Susan T. Fiske’s research while affiliated with Princeton University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (33)


Lost opportunities: How gendered arrangements harm men
  • Article

January 2025

·

149 Reads

·

2 Citations

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

·

Susan T Fiske

Traditional gendered arrangements—norms, roles, prejudices, and hierarchies—shape every human life. Associated harms are primarily framed as women’s issues due to more severe consequences women face. Yet, gendered arrangements also shape men ’s relationships, career paths, and health. Current work on gender equity overlooks men’s perspectives. Despite benefits they gain from out-ranking women, men’s position paradoxically entraps them in restrictive roles, compelling them to prioritize dominance. An inclusive framework challenges prevailing narratives by considering personal costs borne by men. Identifying with a man’s traditional role is a mixed privilege, as five gendered arrangements show for men who subscribe to them: 1. Masculine norms can restrict men’s choices and are associated with adverse health trajectories; 2. Some men’s disengagement from communal roles denies them positive outcomes associated with caring for others; 3. Hostile sexism fosters antipathy, fueling tension in some men’s interactions with women; 4. Benevolent sexism forces some men into scripted interactions, preventing genuine connections and burdening them with unrealistic breadwinner and protector roles; 5. Societal shifts in gender hierarchies can elicit threat responses in men, depending on intersections with social class and racial identities. Understanding costs to men calls for more empirical research. Gender equity for men, whose circumstances differ from those of women, would enable men to make informed choices and achieve better outcomes for themselves—paralleling the progress women have made in many areas of life. Striving for equity for all genders can ultimately enhance overall human well-being.


ASI Review - Online Supplement.pdf
  • Data
  • File available

October 2023

·

236 Reads

Download

The rounded rectangles represent the two ideologies that encompass ambivalent sexism (hostile sexism and benevolent sexism). The hexagons represent the five main domains of research on ambivalent sexism. The rectangles represent aggregated findings for each ideology within each domain. The ovals represent the central processes that characterize each ideology across domains.
The pink circles represent the domains, the orange circles represent the subdomains, and the purple circles group domains based on conceptual similarity of findings (i.e., the main domains are presented next to their adjacent domains). The size of the circles is based on volume (i.e., the number of articles per domain). A table with the list of domains and the number of articles per domain is available on the Open Science Framework at https://osf.io/b6h8x. SDO = social dominance orientation; RWA = right-wing authoritarianism.
Illustration of the geographical areas of the samples reported in the included articles (Narticles = 654; Ncountries = 75). This figure contains also data from four large cross-cultural investigations (more than 10 countries sampled) that were included in the review (Bosson et al., 2021; Glick et al., 2000, 2004; Kosakowska-Berezecka et al., 2020). A table with the list of countries and the number of samples within each country is available at the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/b6h8x).
The number of publications retrieved per year from 1996 to 2020 in the search conducted at the end of July 2021 in the PsycINFO database. The search targeted peer-reviewed articles that have mentioned one or more of the following terms in their titles, abstracts, or keywords: “ambivalent sexis*”, “benevolent sexis*”, “hostile sexis*,” with relevant limiters. The gray line represents the developing trend of publications across the years. The data file for this figure (as well as the full search query) is available at the Open Science Framework (https://osf.io/b6h8x).
A Systematic Review of the Ambivalent Sexism Literature: Hostile Sexism Protects Men’s Power; Benevolent Sexism Guards Traditional Gender Roles

