Steven J. Sherman’s research while affiliated with Indiana University Bloomington and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (185)


The Spatial Ingroup Bias: Ingroup Teams Are Positioned Where Writing Starts
  • Article

January 2021

·

72 Reads

·

3 Citations

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

·

·

·

[...]

·

Steven J. Sherman

In four studies, we test the hypothesis that people, asked to envisage interactions between an ingroup and an outgroup, tend to spatially represent the ingroup where writing starts (e.g., left in Italian) and as acting along script direction. Using soccer as a highly competitive intergroup setting, in Study 1 (N = 100) Italian soccer fans were found to envisage their team on the left side of a horizontal soccer field, hence playing rightward. Studies 2a and 2b (N = 219 Italian and N = 200 English speakers) replicate this finding, regardless of whether the own team was stronger or weaker than the rival team. Study 3 (N = 67 Italian and N = 67 Arabic speakers) illustrates the cultural underpinnings of the Spatial Intergroup Bias, showing a rightward ingroup bias for Italian speakers and a leftward ingroup bias for Arabic speakers. Findings are discussed in relation to how space is deployed to symbolically express ingroup favoritism (Spatial Ingroup Bias) versus shared stereotypes (Spatial Agency Bias).


What's Next? Disentangling availability from representativeness using binary decision tasks.

March 2018

·

242 Reads

·

9 Citations

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

People's intuitive predictions under uncertainty may rely on the representativeness or on the availability heuristics (Tversky & Kahneman, 1974). However, the distinction between these two heuristics has never been clear, and both have been proposed to underlie the same judgment tasks. For instance, when judging what outcome is likely to be next in a coin flip after a streak, representativeness leads to predicting an alternation in the outcome, ending the streak (gambler's fallacy), whereas availability leads to predicting the streak's continuation. We propose that availability (direct use of accessibility) is computed earlier than representativeness (comparing to an abstract representation of the expected outcome). In five studies, we pit one heuristic against the other in binary prediction tasks, both in coin flip and athlete's performance contexts. We find that, although the streak outcome is cognitively more available, judgments are usually based on representativeness, leading more often to a prediction of an alternation after a streak. However, under time-pressure conditions, representativeness processes are constrained and participants are more prone to base their predictions on the most salient and cognitively available outcomes.


On the Cognitive Determinants of Out-Group Dehumanization: Illusory Correlation and the Dehumanization of (Numerical) Group Minorities
  • Article
  • Full-text available

December 2017

·

371 Reads

·

2 Citations

Social Cognition

Humanness is something generally associated with the in-group and denied to out-groups. However, not all out-groups are equally deprived of their humanness. Correlational studies suggest that numerical minorities (from the perceiver’s perspective) are more likely to be judged as less than human. In the present research, we interpreted this phenomenon in terms of the cognitive processes involved in illusory correlation and identified and tested the conditions (i.e., humanness as a frequent and shared characteristic, and relative small group size) that would contribute to minority dehumanization. In two studies in which participants learned about members of two new groups differing in size, we found support for this account. The findings of the present research broaden the current knowledge of out-group dehumanization, showing the contribution of cognitive processes in the denial of humanness to minority groups. Implications for the understanding of the determinants of group dehumanization and illusory correlation are discussed.

Download

Motivated reasoning in the prediction of sports outcomes and the belief in the "hot hand"

October 2016

·

385 Reads

·

11 Citations

The present paper explores the role of motivation to observe a certain outcome in people's predictions, causal attributions, and beliefs about a streak of binary outcomes (basketball scoring shots). In two studies we found that positive streaks (points scored by the participants' favourite team) lead participants to predict the streak's continuation (belief in the hot hand), but negative streaks lead to predictions of its end (gambler's fallacy). More importantly, these wishful predictions are supported by strategic attributions and beliefs about how and why a streak might unfold. Results suggest that the effect of motivation on predictions is mediated by a serial path via causal attributions to the teams at play and belief in the hot hand.


