Thibaud Gruber’s research while affiliated with University of Geneva and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (102)


Old age variably impacts chimpanzee engagement and efficiency in stone tool use
  • Preprint

June 2025

·

12 Reads

·

·

·

[...]

·

Dora Biro

We know vanishingly little about how long-lived apes experience senescence in the wild, particularly with respect to their foraging behaviors, which are essential for survival. Some wild apes use tools during foraging and, given the additional cognitive and physical challenges presented by tool use, we predict that such behaviors are at a heightened risk of senescence. However, until the present, longitudinal analysis of the effects of progressive aging on wild ape tool-use behaviors has not been possible due to a lack of available data. In response to this research gap, we sampled data from a longitudinal video archive that contained footage of wild chimpanzees engaging in one of their most complex forms of tool use - the cracking of hard-shelled nuts with hammers and anvil stones, termed nut cracking - at an ‘outdoor laboratory’ at Bossou, Guinea. By sampling data over a 17-year period, we describe how the extent to which wild chimpanzees engage in – and efficiently perform – nut cracking changes between the ages of approximately 39-44 to 56-61 years of age. Over this extended sampling period, chimpanzees began attending experimental nut cracking sites less frequently than younger individuals. Several elderly chimpanzees exhibited reductions in efficiency across multiple components of nut cracking, including taking more time to select stone tools prior to use, and taking longer to crack open nuts and consume the associated pieces of kernel. Two chimpanzees also began using less streamlined behavioral sequences to crack nuts, including a greater number of actions (such as more numerous strikes of the hammer stone). Most notably, we report interindividual variability in the extent to which elderly chimpanzees’ tool-use behaviors changed during our sample period – ranging from small to profound reductions in tool engagement and efficiency – as well as differences in the specific aspects of nut cracking behaviors that changed for each individual as they aged. We discuss the possible causes of these changes with reference to research into senescence in captive primates, and provide future directions for research of primate aging in both captive and wild settings.


Representative waveforms of 750-ms-long angry/threatening vocalizations expressed by human (in blue), chimpanzee (in green), bonobo (in orange), and rhesus macaque (in pink) species. These graphical representations were extracted using the PhonTools package (Barreda, 2015) in Rstudio (Rstudio Team, 2020).
Probe locations into the MNI space by using SPM12 software implemented in MatLab R2018b (www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). Red and blue dots indicate transmitters and receivers’ positions, respectively. Yellow dots indicate the channel numbers.
Structure of the experiment, with each of the 6 blocks made of 12 mini-blocks, which in turn comprised 12 individual trials.
Mean and SE of human recognition of human (in blue), chimpanzee (in green), bonobo (in orange), and rhesus macaque (in pink) affective vocalizations for categorization (CAT) and discrimination (DIS) tasks and the different kinds of affective states. All contrasts were significant within each condition after Bonferroni correction with Pcorrected = .05/24 = .002, excluding the following contrasts: chimpanzee vs rhesus macaque and bonobo vs rhesus macaque for affiliative cues and bonobo vs rhesus macaque for threatening contents in discrimination task (see Supplementary Material Table 2).
Mean and SE of concentration changes of O2Hb (µM) in right and left FTC/MFC and IFCtri during the categorization and the discrimination tasks by human participants of primate affective vocalizations. N = 20. ***p < .001, *p < .05.

+1

Categorization and discrimination of human and non-human primate affective vocalizations: Investigation of frontal cortex activity through fNIRS
  • Article
  • Full-text available

February 2025

·

46 Reads

Previous research has highlighted the involvement of frontal regions in human participants while they engaged in the explicit decoding, such as categorization (A vs B) and discrimination (A vs non-A), of affective signals. Given its adaptive value and deep evolutionary history, this human capacity to recognize the affective content in human calls is likely to extend to the vocalizations of other closely related species, such as non-human primates. However, few comparative studies have thus far investigated this process at both the behavioral and neural levels. Here, we aimed to study the role of frontal regions in human participants while they engaged in the explicit affective content decoding of primate calls using functional Near Infrared Spectroscopy (fNIRS). Specifically, we recorded frontal regions of participants while they categorized or discriminated positive and negatively valenced vocal signals produced by four different primates: humans, chimpanzee and bonobo (both great apes species), and rhesus macaques (a more distant species). We also analyzed whether behavioral responses correlated with recorded frontal activations. fNIRS data revealed more activations within the inferior frontal cortex pars triangularis (IFCtri), the frontopolar (FPC), and middle frontal cortices (MFC) during discrimination compared with categorization. Activity in these regions was modulated by both the species and the type of task, with greater activity during the discrimination of agonistic chimpanzee calls compared with categorization. Categorization was itself characterized by a decrease of frontal activity during the correct recognition of all chimpanzee calls, and of affiliative rhesus macaque and agonistic bonobo vocalizations. Our results also highlighted behavioral differences related to the type of task. Participants discriminated almost all affective cues of all four species vocalizations above chance level. In comparison, they correctly categorized the affective content of most human and great ape vocalizations above chance level, but not those of rhesus macaque calls, highlighting an effect of both phylogenetic relatedness and the type of task. Overall, these findings support the hypothesis of an evolutionary ancient affective recognition processing system situated in the frontal cortex, inherited from our last common ancestor with other great apes.