October 2023

·

1,200 Reads

·

96 Citations

According to ambivalent sexism theory (Glick & Fiske, 1996), the coexistence of gendered power differences and mutual interdependence creates two apparently opposing but complementary sexist ideologies: hostile sexism (HS; viewing women as manipulative competitors who seek to gain power over men) coincides with benevolent sexism (BS; a chivalrous view of women as pure and moral, yet weak and passive, deserving men’s protection and admiration, as long as they conform). The research on these ideologies employs the Ambivalent Sexism Inventory, used extensively in psychology and allied disciplines, often to understand the roles sexist attitudes play in reinforcing gender inequality. Following contemporary guidelines, this systematic review utilizes a principled approach to synthesize the multidisciplinary empirical literature on ambivalent sexism. After screening 1,870 potentially relevant articles and fully reviewing 654 eligible articles, five main domains emerge in ambivalent sexism research (social ideologies, violence, workplace, stereotypes, intimate relationships). The accumulating evidence across domains offers bottom-up empirical support for ambivalent sexism as a coordinated system to maintain control over women (and sometimes men). Hostile sexism acts through the direct and diverse paths of envious/resentful prejudices, being more sensitive to power and sexuality cues; Benevolent sexism acts through prejudices related to interdependence (primarily gender-based paternalism and gender-role differentiation), enforcing traditional gender relations and being more sensitive to role-related cues. Discussion points to common methodological limitations, suggests guidelines, and finds future avenues for ambivalent sexism research.



Explore-Exploit Tradeoffs Generate Cascading Societal Stereotypes

September 2022

·

9 Reads

Mental representations of human social groups – social stereotypes – are widespread and consequential. Such mental representations are systematic and multidimensional (e.g., stereotypes of immigrant groups are organized by perceived warmth and competence). We show that adaptive exploration alone can create structured societal stereotypes that cascade from historical affordances – such as which group happened to be the first one adequate at a job – without requiring decision-makers to have malicious intentions or cognitive limitations, or social groups to differ in information accessibility or intrinsic quality. Rather than framing social perception as a static, one-shot event, we consider the consequences of sequential decisions in a setting where exploring new options carries an implicit cost (resulting in an “explore-exploit tradeoff”). In this setting, when groups have equal and high potential to succeed in diverse jobs, decision-makers nonetheless settle on a single social group to perform each job and form impressions of that group accordingly. Using stereotypes of immigrant groups based on warmth and competence as an example, we formalize this process as a contextual multi-armed bandit problem, show testable predictions from computational simulations, and demonstrate that human participants act consistently with these predictions in behavioral experiments. Our results show how rich, multidimensional stereotypes can emerge in an absolutely minimal setting.


Love and Work: How to (and Not to) Have Both

May 2022

·

14 Reads

·

1 Citation

This volume traces the life journeys of a cohort of influential and transformative women in psychology, now in or nearing retirement, who have changed the discipline and the broader world of academia in significant ways. The 26 reflective essays record how these scholars thrived in an academic landscape that was often, at best, unwelcoming, and, at worst, hostile, toward them. They explicitly and implicitly acknowledge that their paths were inextricably linked with the evolution of women's roles in society; they highlight and celebrate their achievements as much as they acknowledge and recognize the obstacles, barriers, and hurdles they overcame. They tell their stories with candor and humor, resulting in a compilation of inspiring essays. The end result of these individual narratives is a volume that provides a unique resource for current and future academics to help them navigate through the crossroads, curves, and challenges of their own careers in academia.


Inaccurate Stereotypes from Rational Exploration

April 2021

·

11 Reads

Inaccurate stereotypes -- perceived differences among groups that do not actually differ -- are prevalent and consequential. Past research explains stereotypes as emerging from motivational biases, cognitive limitations, and information deficits. An alternative proposal focuses on exploration: An initial arbitrary interaction, if rewarding enough, may discourage investigating alternatives that would be equal or better. Historical accidents can snowball, through locally rational choices, into globally inaccurate generalizations. Thus, inaccurate stereotypes can emerge from otherwise rational exploration, even in the absence of real group differences. Ironically, the mere act of rationally choosing between groups with the goal of maximizing the long-term benefit of local interactions is enough to produce globally inaccurate stereotypes. Formally describing this problem using multi-armed bandits shows how an optimal solution with existing psychological support (Thompson sampling) produces inaccurate stereotypes. This result replicates in two large online experiments (N = 2404). This minimal-process paradigm thus suffices to produce bias.


