Tanya L. Chartrand’s research while affiliated with Duke University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (102)


The Benefits—and Costs—of Behavioral Mimicry: Applications in Marketing, Sales, and Therapy
  • Chapter
  • Full-text available

November 2024

·

40 Reads

·

Tanya Chartrand

Previous research on the so-called Chameleon effect and other studies on more general mimicry indicate that mimicking another person’s gestures, mannerisms, and speech (whether intentionally or not) leads to several profound social consequences without awareness that mimicry took place (Chartrand and Bargh, J Personality Soc Psychol 76:893–910, 1999). This chapter reviews research on mimicry as a nonconsciously employed mechanism by focusing on the consequences of mimicry. Thereby, we will review positive as well as negative consequences that take place within and beyond the mimicry dyad.

Download

Money can buy me love: Gifts are a more effective form of acute social support than conversations

August 2024

·

29 Reads

Journal of Consumer Psychology

If you are having a hard day, what can someone else do to help you feel better? Maybe they could verbally comfort you or maybe they could give you a small gift. In seven studies, including an in‐person real gifting study, we find that receiving a small material gift, such as a candy bar or flowers, improves receivers' affect more than a supportive conversation with a close other does. We investigate the mechanism for this effect and find that support receivers perceive a gift to be a larger sacrifice than a conversation. This occurs because gifts seem more receiver‐focused (i.e., actions done solely to benefit the receiver) than do conversations. This difference in perceived sacrifice makes gifts (vs. conversations) more effective at promoting emotional recovery.


When and Why Antiegalitarianism Affects Resistance to Supporting Black-Owned Businesses

June 2024

·

33 Reads

·

1 Citation

Psychological Science

Steven Shepherd

·

Rowena Crabbe

·

Tanya L. Chartrand

·

[...]

·

Aaron C. Kay

Understanding how initiatives to support Black-owned businesses are received, and why, has important social and economic implications. To address this, we designed three experiments to investigate the role of antiegalitarian versus egalitarian ideologies among White American adults. In Study 1 ( N = 199), antiegalitarianism (vs. egalitarianism) predicted viewing initiatives supporting a Black-owned business as less fair, but only when the business was competing with other (presumably White-owned) businesses. In Study 2 ( N = 801), antiegalitarianism predicted applying survival-of-the-fittest market beliefs, particularly to Black-owned businesses. Antiegalitarianism also predicted viewing initiatives supporting Black-owned businesses as less fair than initiatives that targeted other (presumably White-owned) businesses, especially for tangible (vs. symbolic) support that directly impacts the success of a business. In Study 3 ( N = 590), antiegalitarianism predicted rejecting a program investing in Black-owned businesses. These insights demonstrate how antiegalitarian ideology can have the effect of maintaining race-based inequality, hindering programs designed to reduce that inequality.


Figure 5. Perceived social support as a function of participant role.
Celebrate Good Times: How Celebrations Increase Perceived Social Support

January 2023

·

1,314 Reads

·

4 Citations

Journal of Public Policy & Marketing

Despite the ubiquity of celebrations in everyday life, very little is known about how they may contribute to consumer well-being. In the current work, the authors propose that celebrations promote perceived social support, which prior work has conceptualized as the belief that others will be there for you in future, negative life events. The authors further theorize that celebrations require three key characteristics which, in combination, are necessary for increasing perceived social support. Specifically, celebrations must 1) mark an individual’s separate positive event and 2) involve consumption 3) with others (i.e., social). They test this theory across eight studies and demonstrate a process mechanism for this effect—these characteristics lead to increases in enacted support and perceived responsiveness, which in turn, lead to increases in more general perceived social support. They then extend these findings by investigating virtually held celebrations, the individual’s role at the celebration, and a downstream pro-social outcome. By doing so, this work highlights the broader benefits of celebrations beyond the focal individual and the immediate experience. Finally, specific policy implications and suggestions for enhancing consumer well-being are provided.


