Ronald Hübner’s research while affiliated with University of Konstanz and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (82)


Traced line drawings of female bodies from Singh¹⁵. The bodies are from the normal weight category (N). Bodies from the WHR category 8, 9, and 10 are superposed with body N7, respectively, to demonstrate how the WHR was varied (see text for details).
Examples of Singh’s line drawings. In the center column are the normal weight stimuli with a WHR of 0.7 (N7) and 0.9 (N9), respectively. Left and right are the corresponding underweight and overweight stimuli in red. For comparison, the stimuli are drawn on top of N7 and N9 bodies, respectively (U: underweight, N: normal weight, O: overweight).
Results of Experiment 1. The average attractiveness ratings are provided as numbers at the top of each panel and are also coded by the background color. The heatmap goes from blue (48) to red (74).
The 25 body stimuli used in Experiment 2, with the number at the bottom indicating the respective WHR. Bodies with a dashed frame have a WHR that is closest to 0.7 for the corresponding width.
The five left-sided (viewer perspective) body contour lines and their curvature ‘comb’⁴⁶. The curvature along the lines is indicated not only by the (scaled) length of the comb teeth, but also by their color according to a heat map.

+2

Curviness is a better predictor of a woman’s body attractiveness than the waist-to-hip ratio
  • Article
  • Full-text available

October 2024

·

304 Reads

Ronald Hübner

·

The waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) is commonly used as an indicator of mid-body fat distribution and is often used to answer health-related questions. It has also been suggested that a woman’s WHR can signal her reproductive fitness. This notion is supported by evidence indicating a relation between WHR and a woman’s physical attractiveness. However, it was also acknowledged that the actual fitness cue is the curviness of a woman’s body. While curviness is easy to perceive, it is difficult to quantify. Therefore, the WHR is often considered as a simple measure of body curviness. However, the WHR and curviness are not uniquely related. After replicating results of a pioneering study in this area, we therefore tested whether the WHR or curviness better predicts a woman’s physical attractiveness. As stimuli, we used simple line drawings of women’s bodies, differing in their curviness and width. The results demonstrate that curviness is a better predictor, even though we used a relatively simple curvature-based measure of curviness. This outcome indicates that the WHR is a poor measure of a woman’s body curviness and underscores the need for a more accurate measure of curviness when assessing the physical attractiveness of a woman’s body.

Download

The effect of expertise on the creation and evaluation of visual compositions in terms of creativity and beauty

June 2024

·

66 Reads

·

1 Citation

The identification of artistically creative individuals is an important matter in the fields of art, design, and psychology. One promising approach involves assessing a person's products rather than his or her personality or cognitive processes. However, the necessity of expert involvement in such evaluations is still debated. To investigate this issue, two experiments were conducted, each consisting of a production phase and an evaluation phase. In Experiment 1, participants were asked to compose a most beautiful picture, which was subsequently assessed in terms of beauty. Experiment 2 was analogous, but participants were asked to compose a most creative picture, which was then assessed in terms of creativity and beauty. The results revealed that expertise did not play a crucial role in the creation or evaluation of beauty. Both experts and non-experts largely agreed on what constitutes beauty. However, when it came to the production and assessment of creative pictures, experts had an advantage. They were the only group that was able to predict a person's creativity based on the evaluation of his or her product.


Golden spiral or Fibonacci spiral: Which is more beautiful and why?

April 2024

·

22 Reads

i-Perception

Spirals, with their widespread presence in both nature and culture, are universally admired. Although there are different types, such as the Archimedean, logarithmic, and golden spirals, they are indiscriminately considered as beautiful. This universal view might explain the lack of studies investigating aesthetic differences among spirals. To show that there are indeed differences, the beauty of the golden spiral was compared with that of the Fibonacci spiral in this empirical study. Since the curvature of the golden spiral changes continuously, whereas that of the Fibonacci spiral does so in steps, the golden spiral was predicted to be aesthetically preferred. The results clearly confirmed this prediction. That the difference in preference was really based on the continuity versus discontinuity of the curvature is supported by the further result that an Archimedean spiral was preferred over a Dürer spiral, which similarly differed in their continuity.


On the beauty of vases: Birkhoff’s aesthetic measure versus Hogarth’s line of beauty

April 2023

·

180 Reads

·

1 Citation

Vases continue to be important aesthetic objects in almost all developed cultures. Nevertheless, there is little to no systematic research on the shape characteristics that determine their beauty. A famous exception is Birkhoff, who in his 1933 book used the geometric ratios of vases to calculate their beauty. One form factor that he discussed theoretically but did not include in his aesthetic measure is the outline curvature of vases. This is despite the fact that William Hogarth recognized curvature as relevant to the aesthetic evaluation of forms as early as 1753, demonstrating this with his Line of Beauty. Given the great influence of these two ideas, the aim of the present study was to examine their contribution to the aesthetics of vases. For this objective, we designed a set of symbolic vases by systematically varying width and curvature, and asked participants to rate their beauty in an online experiment. The results show that both geometric ratios and curvature contribute to the beauty of the vases.


