Sam J. Gilbert's research while affiliated with University College London and other places

Publications (51)

Preprint
Full-text available
Background While most research on the non-verbal communication challenges encountered by autistic people centres on visual stimuli, non-verbal vocalizations remains overlooked. Laughter serves as a socio-emotional signal for affiliative bonding in interactions. Autistic people seem to experience and produce laughter differently to non-autistic peop...
Article
Intention offloading involves the use of external reminders such as diaries, to-do lists, and digital alerts to help us remember delayed intentions. Recent studies have provided evidence for various cognitive and metacognitive factors that guide intention offloading, but little research has investigated the physical cost of reminder-setting itself....
Article
Full-text available
Research into prospective memory suggests that older adults may face particular difficulties remembering delayed intentions. One way to mitigate these difficulties is by using external reminders but relatively little is known about age-related differences in such cognitive offloading strategies. We examined younger and older adults' (N = 88) perfor...
Preprint
Intention offloading refers to the use of external reminders to help remember delayed intentions (e.g., setting an alert to help you remember when you need to take your medication). Research has found that metacognitive processes influence offloading such that individual differences in confidence predict individual differences in offloading regardl...
Preprint
Intention offloading involves the use of external reminders such as diaries, to-do lists, and digital alerts to help us remember delayed intentions. Recent studies have provided evidence for various cognitive and metacognitive factors that guide intention offloading, but little research has investigated the physical cost of reminder-setting itself....
Article
Saving information onto external resources can improve memory for subsequent information—a phenomenon known as the saving‐enhanced memory effect. This article reports two preregistered online experiments investigating (A) whether this effect holds when to‐be‐remembered information is presented before the saved information and (B) whether people cho...
Article
Perceivers can use past experiences to make sense of ambiguous sensory signals. However, this may be inappropriate when the world changes and past experiences no longer predict what the future holds. Optimal learning models propose that observers decide whether to stick with or update their predictions by tracking the uncertainty or "precision" of...
Article
Full-text available
Nowadays individuals can readily set reminders to offload intentions onto external resources, such as smartphone alerts, rather than using internal memory. Individuals tend to be biased, setting more reminders than would be optimal. We address the question whether the reminder bias depends on offloading scenarios being framed as either gains or los...
Preprint
Saving information onto external resources can improve memory for subsequent information. This article reports two preregistered online experiments investigating A) whether the saving-enhanced memory effect holds when to-be-remembered information is presented before the saved information and B) whether people typically choose the most advantageous...
Article
Individuals have the option of remembering delayed intentions by storing them in internal memory or offloading them to an external store such as a diary or smartphone alert. How do we route intentions to the appropriate store, and what are the consequences of this? We report three experiments (two preregistered) investigating the role of value. In...
Article
Full-text available
How do we remember delayed intentions? Three decades of research into prospective memory have provided insight into the cognitive and neural mechanisms involved in this form of memory. However, we depend on more than just our brains to remember intentions. We also use external props and tools such as calendars and diaries, strategically placed obje...
Preprint
Research into prospective memory suggests that older adults may face particular difficulties remembering delayed intentions. One way to mitigate these difficulties is by using external reminders but relatively little is known about age-related changes in such cognitive offloading strategies. We examined younger and older adults’ (N=88) performance...
Preprint
Full-text available
Perceivers can use past experiences to make sense of ambiguous sensory signals. However, this may be inappropriate when the world changes and past experiences no longer predict what the future holds. Optimal learning models propose that observers decide whether to stick with or update their predictions by tracking the uncertainty or ‘precision’ of...
Article
Full-text available
Metacognition describes the process of monitoring one's own mental states, often for the purpose of cognitive control. Previous research has investigated how metacognitive signals are generated (metacognitive monitoring), for example, when people (both female/male) judge their confidence in their decisions and memories. Research has also investigat...
Article
Research suggests that individuals with lower working memory have difficulty remembering to fulfil delayed intentions. The current study examined whether the ability to offload intentions onto the environment mitigated these deficits. Participants (N = 268) completed three versions of a delayed intention task with and without the use of reminders,...
Article
Reasoning about underlying causal relations drives responsibility judgments: agents are held responsible for the outcomes they cause through their behaviors. Two main causal reasoning approaches exist: dependence theories emphasize statistical relations between causes and effects, while transference theories emphasize mechanical transmission of ene...