Participants rated how likable they found the confederate on a scale of 1 (extremely dislikable) to 7 (extremely likable). Mean likable rating was significantly higher for participants with a history of an eating disorder (ED-His) than for the healthy control (HC) participants and significantly higher for all participants in the No-Mimicry condition than for all participants in the Mimicry Condition. We did not find a significant group by condition interaction. Error bars represent standard error
Participants rated the smoothness of the interaction with the confederate on a scale of 1 (extremely awkward) to 7 (extremely smoothly). Within the Mimicry condition, participants with a history of an eating disorder (ED-His) rated the interaction as less smooth than healthy control (HC) participants. There was not a significant group difference within the No-Mimicry condition. Error bars represent standard error
For participants with a history of eating disorder, self-reported duration of eating disorder moderated the effect of condition on likable ratings. Duration of illness showed a positive relationship with likable ratings in the No-Mimicry condition but showed a negative relationship with likable ratings in the Mimicry condition. The Johnson-Neyman Floodlight technique showed that the simple effect of condition on likable ratings is significant when the duration of eating disorder was 3.87 years or longer. The vertical dashed line indicates the region of significance for the simple effect within the ED-His group: for participants who had a duration of eating disorder of 3.87 years or longer, likeable ratings from participants who were not mimicked were significantly higher than those from participants who were mimicked. Scatterplot points are jittered to avoid overlap
Experiences of mimicry in eating disorders

July 2022

·

122 Reads

·

2 Citations

Journal of Eating Disorders

Background People unknowingly mimic the behaviors of others, a process that results in feelings of affiliation. However, some individuals with eating disorders describe feeling “triggered” when mimicked. This study explores the effects of implicit non-verbal mimicry on individuals with a history of an eating disorder (ED-His) compared to healthy controls (HCs). Method Women ( N = 118, n ED-His = 31; M age = 21 years) participated in a laboratory task with a confederate trained to either discreetly mimic (Mimicry condition) or not mimic (No-Mimicry condition) the mannerisms of the participant. Participants rated the likability of the confederate and the smoothness of the interaction. Results Participants in the No-Mimicry condition rated the confederate as significantly more likable than in the Mimicry condition, and ED-His rated the confederate as more likable than HCs. ED-His in the Mimicry condition rated the interaction as less smooth than HCs, whereas this pattern was not found in the No-Mimicry condition. Among ED-His, longer disorder duration (≥ 3.87 years) was associated with less liking of a confederate who mimicked and more liking of a confederate who did not mimic. Conclusions We discuss the implications of these findings for interpersonal therapeutic processes and group treatment settings for eating disorders. Plain English summary Our study on subtle, nonverbal mimicry revealed differences in social behavior for women with a history of an eating disorder compared to healthy women. For participants with an eating disorder history, a longer duration of illness was associated with a worse pattern of affiliation, reflected in lower liking of a mimicker. Further research on how diverging processes of affiliation may function to perpetuate the chronicity of eating disorders and implications for treatment is needed.


Being there without being there: Gifts compensate for lack of in-person support

January 2022

·

387 Reads

·

10 Citations

Psychology and Marketing

Providing social support is a critical part of being in a relationship with someone, but people often struggle to support loved ones in person. In this paper, we show how givers can use gifts to compensate for not providing in-person social support. Study 1 shows that when it is prohibitively difficult for givers to provide in-person support, they give more expensive gifts. Study 2 replicates this effect for likelihood to give a gift and shows it is not due to social desirability. Studies 3, 4a, and 4b find that guilt over not having provided adequate support drives people to give gifts, and that giving gifts partially relieves givers' feelings of guilt. Studies 5 and 6 examine moderation. In Study 5, people only compensate for a lack of in-person support with a gift when they have a strong obligation to support the recipient because they are close to them. Study 6 shows that money does not substitute for in-person support. We show the role of gifts in enhancing givers' wellbeing and provide new customer insights to managers on reasons people purchase gifts.


Figure 4. Results from study 5: Individuals report the greatest perceptions of relationship power when both partner engagement and self-influence are high.
Better to Decide Together: Shared Consumer Decision Making, Perceived Power, and Relationship Satisfaction

June 2021

·

1,473 Reads

·

22 Citations

Journal of Consumer Psychology

Shared consumer decisions, particularly those made with a relationship partner, can be very different from decisions that are made alone. Across multiple studies, we investigate how shared consumer decision making affects perceptions of power and relationship satisfaction. We integrate two streams of research to create a novel theory about consumer decision making and perceived power. Specifically, we suggest that shared consumer decision making combines two necessary components of power—an individual’s influence over and a partner’s engagement in the decision—and that these combined components drive power perceptions. In other words, individuals who relinquish some control and make a decision with their partner, ironically, perceive having greater power than if they had made the decision alone. We further find that shared decision making and greater perceived power lead to greater satisfaction with the relationship in which the decisions are made. By focusing on consumer decision making within relationships, the current research contributes to the literatures on decision making, social influences in consumer behavior, close relationships, consumer well‐being, and power.