Download

Old age variably impacts chimpanzee engagement and efficiency in stone tool use

February 2025

·

10 Reads

We know vanishingly little about how long-lived apes experience senescence in the wild, particularly with respect to their foraging behaviors, which are essential for survival. Some wild apes use tools during foraging and, given the additional cognitive and physical challenges presented by tool use, we predict that such behaviors are at a heightened risk of senescence. However, until the present, longitudinal analysis of the effects of progressive aging on wild ape tool-use behaviors has not been possible due to a lack of available data. In response to this research gap, we sampled data from a longitudinal video archive that contained footage of wild chimpanzees engaging in one of their most complex forms of tool use - the cracking of hard-shelled nuts with hammers and anvil stones, termed nut cracking - at an ‘outdoor laboratory’ at Bossou, Guinea. By sampling data over a 17-year period, we describe how progressive old age influences the extent to which wild chimpanzees engage with – and efficiently perform – nut cracking. With increasingly old age, chimpanzees began attending experimental nut cracking sites less frequently than younger individuals. Several elderly chimpanzees exhibited reductions in efficiency across multiple components of nut cracking, including taking more time to select stone tools prior to use, and taking longer to crack open nuts and consume the associated pieces of kernel. Two chimpanzees also began using less streamlined behavioral sequences to crack nuts, including a greater number of actions (such as more numerous strikes of the hammer stone). Most notably, we report interindividual variability in the extent to which tool-use behaviors senesced during aging – ranging from small to profound reductions in tool engagement and efficiency – as well as differences in the specific aspects of nut cracking behaviors which senesced for each individual. We discuss the possible causes of these changes with reference to research into senescence in captive primates, and provide future directions for research of primate aging in both captive and wild settings.


Old age variably impacts chimpanzee engagement and efficiency in stone tool use

February 2025

·

12 Reads

We know vanishingly little about how long-lived apes experience senescence in the wild, particularly with respect to their foraging behaviors, which are essential for survival. Some wild apes use tools during foraging and, given the additional cognitive and physical challenges presented by tool use, we predict that such behaviors are at a heightened risk of senescence. However, until the present, longitudinal analysis of the effects of progressive aging on wild ape tool-use behaviors has not been possible due to a lack of available data. In response to this research gap, we sampled data from a longitudinal video archive that contained footage of wild chimpanzees engaging in one of their most complex forms of tool use - the cracking of hard-shelled nuts with hammers and anvil stones, termed nut cracking - at an ‘outdoor laboratory’ at Bossou, Guinea. By sampling data over a 17-year period, we describe how progressive old age influences the extent to which wild chimpanzees engage with – and efficiently perform – nut cracking. With increasingly old age, chimpanzees began attending experimental nut cracking sites less frequently than younger individuals. Several elderly chimpanzees exhibited reductions in efficiency across multiple components of nut cracking, including taking more time to select stone tools prior to use, and taking longer to crack open nuts and consume the associated pieces of kernel. Two chimpanzees also began using less streamlined behavioral sequences to crack nuts, including a greater number of actions (such as more numerous strikes of the hammer stone). Most notably, we report interindividual variability in the extent to which tool-use behaviors senesced during aging – ranging from small to profound reductions in tool engagement and efficiency – as well as differences in the specific aspects of nut cracking behaviors which senesced for each individual. We discuss the possible causes of these changes with reference to research into senescence in captive primates, and provide future directions for research of primate aging in both captive and wild settings.


Emotion in Nonverbal Communication: Comparing Animal and Human Vocalizations and Human Text Messages

January 2025

·

87 Reads

Emotion Review

Humans and other animals communicate a large quantity of information vocally through nonverbal means. Here, we review the domains of animal vocalizations, human nonverbal vocal communication and computer-mediated communication (CMC), under the common thread of emotion, which, we suggest, connects them as a dimension of all these types of communication. After reviewing the use of emotions across domains, we focus on two concepts that have often been opposed to emotion in the animal versus human communication literature: control and meaning. Non-human vocal communication is commonly described as emotional, preventing either control or meaning; in contrast, the emotional dimension of human nonverbal signals does not prevent them from being perceived as both intentionally produced and meaningful. Amongst others, we disagree with this position, highlighting here that emotions should be integrated across species and modalities such as the written modality. We conclude by delineating ways in which each of these domains can meaningfully influence each other, and debates in their respective fields, and more generally the debate on the evolution of communication.