Preprint
How do we remember delayed intentions? Three decades of research into prospective memory have provided insight into the cognitive and neural mechanisms involved in this form of memory. However, we depend on more than just our brains to remember intentions. We also use external props and tools such as calendars and diaries, strategically-placed obje...
Article
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) enables identification of the brain regions and networks underpinning cognitive tasks. It has the potential to significantly advance cognitive design science, but is challenging to apply in design studies and methodological guidance for design researchers is lacking. In this Research Note, we reflect on...
Article
Full-text available
The cognitive load of many everyday life tasks exceeds known limitations of short-term memory. One strategy to compensate for information overload is cognitive offloading which refers to the externalization of cognitive processes such as reminder setting instead of memorizing. There appears to be remarkable variance in offloading behavior between p...
Preprint
Individuals have the option of remembering delayed intentions by storing them in internal memory or offloading them to an external store such as a diary or smartphone alert. How do we route intentions to the appropriate store, and what are the consequences of this? We report three experiments (two pre-registered) investigating the role of value. In...
Preprint
Full-text available
The current study examined whether offloading prospective memory (PM) demands onto the environment through the use of reminders eliminates PM differences typically seen between individuals that have poor or good working memory ability. Over two laboratory sessions scheduled one week apart, participants completed three versions of a PM offloading ta...
Article
Previous research has shown that older adults can have difficulty remembering to fulfill delayed intentions. In the present study, we explored whether age differences in prospective memory are affected when participants are permitted to set reminders to help them remember. Furthermore, we examined whether metacognition can influence the use of such...
Preprint
The cognitive load of many everyday life tasks exceeds known limitations of short-term memory. One strategy to compensate for information overload is cognitive offloading which refers to the externalization of cognitive processes such as reminder setting instead of memorizing. There appears to be remarkable variance in offloading behavior between p...
Article
Full-text available
Individuals often choose between remembering information using their own memory ability versus using external resources to reduce cognitive demand (i.e. ‘cognitive offloading’). For example, to remember a future appointment an individual could choose to set a smartphone reminder or depend on their unaided memory ability. Previous studies investigat...
Article
Full-text available
Setting external reminders provides a convenient way to reduce cognitive demand and ensure accurate retrieval of information for prospective tasks. Recent experimental evidence has demonstrated that the decision to offload cognitive information to external resources is guided by metacognitive belief, i.e. individuals’ confidence in their unaided ab...
Article
People often use external reminders to help remember delayed intentions. This is a form of “cognitive offloading”. Individuals sometimes offload more often than would be optimal (Gilbert et al., 2020). This bias has been linked to participants’ erroneous metacognitive underconfidence in their memory abilities. However, underconfidence is unlikely t...
Preprint
Metacognition describes the process of monitoring one’s own mental states, often for the purpose of cognitive control. Previous research has investigated how metacognitive signals are generated (metacognitive monitoring), for example when people judge their confidence in their decisions and memories. Research has also investigated how these metacog...
Preprint
Previous research has shown that older adults can have difficulty remembering to fulfil delayed intentions. In the present study, we explored whether age differences in prospective memory are affected when participants are permitted to set reminders to help them remember. Furthermore, we examined whether metacognition can influence the use of such...
Preprint
Individuals often choose between remembering information using their own memory ability versus using external resources to reduce cognitive demand (i.e. ‘cognitive offloading’). For example, to remember a future appointment an individual could choose to set a smartphone reminder or depend on their unaided memory ability. Previous studies investigat...
Article
The technological advancement that is rapidly taking place in today’s society allows increased opportunity for “cognitive offloading” by storing information in external devices rather than relying on internal memory. This opens the way to fundamental questions regarding the interplay between internal and external memory and the potential benefits a...
Preprint
Setting external reminders provides a convenient way to reduce cognitive demand and ensure accurate retrieval of information for prospective tasks. Recent experimental evidence has demonstrated that the decision to offload cognitive information to external resources is guided by metacognitive belief, i.e. individuals’ confidence in their unaided ab...
Article
In the absence of a pharmacological cure, finding the most sensitive early cognitive markers of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is becoming increasingly important. In this article we review evidence showing that brain mechanisms of spontaneous, but stimulus-dependent, cognition overlap with key hubs of the default mode network (DMN) that become compromise...