Evidence that the aesthetic preference for Hogarth’s Line of Beauty is an evolutionary by-product

March 2023

·

635 Reads

·

3 Citations

Ronald Hübner

·

·

·

[...]

·

In 1753, artist William Hogarth declared a specific S-shaped line to be the ‘Line of Beauty’ (LoB). Hogarth’s assertion has had a profound impact on diverse fields over the past two and a half centuries. However, only one recent (2022) study has investigated whether Hogarth’s assertion accurately captures humans’ actual aesthetic preferences, and no research has explored why people find the LoB beautiful. We conducted two studies testing the hypothesis that the LoB’s perceived beauty is an incidental by-product of cognitive systems that evolved to attend to fitness-relevant morphological features in people. In Study 1, we replicated the finding that female bodies whose lumbar curvature approximates the biomechanical optimum for dealing with the exigencies of pregnancy are rated as more attractive. In Study 2, we found that abstract lines extracted from these bodies were perceived as more beautiful than other lines. These results suggest that the preference for Hogarth’s LoB is an incidental by-product of psychological mechanisms that evolved for other purposes. More broadly, these findings suggest that an evolutionary psychological approach – in particular the concept of evolutionary by-product – may be useful for understanding, explaining, and predicting people’s aesthetic preferences for certain abstract symbols, which otherwise might seem arbitrary and inexplicable.


Learned cognitive control counteracts value-driven attentional capture

February 2023

·

70 Reads

Psychological Research

Stimuli formerly associated with monetary reward capture our attention, even if this attraction is contrary to current goals (so-called value-driven attentional capture [VDAC], see Anderson (Ann N Y Acad Sci 1369:24–39, 2016), for a review). Despite the growing literature to this topic, little is known about the boundary conditions for the occurrence of VDAC. In three experiments, we investigated the role of response conflicts and spatial uncertainty regarding the target location during the training and test phase for the emergence of value-driven effects. Thus, we varied the occurrence of a response conflict, search components, and the type of task in both phases. In the training, value-driven effects were rather observed if the location of the value-associated target was not predictable and a response conflict was present. Value-driven effects also only occurred, if participants have not learned to deal with a response conflict, yet. However, the introduction of a response conflict during learning of the color-value association seemed to prevent attention to be distracted by this feature in a subsequent test. The study provides new insights not only into the boundary conditions of the learning of value associations, but also into the learning of cognitive control.


Position biases in sequential location selection: Effects of region, choice history, and visibility of previous selections

October 2022

·

385 Reads

·

2 Citations

In some situations, e.g., when filling out lottery tickets, it can be advantageous to select random locations. However, people usually have difficulties with this, because they are biased by preferences for certain regions, such as the center of an area. According to ideas from art theory, the preferred regions reflect the hidden structure of perceptual forces within an area. In the present study, these structures were investigated and modeled under different conditions for areas with square and rectangular shape. The general task was to sequentially place a number of dots at random locations in an area by clicking with the computer mouse at corresponding positions on the screen. Whereas in a single-dot condition each dot had to be placed in an empty area, the previously placed dots remained visible in a multiple-dots condition. In three experiments it was found that dots were preferentially placed at the center, the diagonals, and the principal axes. This preference was more pronounced in the single than in the multiple-dot condition. Moreover, sequential analyses revealed that dot placing was not only planned in advance, but that the participants also agreed to some extent in their sequential selections, which produced surprisingly similar sequential spatial patterns across participants, at least for the first dots. Altogether, the results indicate that people have great difficulties with the random selection of locations. Their selections are strongly affected by the attraction of specific regions, by previous selections, and by sequential habits.


Result of the Multiple Regression Analysis. The Coefficients are Defined in Equation (1). * p < .05, * * p < .01.
Is Hogarth’s ‘Line of Beauty’ really the most beautiful? An empirical answer after more than 250 years

March 2022

·

578 Reads

·

7 Citations

i-Perception

Since the Renaissance, different line types have been distinguished by artists and art theorists. However, it took another hundreds of years until the British artist William Hogarth attributed different degrees of beauty to them. Particularly, in his book “The Analysis of Beauty” (1753) he depicted seven waving lines, declared line number 4 as the most beautiful, and called it the “line of beauty”. Until today, the line of beauty has a persistently strong influence in many areas such as landscape art and design, calligraphy, furniture design, architecture, dance, etc. It is astonishing that Hogarth's assumptions have never been empirically tested. Therefore, we asked participants to rate Hogarth's lines by their beauty. As a result, line number 4 was indeed the most preferred, although number 5 was judged similarly. An analysis revealed that curvature was nonlinearly related to beauty and explains more than 90% of the variance in the mean aesthetic judgments.


