level of recall of listed thoughts at a delay, and to determine the types of listed thoughts that were particularly retrievable. Female heads-of-household viewed a series of television commercials and listed thoughts about each. One week later, the respondents, who were recontacted by telephone, were able to recall the content of half of their listed thoughts. More importantly, comparisons of the recall rates for listed-thought categories supported theory-based predictions about the responses that should be particularly retrievable. Thoughts that contained evaluative content mere significantly more likely to be recalled than neutral thoughts, and unfavorable thoughts were slightly more likely to be recalled than favorable thoughts. Also, thoughts about the self were more likely to be recalled than thoughts that did not contain self-relevant content. This greater retrievability of self-relevant thoughts was true for commercials with standard executorial styles. However, for ads with a highly unusual presentation or spokesperson, thoughts about the ad execution appeared to be the most retrievable.

Download

Antecedents and Civic Consequences of Choosing Real versus Ersatz Social Activities

October 2008

·

57 Reads

·

16 Citations

Media Psychology

Social capital (Coleman, 19908. Coleman , J. S. 1990 . Foundations of social theory , Cambridge, MA : Harvard University Press . View all references), democracy's wellspring (Putnam, 199336. Putnam , R. D. 1993 . Making democracy work: Civic traditions in modern Italy , Princeton, NJ : Princeton University Press . View all references; Tocqueville, 183562. Tocqueville , A. 1835 . Democracy in America , Edited by: Mansfield , H. C. and Winthrop , D. Chicago : The University of Chicago Press . View all references/2000), must now accrue within a social fabric that has been changed by the ubiquitous adoption of technologies such as television and the Internet. Three studies (N = 313) of college students supported a theory of ersatz social behavior, which proposed that: a) the use of technological (replacement/ersatz) alternatives to real social interaction may be less conducive to the skill building and opinion formation that foster social capital; b) ersatz activities involve fewer costs (effort, risk of rejection) than real interactions; and c) ersatz alternatives are regularly chosen, even though real interaction is ostensibly preferred. The results provide increased understanding of when and how mediated interaction can be detrimental to social capital formation.


Negligible Scholarly Impact of 38-Witnesses Parable

September 2008

·

43 Reads

·

4 Citations

American Psychologist

Comments on an article by Manning, Levine and Collins, which talks about the original 1964 Kitty Genovese murder 38-witness account. Brock feels there were inaccuracies that have not affected the story's scholarly impact, or lack of impact. First, the story did not launch the social psychology of helping. Second, the story itself in no way "curtailed the imaginative space of helping research in social psychology," as claimed by Manning et al. Third, Manning et al. noted that the social psychology of helping was not included in a recent litany of useful contributions of psychology. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2008 APA, all rights reserved).


Biases in research evaluation: Inflated assessment, oversight, or error-type weighting?

July 2007

·

111 Reads

·

14 Citations

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

Reviewers of research are more lenient when evaluating studies on important topics [Wilson, T. D., Depaulo, B. M., Mook, D. G., & Klaaren, K. J. (1993). Scientists’ evaluations of research: the biasing effects of the importance of the topic. Psychological Science, 4(5), 323–325]. Three experiments (N=145, 36, and 91 psychologists) investigated different explanations of leniency, including inflation of assessments (applying a heuristic associating importance with quality), oversight (failing to detect flaws), and error-weighting (prioritizing Type II error avoidance). In Experiment 1, psychologists evaluated the publishability and rigor of studies in a 2 (topic importance)×2 (accuracy motivation)×2 (research domain) design. Experiment 2 featured an exact replication of Wilson et al. and suggested that report length moderated the effects of importance on perceived rigor, but not on publishability. In Experiment 3, a manipulation of error-weighting replaced the manipulation of domain (Experiment 1). Results favored error-weighting, rather than inflation or oversight. Perceived seriousness of Type II error (in Experiments 1 and 3) and the error-weighting manipulation (in Experiment 3) predicted study evaluations.


The Effects of a Prior Story-Bank on the Processing of a Related Narrative

July 2007

·

47 Reads

·

11 Citations

Media Psychology

Two studies tested whether the possession of a mental story-bank (a set of thematically related narratives) affected the processing of a related narrative. Three competing predictions were proposed: a story-bank may lead to reduced attention, increased attention, or selective attention to a new, related story. The results of Study 1 (N = 125 undergraduates) suggested that a prior story-bank led to more efficient processing of a target narrative (as indicated by recall data), favoring a selective attention interpretation. Study 2 (N = 114 undergraduates) showed that individuals who possessed a relevant story-bank were more persuaded by a related target narrative, also consistent with the selective attention interpretation. Story-banks thus appeared to facilitate, rather than hinder, processing of new narratives.