Article
Full-text available
In product design engineering (PDE), ideation involves the generation of technical behaviours and physical structures to address specific functional requirements. This differs from generic creative ideation tasks, which emphasise functional and technical considerations less. To advance knowledge about the neural basis of PDE ideation, we present th...
Article
Full-text available
Background: Cognitive offloading is the use of physical action to reduce the cognitive demands of a task. Everyday memory relies heavily on this practice; for example, when we write down to-be-remembered information or use diaries, alerts, and reminders to trigger delayed intentions. A key goal of recent research has been to investigate the proces...
Preprint
Full-text available
Engaging in a demanding activity while holding in mind another task to be performed in the near future requires the maintenance of information about both the currently-active task set and the intended one. However, little is known about how the human brain implements such action plans. While some previous studies have examined the neural representa...
Article
Full-text available
Engaging in a demanding activity while holding in mind another task to be performed in the near future requires the maintenance of information about both the currently-active task set and the intended one. However, little is known about how the human brain implements such action plans. While some previous studies have examined the neural representa...
Article
Full-text available
Individuals frequently choose between accomplishing goals using unaided cognitive abilities or offloading cognitive demands onto external tools and resources. For example, in order to remember an upcoming appointment one might rely on unaided memory or create a reminder by setting a smartphone alert. Setting a reminder incurs both a cost (the time/...
Preprint
The technological advancement that is rapidly taking place in today’s society allows increased opportunity for “cognitive offloading” by storing information in external devices rather than relying on internal memory. This opens the way to fundamental questions regarding the interplay between internal and external memory and the potential benefits a...
Preprint
Background: Cognitive offloading is the use of physical action to reduce the cognitive demands of a task. Everyday memory relies heavily on this practice, for example when we write down to-be-remembered information or use diaries, alerts, and reminders to trigger delayed intentions. A key goal of recent research has been to investigate the processe...
Article
In event-based prospective memory (PM) paradigms, participants are engaged in an ongoing task (e.g. lexical decision) while maintaining an intention to produce a special response if they encounter pre-defined targets (e.g. animal words). This leads to slowed response times even on nontarget trials, which might be caused by: (A) a periodic or interm...
Article
Full-text available
When we produce actions we predict their likely consequences. Dominant models of action control suggest that these predictions are used to ‘cancel’ perceptual processing of expected outcomes. However, normative Bayesian models of sensory cognition developed outside of action propose that rather than being cancelled, expected sensory signals are rep...
Preprint
Individuals frequently choose between accomplishing goals using unaided cognitive abilities or offloading cognitive demands onto external tools and resources. For example, in order to remember an upcoming appointment one might rely on unaided memory or create a reminder by setting a smartphone alert. Setting a reminder incurs both a cost (the time/...
Conference Paper
Full-text available
Brain-computer interfaces (BCIs) augment traditional interfaces for human-computer interaction and provide alternative communication devices to enable the physically impaired to work. Imagined object/shape classification from electroencephalography (EEG) may lead, for example, to enhanced tools for fields such as engineering, design, and the visual...
Article
Full-text available
Background: After stroke, the learned non-use of a paretic arm is a major obstacle to the improvement of hand function. Objective: We examined whether patients with a central paresis could profit from applying the self-regulation strategy of making if-then plans that specify situational triggers to using the paretic arm. Method: Seventeen stro...
Article
Full-text available
Introduction The generation of creative visual imagery contributes to technological and scientific innovation and production of visual art. The underlying cognitive and neural processes are, however, poorly understood. Methods This review synthesizes functional neuroimaging studies of visual creativity. Seven functional magnetic resonance imaging...

Citations

... Such compensation strategies may be especially useful in naturalistic PM tasks (Maylor, 2018;Tomaszewski Farias et al., 2018;Rummel et al., 2019), and thereby also possibly contribute to the age-prospective memory paradox, as older adults might be more accustomed to using them than younger adults (Aronov et al., 2015). Indeed, when they are given the opportunity older adults seem to rely more on intention offloading, the use of physical reminders to reduce the cognitive demand posed by the intention that has to be kept in mind, than younger adults: Older adults set more reminders for future tasks, although not to the extent that it fully compensates for their decline in prospective memory (Scarampi and Gilbert, 2021;Tsai et al., 2022). Still, the use of compensation strategies in general has been demonstrated to be positively related to higher levels of functioning in daily life in older adults (Tomaszewski Farias et al., 2018). ...