Figure 4 Example curvature analysis for the composite model. The panel on the right shows the composite model describing MI Adj decay for Jeje. The panel on the left illustrates the second differential of this composite function. The point after the minimand of the second differential indicates the transition to power-law decay (indicated by a vertical dashed line). This point is mirrored on the plot on the right by the same line. Full-size  DOI: 10.7717/peerj.18484/fig-4
Summary of sequence data collected from the 2011/2012 field season at Bossou (including all complete and incomplete sequences).
Summary of maximum comparative distances, model preferences, and transition points for action sequences with repeating actions.
AICc scores for MI Adj decay models, derived from condensed sequences.
Nonadjacent dependencies and sequential structure of chimpanzee action during a natural tool-use task

December 2024

·

26 Reads

·

3 Citations

Many of the complex behaviours of humans involve the production of nonadjacent dependencies between sequence elements, which in part can be generated through the hierarchical organization of sequences. To understand how these structural properties of human behaviours evolved, we can gain valuable insight from studying the sequential behaviours of nonhuman animals. Among the behaviours of nonhuman apes, tool use has been hypothesised to be a domain of behaviour which likely involves hierarchical organization, and may therefore possess nonadjacent dependencies between sequential actions. However thus far, evidence supporting hierarchical organization of great-ape tool use comes from methodologies which have been criticised in their objectivity. Additionally, the extent to which nonadjacent dependencies appear in primate action sequences during tool use has not been formally investigated. We used estimations of mutual information (MI)—a measure of dependency strength between sequence elements—to evaluate both the extent to which wild chimpanzees produce nonadjacent dependencies during a naturalistic tool-use task (nut cracking), as well as how sequences of actions are likely organized during tool use. Half of adult chimpanzees produced nonadjacent dependencies at significantly greater sequential distances than comparable, nonhierarchical Markov models once repeated actions had been accounted for. Additionally, for the majority of chimpanzees, MI decay with increasing sequential distance included a power-law relationship, which is a key indicator that the action sequences produced by chimpanzees likely entail some degree of hierarchical organization. Our analysis offered the greatest support for a system of organization where short subroutines of actions (2–8 actions long) are hierarchically arranged into longer sequences—a finding which is consistent with previous qualitative descriptions of ape tool-use behaviours. Interindividual variability was detected within our analysis in both the maximum distance dependencies were detected, and the most likely structuring mechanism for sequential action organization. We discuss these results in light of possible interindividual variation in the systems of action organization used by chimpanzees during tool use, in addition to methodological considerations for applications of MI estimations to sequential behaviours. Moreover, we discuss our main findings alongside hypotheses for the coevolution of complex syntax in language and tool-action across hominin evolutionary history.


Figure 1. The experimental set-up and behaviors at the outdoor laboratory. (a) Yo, an elderly female (approx. 51 years old),
Figure 2. Attendance and behavior of old-aged chimpanzees at the outdoor laboratory. (a) Attendance rate for individuals
Figure 3. Duration of stone-tool selection events. (a) Tool-selection duration times for each old-aged individual. Color
Figure 4. Metrics of efficiency for the cracking and processing of oil-palm nuts. This plot only includes metrics in which
Figure 5. Metrics for the cracking and processing of both oil-palm and coula nuts. Data is confined to individuals who
Old age variably impacts chimpanzee engagement and efficiency in stone tool use

November 2024

·

73 Reads

·

1 Citation

We know vanishingly little about how long-lived apes experience senescence in the wild, particularly with respect to their foraging behaviors, which are essential for survival. Some wild apes use tools during foraging and, given the additional cognitive and physical challenges presented by tool use, we predict that such behaviors are at a heightened risk of senescence. However, until the present, longitudinal analysis of the effects of progressive aging on wild ape tool-use behaviors has not been possible due to a lack of available data. In response to this research gap, we sampled data from a longitudinal video archive that contained footage of wild chimpanzees engaging in one of their most complex forms of tool use - the cracking of hard-shelled nuts with hammers and anvil stones, termed nut cracking - at an 'outdoor laboratory' at Bossou, Guinea. By sampling data over a 17-year period, we describe how progressive old age influences the extent to which wild chimpanzees engage with - and efficiently perform - nut cracking. With increasingly old age, chimpanzees began attending experimental nut cracking sites less frequently than younger individuals. Several elderly chimpanzees exhibited reductions in efficiency across multiple components of nut cracking, including taking more time to select stone tools prior to use, and taking longer to crack open nuts and consume the associated pieces of kernel. Two chimpanzees also began using less streamlined behavioral sequences to crack nuts, including a greater number of actions (such as more numerous strikes of the hammer stone). Most notably, we report interindividual variability in the extent to which tool-use behaviors senesced during aging - ranging from small to profound reductions in tool engagement and efficiency - as well as differences in the specific aspects of nut cracking behaviors which senesced for each individual. We discuss the possible causes of these changes with reference to research into senescence in captive primates, and provide future directions for research of primate aging in both captive and wild settings.