On the Difficulty of Overcoming One’s Accuracy Bias for Choosing an Optimal Speed–Accuracy Tradeoff

October 2021

·

31 Reads

·

2 Citations

Under time pressure, it is usually not possible to respond quickly and accurately at the same time. Therefore, people must trade speed for accuracy, depending on the current payoff conditions. Ideally, they should choose a speed-accuracy tradeoff (SAT) that optimizes their monetary reward. However, this is hardly the case. Rather, persons exhibit an accuracy bias, which is often disadvantageous. To further investigate the role of errors for optimizing reward, we conducted a flanker-task study with different payoff and framing conditions. Whereas the reward for correct responses always increased continuously with speed, the costs of errors varied. In three of four conditions, responding very fast, even with low accuracy, was favorable. Furthermore, in addition to the usual gain framing, half of our participants were instructed according to a loss frame. Whereas framing had little effect on performance, we found a substantial accuracy bias. Only in the most extreme condition some participants overcame their bias and responded very quickly. To examine how SAT strategies differed between participants, we modeled the performance with a sequential-sampling model. The results suggest that various mechanisms were involved in realizing specific SATs. However, they were hardly applied to optimize reward. Rather, participants seem to have optimized their well-being. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Illustration of the congruent (A) and incongruent (B) condition in the Simon search task, as well as an example trial sequence in Experiment 1 (C).
Experiment 1. The left panel displays the RTs and error rates for the different congruency and distractor conditions. The right panel displays differences in the Simon congruency effects for respective pairs of distractor conditions. Displayed are only data from trials in which the deadline was met. Error bars correspond to the within-subject confidence intervals (Morey, 2008). *Significant on p < 0.05, **significant on p < 0.01, ***significant on p < 0.001, n.s. = not significant.
RTs averaged across congruent and incongruent conditions for successive blocks in Experiment 1. The graph in the upper panel shows the complete data, irrespective of timeout errors. The graph in the lower panel displays only data in which the deadline was met. For clarity, error bars are not included.
RTs averaged across congruent and incongruent conditions for successive blocks in Experiment 2. The graph in the upper panel shows the complete data, irrespective of timeout errors. The graph in the lower panel displays only data, where the deadline was met. For clarity, error bars are not included.
Experiment 2. The left panel displays the RTs and error rates for the different congruency and distractor conditions. The right panel displays differences in the Simon congruency effects for respective pairs of distractor conditions. Displayed are only data from trials in which the deadline was met. Error bars correspond to the within-subject confidence intervals (Morey, 2008). *Significant on p < 0.05, **significant on p < 0.01, ***significant on p < 0.001, n.s. = not significant.
Value Associations Modulate Visual Attention and Response Selection

May 2021

·

79 Reads

·

2 Citations

Every day, we are confronted with a vast amount of information that all competes for our attention. Some of this information might be associated with rewards (e.g., gambling) or losses (e.g., insurances). To what extent such information, even if irrelevant for our current task, not only attracts attention but also affects our actions is still a topic under examination. To address this issue, we applied a new experimental paradigm that combines visual search and a spatial compatibility task. Although colored stimuli did not modulate the spatial compatibility effect more than gray stimuli, we found clear evidence that reward and loss associations attenuated this effect, presumably by affecting attention and response selection. Moreover, there are hints that differences in these associations are also reflected in a modulation of the spatial compatibility effect. We discuss theoretical implications of our results with respect to the influences of color, reward, and loss association on selective attention and response selection.


Citations (75)


... Although taste is inherent, experience and training are necessary to make 'accurate' judgments. Many recent studies have compared the judgments of experts and non-experts in the arts to understand the possible differential effect of art affinity on aesthetic appreciation (e.g., Era, Candidi, & Aglioti, 2019;Leder, Gerger, Brieber, & Schwarz, 2014;Locher, Smith, & Smith, 2001;Mutter & Hübner, 2024). Some findings showed that experts' evaluations could be more sensitive to the structure of the object (Locher et al., 2001), milder, with less extreme ratings compared to non-experts as well as with much likelihood of seeing beauty in seemingly 'negative' artworks , and may involve a more enriching cognitive process compared to "gut reaction" (Graf & Landwehr, 2015). ...