... They also examined how such offloading impacts subsequent memory, either with or without access to the external resource. Tsai et al. (2023) explored some telling boundary conditions for the saving enhanced memory effect, which is the finding that saving an initial set of information can enhance the subsequent encoding and recall of a second set of information (Storm & Stone, 2015). Tsai et al.'s studies provide two novel insights into the phenomenon: first, that saving enhanced memory seems to follow similar cognitive mechanisms to the well-studied process of list method directed forgetting. ...
... Based on univariate ERP approaches, they showed mixed findings (Bäß et al., 2008;Hughes & Waszak, 2011;Martikainen et al., 2005;Mifsud et al., 2016). Using MVPA, a few recent studies reported enhanced perception of stimuli induced by active movements (e.g., Yon et al., 2018Yon et al., , 2023, but no literature has directly used MVPA to examine motor-preparatory EEGs. Thus, due to a lack of directly comparable previous literature, we refrain from hypothesising the direction of this comparison. ...
... Some smartphone applications may help in decreasing cognitive load through certain tools and applications (i.e., writing down tasks in a list using a smartphone, smartphone calendar reminders for appointments, registering contacts' phone numbers) [95], allowing for said cognitive space to be used for other purposes. In one study, participants were able to more accurately complete a task by using smartphones to help record and remember parts of the task [105], but when smartphones were taken away in a subsequent task, participants fared worse than when they had not depended on a smartphone originally. Smartphones can also have specific applications geared toward training better cognitive function effectively, especially for the elderly [106]. ...
... Other latest work is related with (i) cognitive offloading is useful for older adults (Burnett & Richmond, 2023) (ii) full offloading is more useful than partial offloading . Not only this, framing is key to offloading behaviour (Fröscher et al., 2022). Sometimes aging can also be associated with reduced choice for offloading behaviour (Tsai et al., 2022). ...
... can be particularly burdensome when these thoughts become ruminative (Beaty et al., 2019;Miranda et al., 2017Miranda et al., , 2023. To reduce these demands while improving goal completion, intentions can be offloaded onto the environment (e.g., setting an electronic calendar reminder; for reviews, see Risko & Gilbert, 2016;Gilbert et al., 2023). But what becomes of the fate of these offloaded memory representations during the retention interval? ...
... A follow-up study by Tsai et al. (2022) used the optimal reminders paradigm introduced by Gilbert et al. (2020) to investigate the bias towards or away from reminders, relative to the optimal strategy (see Optimality of intention offloading above). Older adults performed significantly worse when using unaided memory. ...
... However, while predictive cancellation has seemed foundational to successful theories of action control and awareness, research in recent years has begun to undermine the idea that predicted action outcomes really are cancelled. An emerging body of work has begun to show thatcontrary to classic ideasexpected action outcomes may be perceptually and neurally enhanced Dogge, Custers, Gayet, et al., 2019;Guo & Song, 2019;Paraskevoudi & SanMiguel, 2021;Reznik et al., 2014Reznik et al., , 2021Reznik & Mukamel, 2019;Thomas et al., 2022;Yon et al., 2018Yon et al., , 2021Yon et al., , 2022; see also Hudson et al., 2015Hudson et al., , 2018. These findings accord with a general picture of perceptual prediction described by Bayesian models which stress that it is adaptive for agents to bias their perceptual inferences towards what they expect, perhaps via increasing the gain on expected sensory channels (Bar, 2004;Yuille & Kersten, 2006;Press et al., 2020a). ...
... Adding to the complexity of the summarized results, other studies have shown that individual differences in memory can also affect offloading behavior. It was demonstrated that people with a lower working memory capacity choose to offload more information (Ball et al., 2022;Gilbert, 2015;Meyerhoff et al., 2021;Risko & Dunn, 2015; but see also Morrison & Richmond, 2020). ...
... These results are consistent with recent studies that showed that making a system more "explainable", i.e., making it less opaque (via a bonus or confidence in its decision), benefits to the SoA of participants interacting with it 13 . Our results further suggest that the confidence communicated by the assistance system can be used by the participant as a metacognitive signal to adjust the amount of resources to invest in the current task (metacognitive control 40 ). ...