Analytic and heuristic processes in the detection and resolution of conflict

April 2016

·

332 Reads

·

21 Citations

Memory & Cognition

Previous research with the ratio-bias task found larger response latencies for conflict trials where the heuristic- and analytic-based responses are assumed to be in opposition (e.g., choosing between 1/10 and 9/100 ratios of success) when compared to no-conflict trials where both processes converge on the same response (e.g., choosing between 1/10 and 11/100). This pattern is consistent with parallel dual-process models, which assume that there is effective, rather than lax, monitoring of the output of heuristic processing. It is, however, unclear why conflict resolution sometimes fails. Ratio-biased choices may increase because of a decline in analytical reasoning (leaving heuristic-based responses unopposed) or to a rise in heuristic processing (making it more difficult for analytic processes to override the heuristic preferences). Using the process-dissociation procedure, we found that instructions to respond logically and response speed affected analytic (controlled) processing (C), leaving heuristic processing (H) unchanged, whereas the intuitive preference for large nominators (as assessed by responses to equal ratio trials) affected H but not C. These findings create new challenges to the debate between dual-process and single-process accounts, which are discussed.


Strategic Numeracy: Self-Serving Reasoning About Health Statistics

June 2015

·

125 Reads

·

19 Citations

Basic and Applied Social Psychology

This research shows that the same people who appear to have low numerical competence when analyzing personally irrelevant health-related numerical information are able to overcome their reasoning shortcomings and make better judgments when they are shown equivalent information that is personally relevant, and when only a sophisticated kind of reasoning enables them to interpret this information in a favorable way. The fact that people can engage in poorer or more sophisticated numerical reasoning depending on whether that reasoning produces favorable or unfavorable conclusions has implications both for the concept of numeracy as an individual-difference variable and for health communication.




Don't Rush to Flush Appendix B SI text 2015

January 2015

·

35 Reads

Encouraging sustainable water use is a critical endeavor in addressing issues associated with short- and long-term droughts. Toilets account for up to 26.7% of indoor household water use. Therefore substantial opportunity exists to conserve water via reduced flushing after urination at home. Here, we use an online survey (N = 1008) to identify barriers to reduced flushing. The majority of participants reported they always flush (63%) and believed that others should always flush. Social norms surrounding cleanliness are the most prevalent reasons for flushing. Results suggest four main barriers to reducing flushing: disgust sensitivity, habitual nature of flushing, norms regarding cleanliness, and lack of pro-environmental motivations. Participants who always flush are less likely to sacrifice for the environment than occasional flushers. Participants tended to underestimate average American water use and their own water use. Targeted interventions to decrease urine-related disgust and increase pro-environmental motivations may help achieve water conservation goals.


Don't Rush to Flush Appendix A Final Survey

January 2015

·

60 Reads

Encouraging sustainable water use is a critical endeavor in addressing issues associated with short- and long-term droughts. Toilets account for up to 26.7% of indoor household water use. Therefore substantial opportunity exists to conserve water via reduced flushing after urination at home. Here, we use an online survey (N = 1008) to identify barriers to reduced flushing. The majority of participants reported they always flush (63%) and believed that others should always flush. Social norms surrounding cleanliness are the most prevalent reasons for flushing. Results suggest four main barriers to reducing flushing: disgust sensitivity, habitual nature of flushing, norms regarding cleanliness, and lack of pro-environmental motivations. Participants who always flush are less likely to sacrifice for the environment than occasional flushers. Participants tended to underestimate average American water use and their own water use. Targeted interventions to decrease urine-related disgust and increase pro-environmental motivations may help achieve water conservation goals.


Citations (89)


... The selection of support sources involved in MSSA assessment is driven by their relevance to the research goal, but also by the need to ensure uniformity among types of sources (groups or persons). Social psychology research has extensively demonstrated that although groups and persons are analyzed according to common cognitive mechanisms, their entitativity (meaning the degree to which a social entity is perceived as unique, coherent, and distinct from others) is different (Hamilton et al. 2015). Support sources entitativity may, thus, influence multiple social support perceptions and is a central feature of an MSSA approach. ...

Reference:

Disadvantaged Youths’ Subjective Well-Being: The Role of Gender, Age, and Multiple Social Support Attunement
Convergence and divergence in perceptions of persons and groups.
  • Citing Chapter
  • January 2015

... For adolescents and young adults who have pre-existing NA reduction expectations about cigarette use, initiation of cigarette use is more likely (Wetter et al., 2004) as well as transition from "trier" to "experimental" or "regular-user" (Mayhew et al., 2000). Young adult smokers are an important area of focus, as many adult nicotine dependent smokers initiate cigarette use by late adolescence, potentially due to this developmental life period being characterized by higher levels of NA with less developed internal emotion regulation skills (Chassin et al., 2000;Cole et al., 2004;Kovacs et al., 2006). ...