Understanding Human Impressions of Artificial Intelligence

February 2021

·

190 Reads

·

11 Citations

Artificial intelligence increasingly suffuses everyday life. However, people are frequently reluctant to interact with A.I. systems. This challenges both the deployment of beneficial A.I. technology and the development of deep learning systems that depend on humans for oversight, direction, and training. Previously neglected but fundamental, social-cognitive processes guide human interactions with A.I. systems. In five behavioral studies (N = 3,099), warmth and competence feature prominently in participants’ impressions of artificially intelligent systems. Judgments of warmth and competence systematically depend on human-A.I. interdependence. In particular, participants perceive systems that optimize interests aligned with human interests as warmer and systems that operate independently from human direction as more competent. Finally, a prisoner’s dilemma game shows that warmth and competence judgments predict participants’ willingness to cooperate with a deep learning system. These results demonstrate the generality of intent detection to interactions with technological actors. Researchers and developers should carefully consider the degree and alignment of interdependence between humans and new artificial intelligence systems.


Social Cognition evolves: Illustrations from our work on Intergroup Bias and on Healthy Adaptation

August 2020

·

96 Reads

·

22 Citations

Psicothema

When we first wrote Social Cognition (1984), social psychology's crisis critiqued methods, replicability, theory, and relevance. Social cognition research illustrates four phases of response to these challenges. First, the Cognitive Miser approach introduced methods less prone to experimenter or participant interference: looking time as attention, categorical memory for who said what. Next, the Motivated Tactician approach addressed replicability by identifying moderator variables, primarily goals and motivations. For example, interdependence (Fiske) and threat (Taylor) are prominent motivations in our respective research. The third wave, perceivers as Activated Actors, translated mental states to behavior, using theory-guided prediction. In intergroup bias, for example, Fiske's Stereotype Content Model predicts patterns of discriminatory behavior distinctive to each combination of stereotypic warmth and competence. Going beyond reported behavior, distinctive activations emerged in brain-imaging and muscle responses. In health psychology, Taylor's Positive Illusions theory predicts people cope with life-threatening illness by viewing the odds optimistically, the self positively, and possible control affirmatively. Again, the social cognitive processes interplay with psycho-physiology. Recently, social cognitive approaches have increasingly addressed inequality: health disparities, bias interventions, power dynamics, class effects, social morality, and intent inferences. Viewing perceivers as Inequality Enablers answers any remaining doubts about the field's continuing relevance.



Citations (22)


... Social roles imposed on men put pressure on them, causing them to experience various mental and physical health problems more often (Brod, 2018). It is important for men to be a part of gender discussions; studies show that (Bareket & Fiske, 2025;Rice et al., 2021;Moss-Racusin, 2014;Fleming et al., 2014) norms harm individuals. Sandra Lipsitz Bem (1981), who evaluates femininity and masculinity as binary categories, developed the schema theory to determine gender roles. ...

Reference:

SEX-TYPING OF GENDER IDENTITY: A CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE TO K-POP IDOLS’ PORTRAYALS OF PERCEIVED MASCULINITY
Lost opportunities: How gendered arrangements harm men
  • Citing Article
  • January 2025

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

... Gün and Bayraktar's (2008) study demonstrating a reduction in social support networks of adolescents in the period following their migration process also supports this view. The ethnic identity and heteronomousrelated self-construal tend to be stronger in individuals who spend more time among their own social groups (Fente & Fiske, 2018). Moreover, following their migration, immigrants encounter a lot of problems that can contribute to this result. ...

Ethnic Identity and Ethnic Organizations: The Role of Self-Construal in the Psychological Well-Being of Migrants
  • Citing Conference Paper
  • January 2018

... Most academic advisory personnel allied with sponsorship, whom female staff believed could assist with obtaining a promotion, were unattainable, perceived as discriminatory to women, and were male. Women perceived their male counterparts as favouring men for positions of institution wide significance and sought to ease what was believed to be a 'glass ceiling' preventing women from attaining positions of power [29,30,31]. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), ...