Sincere, Not Sinful: Political Ideology and the Unique Role of Brand Sincerity in Shaping Heterosexual and LGBTQ Consumers' Views of LGBTQ Ads

April 2021

·

2,013 Reads

·

13 Citations

Journal of the Association for Consumer Research

Brand sincerity (wholesome, family-oriented) is core dimension of brand personality; however, to what extent is LGBTQ representation congruent with brand sincerity? We argue that whether or not LGBTQ representation is considered “wholesome” or “family-friendly,” and how consumers respond to such representation, is dependent on political ideology. Results show that an ad featuring a same-sex (vs. opposite-sex) couple is rated lower in brand sincerity by conservatives, which then uniquely predicts ad attitudes. Conservatives also saw a same-sex couple as more incongruent with a sincere brand, and this incongruence predicted ad attitudes. However, these effects were not found when the brand was exciting. Conversely, LGBTQ consumers’ preferred same-sex couple representation in the context of a sincere (vs. exciting) brand. Finally, an archival analysis of ads found that sincere (vs. exciting) brands represent LGBTQ consumers more favorably. Implications are discussed.


Mimicry and Modeling of Health(-Risk) Behaviors: How Others Impact Our Health(-Risk) Behaviors Without Our Awareness

March 2020

·

133 Reads

·

12 Citations

Journal of Nonverbal Behavior

When it comes to health(-risk) behaviors, researchers are apt to consider how to change behaviors from a top-down approach (i.e., using the conscious, reflective, deliberate system) even though much of human behavior is determined by bottom-up processes (i.e., the nonconscious, reflexive, impulsive system). Given that researchers have proposed that interventions that target nonconscious processes underlying health(-risk) behaviors may prove to be more effective than interventions that target conscious processes, we argue that understanding the ways in which mimicking and modeling affect health(-risk) behaviors is a critical—albeit largely unexplored—domain. In this paper, we review existing evidence that people mimic and model a broad range of health(-risk) behaviors: specifically, the cigarette smoking, alcohol drinking, (un)healthy eating, and physical activity of others. We then discuss the neural and psychological mechanisms underlying these effects. Next, we outline moderators: specifically, we discuss when mimicking and modeling are more likely to occur, who is more likely to mimic and model, as well as who is more likely to be mimicked and modeled. Finally, we consider how mimicry and modeling could be used to leverage healthy behavioral change.


Figure 1. A conceptual diagram of the studies.
The Gift of Psychological Closeness: How Feasible Versus Desirable Gifts Reduce Psychological Distance to the Giver

March 2019

·

2,075 Reads

·

31 Citations

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

Gift-giving is a common form of social exchange but little research has examined how different gift types affect the psychological distance between giver and recipient. We examined how two types of gifts influence recipients’ perceived psychological distance to the giver. Specifically, we compared desirable gifts focused on the quality of the gift with feasible gifts focused on the gift’s practicality or ease of use. We found that feasible (vs. desirable) gifts led recipients to feel psychologically closer to givers (Studies 1 – 4). Further clarifying the process by which receiving a desirable versus feasible gift affects perceived distance, when recipients were told that the giver focused on the gift’s practicality or ease of use (vs. the gift’s overall quality), while holding the specific features of the gifts constant, they felt closer to the gift-giver (Study 5). These results shed light on how different gifts can influence interpersonal relationships.


Citations (90)


... Based on research on affiliation goals, it has often been argued that individuals imitate people high in power and status more than people low in power and status, because individuals have a stronger motivation to affiliate with high than with low power and status people (Chartrand & Dalton, 2009;Chartrand et al., 2005;Duffy & Chartrand, 2015). However, there is not much evidence that power and status itself actually influence imitative behavior. ...

Reference:

Social Modulation of Imitative Behavior
Beyond the Perception-Behavior Link: The Ubiquitous Utility and Motivational Moderators of Nonconscious Mimicry
  • Citing Chapter
  • October 2004

... Various mechanisms have been proposed to underlie this link between perception and behavior, which include, in addition to mirror neurons, shared schemas (Barresi & Moore, 1996), shared representations (Prinz, 1997), or spreading activation (see Chartrand & Dalton, 2009). In either case, the perceptual activity is presumed to spread to behavioral representations, which in turn increases the probability of imitating that same behavior, without conscious awareness, control, or intent (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999). ...