The Natural History of Cigarette Smoking From Adolescence to Adulthood in a Midwestern Community Sample: Multiple Trajectories and Their Psychosocial Correlates

Health Psychology

... The easier the images of crime occurring are to imagine for the viewer, the more likely they are perceived to occur. Similarly, if images become difficult to imagine, perceived likelihood will decrease (Sherman et al., 2002). Chadee et al. (2007) explains that the degree to which individuals estimate their own likelihood of crime victimization may depend on the availability of similar instances of crime victimization in their local environments that are easy to recall. ...

Imagining Can Heighten or Lower the Perceived Likelihood of Contracting a Disease: The Mediating Effect of Ease of Imagery
  • Citing Chapter
  • July 2002

... The above evidence suggests that metaphoric mappings are flexibly guided by social knowledge. In a related vein, culture has also been shown to be important in determining spatial representations [19,42,43], while recent work demonstrates that concepts can be mapped onto multiple spatial dimensions, e.g. at around 5 years of age, children begin to show a preference for representing temporal events from left-to-right, but also top-to-bottom ( [44,45]; also see [46,47]). We extend this literature by focusing on coherence because this experience is distinct from other previously studied magnitude representations (power, dominance). ...

The Spatial Ingroup Bias: Ingroup Teams Are Positioned Where Writing Starts
  • Citing Article
  • January 2021

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

... no obstante, a pesar de esta aparente centralidad, resulta curioso que el concepto de grupo no se haya sometido a escrutinio en los años recientes. Es cierto que la psicología social cuenta con una importante literatura sobre el tema (Hamilton et al., 1998;McGrath, 1984), pero esta ha tenido escasa resonancia fuera de la subdisciplina. En otras vertientes de las ciencias sociales, la bibliografía reciente sobre el concepto de grupo es exigua, en especial si se la compara con la inmensa cantidad de libros que abordan conceptos como los de clase, identidad, género, etnicidad o multiculturalismo, temáticas que implican el concepto de grupo pero en las que rara vez se lo somete a análisis. ...

Perceiving Social Groups: The Importance of the Entitativity Continuum: Key Readings
  • Citing Chapter
  • December 2004

... The influence of cultural value is necessary to be considered in the study of individual behavior (20)(21)(22). For example, an international tobacco control survey has shown that the difference in quit intentions among smokers was, in part, explained by the cultural value (21). ...

Long-Term Psychological Sequelae of Smoking Cessation and Relapse

Health Psychology

... In the marketplace, consumers might fnd multiple products grouped together when making purchases. In general, consumers recognize whole objects, and not partial ones, when facing stimuli [45]. Hou et al. [46] show that colors can directly infuence consumers' emotional responses and cognitive associations. ...

Perceived Entitativity, Stereotype Formation, and the Interchangeability of Group Members

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

... The major aim of the present work is therefore to assess whether people organize information in memory as a function of the mask-related behaviors of the person perceived. To this end, we employed a specific memory confusion paradigm-often called "who-said-what" task-which has been widely applied to investigate social categorization processes [40][41][42][43] . In the typical task, participants are required to attend to a sequence of speakers each expressing a predefined number of sentences, and afterwards to try to match each sentence with the actual original speaker. ...

The Spontaneous Use of a Group Typology as an Organizing Principle in Memory

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

... Previous work aiming at disentangling the cognitive processes underlying these two heuristics used binary decision tasks to pit one heuristic against the other and showed that representativeness judgments are relatively more complex and take longer to compute than availability (Braga et al., 2018). In these studies participants were asked to predict the following outcome of a coin toss in a sequence. ...

What's Next? Disentangling availability from representativeness using binary decision tasks.
  • Citing Article
  • March 2018

Journal of Experimental Social Psychology

... On this point, previous research addressed similar issues by exploring how the perceived group size of immigrant groups related to perceptions of the immigrant's right to stay in the host country (Zagefka et al., 2020). Similarly, research on dehumanization also shows that numeral minorities are more likely to be judged as less human (Prazienkova et al., 2017). Despite the differences between this previous work and the present study, there are grounds to believe that the perceived group size of poor (vs. ...

On the Cognitive Determinants of Out-Group Dehumanization: Illusory Correlation and the Dehumanization of (Numerical) Group Minorities

Social Cognition