Figure 2. The maximum sequential distance dependencies were detected in action sequences and sequences from Markov
Figure 3. MIAdj decay profiles for each individual. The three candidate decay models are fitted. The exponential model is in green;
Figure 4. Example curvature analysis for the composite model. The panel on the right shows the composite model describing MI
Figure 5. The proportion of instances MIAdj decay was preferred when generating sequences from community Markov
Nonadjacent Dependencies and Syntactic Structure of Chimpanzee Action During a Natural Tool-Use Task

March 2024

·

93 Reads

The hierarchical organization of sequential behaviour, and the ability to compensate for nonadjacent sequential dependencies, are fundamental and interrelated abilities supporting complex human behaviours, including language and tool use. To understand how the cognition underlying these structural properties of human behaviour evolved, we can gain valuable insight from studying the sequential behaviours of nonhuman animals. Among the behaviours of nonhuman apes, tool use has been hypothesised to be a domain of behaviour which likely involves hierarchical organization. However thus far, evidence supporting this hypothesis comes from methodologies which have been criticised in their objectivity. Additionally, the extent to which nonadjacent dependencies appear in primate action sequences during tool use has not been formally investigated. We used estimations of mutual information (MI, a measure of dependency strength between sequence elements) to evaluate both the extent to which wild chimpanzees produce nonadjacent dependencies during a naturalistic tool use task (nut cracking), as well as how sequences of actions are organized during tool use. Half of adult chimpanzees produce nonadjacent dependencies at significantly greater sequential distances than comparable, nonhierarchical Markov models, including when repeated actions had been accounted for. Additionally, for the majority of chimpanzees, MI decay with increasing sequential distance included a power law relationship, which is a key indicator that most chimpanzees draw upon forms of hierarchical structuring when organizing behaviours during tool use. Our analysis offered the greatest support for a system of organization which involved the production of short subroutines of actions (2 to 8 actions), which are hierarchically arranged into sequences, and is consistent with previous qualitative descriptions of ape tool-use behaviours. Interindividual variability was detected within our analysis in both the distance dependencies were detected, and the most likely structuring mechanism for sequential action organization. We discuss these results in light of possible interindividual variation, in addition to methodological considerations for applications of MI estimations to sequential behaviours. Moreover, we discuss our main findings alongside hypotheses for the coevolution of complex syntax in language and tool action across hominin evolutionary history.



In search of animal normativity: a framework for studying social norms in non-human animals

January 2024

·

223 Reads

·

16 Citations

Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society

Social norms – rules governing which behaviours are deemed appropriate or inappropriate within a given community – are typically taken to be uniquely human. Recently, this position has been challenged by a number of philosophers, cognitive scientists, and ethologists, who have suggested that social norms may also be found in certain non-human animal communities. Such claims have elicited considerable scepticism from norm cognition researchers, who doubt that any non-human animals possess the psychological capacities necessary for normative cognition. However, there is little agreement among these researchers about what these psychological prerequisites are. This makes empirical study of animal social norms difficult, since it is not clear what we are looking for and thus what should count as behavioural evidence for the presence (or absence) of social norms in animals. To break this impasse, we offer an approach that moves beyond contested psychological criteria for social norms. This approach is inspired by the animal culture research program, which has made a similar shift away from heavily psychological definitions of ‘culture’ and to become organized around a cluster of more empirically tractable concepts of culture. Here, we propose an analogous set of constructs built around the core notion of a normative regularity, which we define as a socially maintained pattern of behavioural conformity within a community. We suggest methods for studying potential normative regularities in wild and captive primates. We also discuss the broader scientific and philosophical implications of this research program with respect to questions of human uniqueness, animal welfare and conservation.