Fact Versus Fiction Labeling: Persuasion Parity Despite Heightened Scrutiny of Fact

August 2006

·

277 Reads

·

66 Citations

Media Psychology

Two studies investigated the effect of fact or fiction labeling on the processing of advocacy communication. Labeling a communication as fact, rather than fiction, appeared to enhance critical processing (scrutiny). In 2 experiments, 392 students, who were low or high in need for cognition (NC) read a speech (nonnarrative). This discrepant speech, reported to be an actual event or a dramatic creation, enabled variation of label (fact/fiction), argument quality (strong/weak), and personal-outcome relevance (unspecified, Experiment 1; enhanced/reduced, Experiment 2). When personal relevance was unspecified (or reduced), speeches with a fact label instigated scrutiny for low-NC individuals; high-NC individuals engaged in scrutiny regardless of fact/fiction label. Under enhanced relevance, scrutiny was observed regardless of fact/fiction label and NC level. Across the experiments, communications labeled as fact were no more persuasive than those labeled as fiction.


Erotic Materials: A Commodity Theory Analysis of the Enhanced Desirability that May Accompany Their Unavailability

July 2006

·

338 Reads

·

18 Citations

Journal of Applied Social Psychology

This report analyzes one dimension of the pornography-control issue, i.e., the probable reactions of individuals who are confronted with information that is not freely or easily available to all. After a review of commodity theory and related research, it is concluded that making erotic materials more difficult to obtain, harassing and punishing pornographers and purveyors of pornography, and restricting certain materials to certain age groups may increase interest in the materials and render them more desirable than would have been the case without the restriction, harassment, or difficulty.


Effects of Responding or Not Responding to Hecklers on Audience Agreement with a Speaker

July 2006

·

48 Reads

·

9 Citations

Journal of Applied Social Psychology

The hypothesis that responding to hecklers would produce more agreement with a speaker than not responding, stemmed from commodity theory (Brock, 1968). One hundred twenty-one introductory speech students participated in what they were told was a “speech workshop” (not a psychology experiment). Two types of responding to live hecklers were used: In one, the speaker responded in a calm, relevant manner; in the other, she responded in an upset, irrelevant manner. In a third condition, the speaker did not respond to the heckles. There were two additional conditions: One in which the speaker responded to interruptions, and a further control in which there were neither heckles nor interruptions. In these five conditions, the speaker either argued for or against the audience's position. Regardless of whether or not the speaker's position agreed with the audience's, upset-irrelevant responding decreased the speaker's persuasiveness over making no response, while calm-relevant responding tended to enhance persuasiveness. Finally, in agreement with all other empirical studies, it was clearly shown that heckling, whether responded to or not, did not improve the speaker's effectiveness.


The Cost of Being Black: White Americans’ Perceptions and the Question of Reparations

May 2006

·

340 Reads

·

20 Citations

Du Bois Review Social Science Research on Race

Philip J. Mazzocco

·

Timothy C Brock

·

·

[...]

·

White Americans have long resisted the idea of reparations to the descendants of slaves. We examine the psychological basis of such resistance, primarily testing the possibility that resistance may be a function of Whites’ perception of the ongoing cost of being Black. White participants (n = 958) across twelve independent samples (varying in age, student status, and geographic location) were asked variations of the question: How much should you be paid to continue to live the remainder of your life as a Black person? Participants generally required low median amounts, less than 10,000,tomaketheracechange,whereastheyrequestedhighamounts,10,000, to make the race change, whereas they requested high amounts, 1,000,000, to give up television. To the extent that larger amounts were requested, support for reparations also increased. Attempts to educate participants about Black cost0White privilege had negligible effects on assessments of the cost of being Black and support for reparations. Together, these results suggest that White resistance to reparations for Black Americans stems from fundamental biases in estimating the true cost of being Black. The implications of our findings for color-blind and multi-culturalist conceptual approaches are discussed. Du Bois Review: Social Science Research on Race, Vol. 3(2), 2006, pp. 261-297


Citations (56)


... In the present study, we investigated whether nudging could lead adults to create strong passwords and the factors related to strong password practices. We modeled our approach of using nudging messages on prior research supporting schema correspondence theory (Brannon & Brock, 2006), which claims that messages that are consistent with one's self-schema (i.e., how one thinks of oneself) can indirectly influence (or nudge) subsequent behaviors (Brannon & Brock, 1994;Brannon & McCabe, 2002, 2003Brock et al., 1990;Miller & Brannon, 2015;Pease et al., 2006;Pilling & Brannon, 2007;York et al., 2012a;See also;. For example, Brannon and McCabe (2002) showed that there was greater impact of information contained in an AIDS prevention message when the message was matched to participants' self-schema. ...

Reference:

Who creates strong passwords when nudging fails
Test of Schema Correspondence Theory of Persuasion: Effects of Matching an Appeal to Actual, Ideal, and Product “Selves”
  • Citing Chapter
  • October 2019

... To test a conceptual framework for interpersonal physical pleasuring, Davis, Rainey, and Brock (1976) conducted four experiments that examined the effects of sex combinations, recipient attributes, and anticipated future interaction. Using what was essentially a modified aggression machine, individuals were asked to administer waves of pleasure to a confederate's buttocks in the context of a learning experiment. ...