A Systematic Review of the Ambivalent Sexism Literature: Hostile Sexism Protects Men’s Power; Benevolent Sexism Guards Traditional Gender Roles

... In HCI, the SCM has been successfully used to investigate how people react to various stimuli such as digital avatars [45], to understand social acceptability for mobile devices [46], and to characterize stereotypical portrayals in personas [47]. Past work has also used the SCM to tackle stereotypical language with anti-stereotypes [48] and to detect stereotypes in the news [49]. ...

Understanding Human Impressions of Artificial Intelligence
  • Citing Preprint
  • February 2021

... Despite enforcing anti-discriminatory laws, the legacy of Jim Crow and redlining has shaped the value of the housing market and has remained in a United States that is largely divided along racial lines 43 . This spatial separation has profound implications for access to employment opportunities [44][45][46] . Such segregation creates an environment where certain communities have access to subpar jobs, setting the stage for living in a disadvantaged life for Black and Latino people despite having education and employment 47,48 . ...

Gendered and Racialized Occupations
  • Citing Article
  • August 2020

Academy of Management Proceedings

... These beliefs are often based on limited information and used as mental "shortcuts" to help individuals process information quickly and make sense of the world around them. (Fiske, Taylor, 2020) Empathy is 'the ability to experience affective and cognitive states of another person, whilst maintaining a distinct self, in order to understand the other'. This is consistent with the understanding that empathy includes at least two key dimensions: cognition and affect (Decety, 2011). ...

Social Cognition evolves: Illustrations from our work on Intergroup Bias and on Healthy Adaptation
  • Citing Article
  • August 2020

Psicothema

... The aim of our study was to explore health providers' perspectives on the effect of COVID-19 on maternal health services in Nairobi, Kenya. This study is relevant because it highlights vulnerability, resilience, motivation, and human and financial resource-related factors relevant to informing measures to prepare for future crises [21]. ...

Research priorities for the COVID‐19 pandemic and beyond: A call to action for psychological science

... If they seem unfriendly but competent, you may resent them. On the other hand, the ABC model [Koch et al. 2016[Koch et al. , 2020 suggests that beyond warmth and competence, you also consider their beliefs , i.e., do they share your values or opinions? For example, if your coworker is friendly and competent but has completely different political or ethical views, you might still feel distant from them. ...

Groups' warmth is a personal matter: Understanding consensus on stereotype dimensions reconciles adversarial models of social evaluation
  • Citing Article
  • July 2020

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

... Alternatively, Plant and Devine (1998) separate "internal and external motivations to respond without prejudice," noting that personally rejecting prejudice is different from merely seeking to avoid punishment and gain approval from others who reject prejudice. Elsewhere, Fiske and colleagues (Fiske et al. 2002, also see Abele-Brehm et al. 2020) propose the Stereotype Content Model, which posits a twodimensional space defined by stereotypes of warmth and competence, yielding four emotions toward outgroups: admiration toward groups stereotyped as high in both warmth and competence, envy toward groups seen as high in competence but low in warmth, pity toward groups seen as high in warmth but low in competence, and contempt toward groups seen as low in both warmth and competence. Finally, Intergroup Emotions Theory (Devos et al. 2002) attempts to combine appraisal theories of emotion (Smith and Ellsworth 1985) with self-categorization theory (Turner 1985), yielding a single theory that identifies intergroup emotions as appraisals of situations in which outgroups affect the goals and interests of the agent's ingroup. ...

Navigating the social world_Feb 27 2020
  • Citing Preprint
  • February 2020

... This perspective suggests that even when non-marginalized people intend to alleviate the distress of others, their actions can inadvertently backfire and harm individuals of historically marginalized groups. In this sense, the wrong actions -regardless of how much they are compassionately motivated -can lead to greater disadvantages for marginalized individuals (Kreps et al., 2019;Warren & Warren, 2021). Supporting this idea, research has shown that poorly enacted allyship can lead to increased patronization and infantilization of marginalized groups (e.g., , microaggressions (Sue et al., 2007) and upholding discriminatory power structures (Russell & Bohan, 2016). ...

Allies' Motives, Merits and Missteps: How Dominant Group Members Can Promote Inclusive Organizations
  • Citing Article
  • August 2019

Academy of Management Proceedings