Citations (51)


... In human language, syntax is characterized by a hierarchical structure where shorter sentences are often embedded in longer ones. While some species like chimpanzees are capable of producing hierarchically structured actions like when using tools (16), hierarchical structures have not yet been identified in animal vocal sequence productions. However, establishing whether the meaning of a combination is affected by simple ordering effects such as the order in which two units are combined is an essential step in investigating the evolutionary origins of syntax. ...

Reference:

Versatile use of chimpanzee call combinations promotes meaning expansion
Nonadjacent dependencies and sequential structure of chimpanzee action during a natural tool-use task

... Mainstream Western schooling may promote a more dyadic or individual approach to learning than one that is collaborative (Mejía-Arauz et al. 2007). In a recent study in Switzerland, children at a Montessori school displayed weaker imitative tendencies than did children at a traditional kindergarten (Décaillet et al. 2024). ...

Variation in pedagogy affects overimitation in children and adolescents
  • Citing Article
  • February 2024

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

... Jacob's tale is not just about the intensity of grief, but also emphasizes the value of forgiveness. Importantly, these other culturally transmitted traits may well be bio-culturally adaptive: how an individual treats others can be a major factor in how well they fare in society and also contribute to the stability of that society (Whiten and Byrne, 1997;Sterelny, 2003;Machery and Mallon, 2010;Mikhail, 2011;Tomasello, 2021;Westra and Andrews, 2022;Westra et al., 2024). ...

In search of animal normativity: a framework for studying social norms in non-human animals

Biological reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society

... However, as these classifications are not guaranteed to be accurate (Sanz et al., 2007; e.g. chimpanzees may also use terrestrial night nests, Romani et al., 2023;Tagg et al., 2013), we opted to conduct all analyses for both apes separately and pooled. ...

Decay rates of arboreal and terrestrial nests of Eastern chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the Bugoma Central Forest Reserve, Uganda: Implications for population size estimates

American Journal of Primatology

... The emotional content of the neighbor effects was thus not constrained to a particular context or vocalization, but the caller's state seems to have diffused to those who heard. Relatedly, Debracque et al. (2023) additionally find that even humans can recognize the emotional content of many primate vocalizations, with the highest performance with chimpanzee calls. This suggests deep-rooted emotional content embedded in these vocalizations. ...

Humans recognize affective cues in primate vocalizations: acoustic and phylogenetic perspectives

... Morgan et al. (2016) reported that the terrestrial nests of Western lowland gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) decayed more rapidly than arboreal ones, which they argued was influenced by nest construction type, canopy cover, rainfall, and forest structure. To our knowledge, no study has compared decay rates between ground and tree nests in chimpanzees; thus between 2020 and 2021, we conducted such a study in the Bugoma Central Forest Reserve (BCFR), where the chimpanzees are known to regularly build nighttime ground nests (Hobaiter et al., 2022). ...

Habitual ground nesting in the Bugoma Forest chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii ), Uganda

... Hand clasp, leaf strip and buttress drumming (Hohmann and Fruth [2003]) primatologists researching great apes in the wild (Gruber & Biro, 2023;Koops et al., 2023). Thus far, the strongest evidence for cumulative culture in nonhuman primates arguably comes from decades of observations collected on the food washing behaviors of Japanese macaques (Schofield et al., 2018 . ...

Escaping a blind alley: The ZLS as a ‘cultural crucible’?
  • Citing Article
  • December 2022

Physics of Life Reviews

... Information on propofol's effect on late auditory response, and especially ERP components N100 and P200 is however lacking. To date, only one study used propofol in a near-infrared spectroscopy experiment, and reported similarities in the processing of conspecific emotional vocalisations between humans and sedated baboons, highlighting propofol's potential 21 . Importantly, propofol's rapid anaesthetic response allows for fine-tuning anaesthesia at very low dosages 22 , enabling the induction of light sleep stages (NREM1-NREM2), where auditory processing is maintained, while avoiding deeper stages (NREM3-REM) that would impair auditory processing 23,24 . ...

Cerebral Activity in Female Baboons (Papio anubis) During the Perception of Conspecific and Heterospecific Agonistic Vocalizations: a Functional Near Infrared Spectroscopy Study

Affective Science

... Our synthesis supports the calls for incorporating nonhuman culture in conservation efforts [28,68] while cautioning that this concept should enhance, not necessarily replace, existing strategies [29]. Despite regular discourse in recent years on the merits of integrating culturally transmitted information into conservation unit designation [11,69], barriers and disincentives continue to challenge evidence-based conservation interventions, and practical recommendations for how to integrate nonhuman culture into said interventions have only recently emerged [10,70]. These new resources can, in theory, help us transition from acknowledging the existence of animal cultures to actively conserving them [70]. ...

A multicomponent approach to studying cultural propensities during foraging in the wild