Mimicry: Its Ubiquity, Importance, and Functionality
  • Citing Chapter
  • October 2008

... i Appreciating the fact that life education teaching, which differs from death education (4,6,9), is proactive and positive. For example, life education emphasizes the importance of the functioning of positive attributes such as aspiration (21), celebration (22), hope (23), optimism (24), and the like. A student's aspiration to enter medical school, in this analysis, may serve to motivate him or her to strive for exceptional success. ...

Celebrate Good Times: How Celebrations Increase Perceived Social Support

Journal of Public Policy & Marketing

... The authors highlight the importance of naturalistic settings to investigate the reciprocal nonverbal processes between clients and counselors and reference Scheflens idea (1964) that the congruence of body parts between two individuals is an indication of empathy, rapport, and mutual interest. However, people affected by an eating disorder seem to react differently to being mimicked (Erwin et al., 2022). Eating disorders involve abnormal eating, and exaggerated preoccupation with food, body weight, or shape, which cannot be explained by other health conditions or the cultural context. ...

Experiences of mimicry in eating disorders

Journal of Eating Disorders

... Specifically, de Hooge (2014) finds that negative other-caused emotions (e.g., anger) decrease gift-giving while negative selfcaused emotions (e.g., guilt) increase gift-giving. Adding to the literature on emotions in gift-giving, the present research focuses on a negative emotion, guilt, which motivates an individual to give a gift to lessen feelings of guilt and restore or improve the relationship with the recipient (Chan, Mogilner, and Boven 2014;Wiener, Howe, and Chartrand 2022). We propose that in the face of fault and perceived accountability, a guilty gift-giver will pay attention to possible threats or punishment from the receiver and feel a heightened sense of powerlessness. ...

Being there without being there: Gifts compensate for lack of in-person support

Psychology and Marketing

... At the meso level, SWB articles explore concepts of shared decision-making (Dahl et al., 2021) and relationship satisfaction (Brick et al., 2021;Brick et al., 2018). ...

Better to Decide Together: Shared Consumer Decision Making, Perceived Power, and Relationship Satisfaction

Journal of Consumer Psychology

... In the marketing domain, it is possible that companies design poor products for gay consumers because they have inaccurate stereotypes about their product preferences. There is little work in consumer research that shows what people think of gay consumers product preferences, and that tests whether these stereotypes are correct, as most research is about reactions to the presence of gay people in advertisement campaigns (Bond & Farrell, 2020;Chinchanachokchai et al., 2022;Nölke, 2018;Oakenfull & Greenlee, 2004;Shepherd et al., 2021), or ethnographic research about gay consumers' experiences (Buford, 2000;Kates, 2002;Rudd, 1996). ...

Sincere, Not Sinful: Political Ideology and the Unique Role of Brand Sincerity in Shaping Heterosexual and LGBTQ Consumers' Views of LGBTQ Ads
  • Citing Article
  • April 2021

Journal of the Association for Consumer Research

... Indeed, individuals who share the same environment are exposed to similar social and environmental determinants. Habits and automatisms are easily shared (Marteau et al., 2012) by mimicry (Duffy et al., 2020) within neighborhoods. Spatial autocorrelation is the statistical measure that quantifies this similarity of attributes between sample point locations-i.e., where individuals live. ...

Mimicry and Modeling of Health(-Risk) Behaviors: How Others Impact Our Health(-Risk) Behaviors Without Our Awareness

Journal of Nonverbal Behavior

... Humans unconsciously mimic the gestures, facial expressions, or speech of others during social interactions (Chartrand, Bargh, 1999;Clarke, 2013;Kulesza et al., 2015). However, it does not mean that mimicry cannot be used strategically (Duffy et al., 2019). Mimicry can cause many positive social consequences, even when people are unaware of its presence or when they do not know each other (Chartrand, Bargh, 1999). ...

Enhancing activation in the right temporoparietal junction using theta-burst stimulation: Disambiguating between two hypotheses of top-down control of behavioral mimicry

... Facial mimicry is of particular importance because it has a specific emotional meaning as it represents a congruent mimic response to an emotional facial expression [2]. It is detectable in the electromyogram (EMG) after only 200-400 ms [3,4] and occurs unconsciously and automatically [5]. Facial mimicry has been shown to be triggered even when affective cues are perceived only unconsciously [4,6]. ...

The Unbearable Automaticity of Being: Key Readings
  • Citing Chapter
  • December 2004