Reference:

Exploring the Dynamics of Aesthetic Judgments: The Interplay of Subjective and Objective Elements Beyond Socio-economic Divides
The effect of expertise on the creation and evaluation of visual compositions in terms of creativity and beauty

... Our hypothesis was also motivated by results of a recent study on the beauty of vases 40 , in which we dealt with a similar problem. In his book "Aesthetic measure", Birkhoff 41 attributed the beauty of S-shaped vases to some of their proportions, one of which resembles the WHR. ...

On the beauty of vases: Birkhoff’s aesthetic measure versus Hogarth’s line of beauty

... However, he was referring more to the back profile, which also has an S-shaped outline 33 . In a recent study, it was indeed shown that the back profile of a physically attractive woman is highly similar to Hogarth's Line of Beauty 34 . ...

Evidence that the aesthetic preference for Hogarth’s Line of Beauty is an evolutionary by-product

... The center is followed by its corners, the horizontal and vertical axis and then the diagonal axes. According to this model, every element placed in an image is pulled by all the invisible forces stemming from the pixelwise structure and, additionally, by all other pictorial elements in the image, thus creating an inner tension (Hübner, 2022;Hübner & Fillinger, 2016;McManus et al., 2011). ...

Position biases in sequential location selection: Effects of region, choice history, and visibility of previous selections

... Slight deviations from this symmetry result in reduced preference. Previous research mainly focused on curved lines taken from Hogarth's line of beauty (Hogarth, 1753;Hübner & Ufken, 2022), which examines variations in seven categories of S-shaped curves. Our work extends beyond this finding and demonstrates that curves pointing in the same direction (e.g., difference of 100) are equally, or even more preferred than S-shaped curves. ...

Is Hogarth’s ‘Line of Beauty’ really the most beautiful? An empirical answer after more than 250 years

i-Perception

... Nevertheless, the accuracy in the 135° rotated faces still tended to be higher for incongruent trials (93.39%) compared to congruent trials (92.55%), although it did not reach statistical significance. This pattern of results provided speed-accuracy trade-off evidence between congruent and incongruent conditions at the 135° face orientation (Hübner, Druey, Pelzer, & Walle, 2021;Liesefeld, Fu, & Zimmer, 2015). ...

On the Difficulty of Overcoming One’s Accuracy Bias for Choosing an Optimal Speed–Accuracy Tradeoff

... The modulation of synaptic plasticity in these devices follows the learning principles of the human brain. Associative learning is a learning principle in which ideas and experiences reinforce one another, which is critical to individuals during the extraction of the world logical structure for quick adaptation to the environment [11][12][13][14]. Classical associative learning is described by the Pavlovian dog, which starts to salivate to the ringing of a bell after a training process of feeding (unconditioned stimulus) and ringing a bell (neutral stimulus). ...

Value Associations Modulate Visual Attention and Response Selection

... Collection of such object properties, which enable the viewer to recognize different expressions of that object, is called expressiveness. For example, sharp angular lines can have an impression of danger; diagonal lines can induce dynamics; arrangement of elements in a gravitationally stable way induces perceived stability of the composition (Fillinger & Hübner, 2020). Therefore, expressiveness is a wider concept than expression, since it reflects not only spontaneous manifestation of mental states, but all characteristics which can lead to certain impressions in the viewer. ...

On the relation between perceived stability and aesthetic appreciation
  • Citing Article
  • July 2020

Acta Psychologica

... To ensure which model can adequately explain qualitative trends in the human data, we provided an intuitive assessment of the fit of three conflict DDMs (DMC, DSTP, SSP) using cumulative distribution function (CDF) plots and conditional accuracy function (CAF) plots (Hübner & Pelzer, 2020). CDF plots can be constructed by specifying specific percentiles of correct trials for human data and predicted data under different conditions, represented by five CDF percentiles (0.1, 0.3, 0.5, 0.7, 0.9). ...

Improving parameter recovery for conflict drift-diffusion models

Behavior Research Methods

... or 3 loss cards) presented to participants within each trial. This allows researchers to gain a deeper understanding of participants' considerations when making decisions under conditions of known risk (Haffke & Hübner, 2020;Kluwe-Schiavon et al., 2020). The CCT has not been as extensively utilized in research compared to the IGT and BART (Buelow, 2015), although recent studies have demonstrated that cocaine-dependent users (Kluwe-Schiavon et al., 2020), VMPFC lesion patients (Spaniol et al., 2019), and sleep-deprived individuals (Salfi et al., 2020) all displayed significantly greater risk propensity on the CCT compared to control counterparts. ...

Are choices based on conditional or conjunctive probabilities in a sequential risk‐taking task?

Journal of Behavioral Decision Making