Interpersonal physical pleasuring: Effects of sex combinations, recipient attributes, and anticipated future interaction.
  • Citing Article
  • January 1976

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

... Brock and colleagues (Green, Wheeler, Hermann, & Brock, 1998) described the taxonomy of technology used in my article (skill, mechanized, automated) as rudimentary and presumably of no use for advancing knowledge in the social sciences. I suggest that Brock and colleagues should read more about this topic. ...

Social psychology and changing technologies: Reality versus caricature.
  • Citing Article
  • September 1998

American Psychologist

... Such concerns are not unique to the Internet, but are exceptionally salient given its structure. For example, many commentators (e.g., Brock, Green, & Reich, 1998;Brock, Green, Reich, & Evans, 1996;Hunt, 1996;Jaeobson & Christensen, 1996;Mintz, Drake, & Crits-Christoph, 1996) raised serious questions regarding the Consumer Reports analysis of patients' perceptions of the effectiveness of psychotherapy and Seligman's (1995) interpretation of the results. Central to many of the criticisms were questions regarding the representativeness of the sampling frame and the potential for self-selection bias. ...

New evidence of flaws in the Consumer Reports study of psychotherapy.
  • Citing Article
  • January 1998

American Psychologist

... However, the persuasive process aims to alter individuals' cognitive perspectives on specific events, individuals, or situations through ongoing communication, rather than mere fluency in language expression. Research on persuasion mechanisms suggests that achieving persuasion requires understanding the cognitive states of the persuadees and employing appropriate cognitive strategies to effectively change their viewpoints in a readily acceptable manner [23][24][25][26]. Therefore, designing persuasive dialogue agents necessitates drawing from cognitive CogAgent psychology and incorporating cognitive strategies to logically organize the content, logic, and language style of persuasive responses [13,27,28]. ...

Cognitive responses in persuasion: Historical foundations of the cognitive response approach to attitudes and persuasion
  • Citing Article

... Typically, the use of the rightmost digit in the retail advertisement of prices (e.g., $899, $459.95, $540, ₹199, and ₹99) is a very ubiquitous practice to highlight prices fall just-below the whole figure amounts (Clark et al., 1994). Retailers ubiquitously use digit 9, 5, and 0 as the rightmost digit in decreasing order when advertising their products' or services' prices, i.e., the frequency of the digit 9 is the highest as the rightmost digit in price-endings, 5 is the second-highest and the digit 0 has less frequency at price-endings in western economies (Schindler and Kirby, 1997;Nguyen et al., 2007). ...

Attention, Attitude, and Affect in Response to Advertising
  • Citing Article
  • August 1994

Journal of Marketing Research

... Trust in the source is key in forming positive or negative perceptions (Ryu et al., 2018). The observable and inferred characteristics of the source, such as competence and intentionality, are crucial for the message's effectiveness (Milburn, 1991;Perloff, 1993;Twyman et al., 2008). In this context, experts, especially scientists, are perceived as credible and trustworthy sources (Cologna and Siegrist, 2020), making them particularly persuasive (Meyer, 1988). ...

Persuasion and Politics: The Social Psychology of Public Opinion
  • Citing Article
  • September 1995

Political Psychology

... As an index of the valence of message-related thoughts, we subtracted the number of unfavourable thoughts from the number of favourable thoughts and divided the difference by the total number of message-related thoughts. Most of the attitudes research conducted in the last three decades has used and agreed upon this index of thought favourability (e.g., Briñol, Petty and Tormala 2004;Cacioppo and Petty 1981;Shavitt and Brock 1990;Wright 1973). ...

Delayed Recall of Copytest Responses: The Temporal Stability of Listed Thoughts

...  Awareness; Creating awareness of the product/service,  Understanding; Ensuring that the value of the product/service to the consumer is understood,  Persuasion; Providing rational justifications for the purchase of a product/service,  Action; Enabling the consumer to realize the purchase Emphasizing the importance of selling as in other models, the DAGMAR model states that cognitive factors should also be taken into account. In addition, it also refers to the importance of persuasion, creating awareness and the importance of emotional factors of the consumers who make up the target market group (Clark, Brock and Steawart, 1994). ...

Attention, attitude, and affect in response to advertising
  • Citing Book
  • January 1994

... One such technique is referred to as the "Tuner-transmitter" technique and has been demonstrated to significantly improve recall in listeners. According to this theory, if listeners believe they will need to pass along the information to others at a later time, they will listen more actively, comprehend and recall more of a message, and attitude change will persist over time (Boninger et al., 1993(Boninger et al., , 1990. Packard and Berger (2021) proposed: "Encouraging employees to speak concretely could also help improve actual employee listening. ...

Effects of Transmitter Tuning on Attitude Change Persistence: An Examination of Alternative Explanations
  • Citing Article
  • May 1993

Psychological Science