Robert Meksin’s research while affiliated with New School and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (13)


Durability of Retrieval-Induced Forgetting: Effects of Different Practice Schedules
  • Article
  • Full-text available

September 2024

·

84 Reads

Memory & Cognition

·

Martin Fagin

·

Robert Meksin

·

If retrieval-induced forgetting (RIF) is to play a role in the formation of collective memories, it should be long lasting. Although several studies have found that RIF is short-lived, there is other evidence to suggest that repeated selective practice schedules with a temporal gap between each practice trial may increase the durability of RIF. We tested this possibility in three experiments, focusing on socially shared retrieval-induced forgetting (SSRIF). In two experiments, participants studied scientific or story materials, then listened to someone selectively recall the material repeatedly, either in rapid succession or over an extended time period, and finally recalled the original materials either immediately, after a 1-week delay, or after a 3-week delay. A third experiment examined selective practice in free-flowing conversations. In each instance, RIF was found with repeated selective practice with a temporal gap between trials. The results are discussed in terms of the role RIF might play in the formation of collective memory.

Download

Overestimating the intensity of negative feelings in autobiographical memory: evidence from the 9/11 attack and COVID-19 pandemic

May 2024

·

49 Reads

·

1 Citation

·

·

Olivia T Karaman

·

[...]

·

Elizabeth A Phelps

When recalling autobiographical events, people not only retrieve event details but also the feelings they experienced. The current study examined whether people are able to consistently recall the intensity of past feelings associated with two consequential and negatively valenced events, i.e. the 9/11 attack (N = 769) and the COVID-19 pandemic (N = 726). By comparing experienced and recalled intensities of negative feelings, we discovered that people systematically recall a higher intensity of negative feelings than initially reported - overestimating the intensity of past negative emotional experiences. The COVID-19 dataset also revealed that individuals who experienced greater improvement in emotional well-being displayed smaller biases in recalling their feelings. Across both datasets, the intensity of remembered feelings was correlated with initial feelings and current feelings, but the impact of the current feelings was stronger in the COVID-19 dataset than in the 9/11 dataset. Our results demonstrate that when recalling negative autobiographical events, people tend to overestimate the intensity of prior negative emotional experiences with their degree of bias influenced by current feelings and well-being.


Emotion Language Use in Narratives of the 9/11 Attacks Predicts Long-Term Memory

Emotion

Despite considerable cognitive neuroscience research demonstrating that emotions can influence the encoding and consolidation of memory, research has failed to demonstrate a relationship between self-reported ratings of emotions collected soon after a traumatic event and memory for the event over time. This secondary analysis of data from a multisite longitudinal study of memories of the September 11, 2001 terrorist attacks, asked the question of whether emotional language use could predict memory over time. In the 2 weeks following the 9/11 attacks, participants (N = 691; Mage = 36.8; 72% identifying as male; 76% identifying as white) wrote narratives about how they learned of the attacks and the impact of the attacks on them. Language features of these narratives were extracted using the Linguistic Inquiry Word Count program and used to predict three types of memory: (a) event memory accuracy, (b) flashbulb memory consistency, and (c) emotion memory consistency. These outcomes were assessed at the time of writing, 1, 3, and 10 years after the 9/11 attacks. Results of linear mixed-effects models indicate that greater use of negative emotion words in narratives predicts better event memory accuracy 3 and 10 years after the attacks and worse flashbulb memory consistency 10 years after the attacks. However, emotion word use does not predict emotion memory consistency across time. We also examine whether other exploratory linguistic predictors are associated with memory over time. These findings suggest that written language may serve as a potential early indicator of memory over time.


Figure 1. Relationship between previous, current, and remembered negative feelings in the (a) 9/11 and (b) Covid-19 datasets. At the 1-year follow-up survey, people reported currently experiencing less negative feelings while recalling higher levels of negative feelings than what they originally reported. Previous = T1 experienced; current = T2 experienced; remembered =
Figure 2. Influences of previous and current feelings on remembered feelings probed after 1 year. In 9/11 (a,b) and Covid-19 (c,d) datasets, negative feelings and stress experienced at T1 and T2 (1-year follow-up) predict remembered negative feelings at T2, respectively. In 9/11 dataset (a,b), the regression coefficients for T1 and T2 negative feelings did not differ in magnitude Shaded areas indicate 95% credible interval of the posterior estimate of the
Figure 3. Deviation of remembered negative feelings from experienced negative feelings concerning the 9/11 attack after 1, 3, and 10 years. A positive memory deviation index indicates the overestimation of previous negative emotional experience. Deviation peaked during the 3-year follow-up survey. Error bars indicate ±1 standard error.
Fig 4. Association between improvements in emotional well-being and deviation in memory of Covid-19 related stress. A positive memory deviation index indicates an overestimation of previous negative emotional experience. People with higher emotional well-being at the 1-year follow-up have smaller deviations in memory.
Overestimating the intensity of negative feelings in autobiographical memory: evidence from the 9/11 attack and Covid-19 pandemic

December 2022

·

106 Reads

·

1 Citation

When recalling autobiographical events, people retrieve not only the event details, but also the feelings they experienced. Past work with different measures of memories for feelings remain inconclusive, suggesting that people are either highly consistent or inconsistent with remembering feelings. The current study examined whether people are able to consistently recall the intensity of previous feelings associated with consequential and negatively valenced emotional events, i.e., the 9/11 attack (N = 769) and Covid-19 pandemic (N = 726). By comparing the initial and recalled intensities of negative feelings, we found that people systematically recall more intense negative feelings than they initially reported – overestimating the intensity of past negative emotional experience. The Covid-19 dataset further showed that people whose emotional well-being improved more demonstrate smaller biases in remembered feelings. Across both datasets, the remembered intensity of feelings correlated with initial feelings and were also influenced by current feelings, although the impact of the current feelings was stronger in the Covid-19 dataset than the 9/11 dataset. Our results suggest that when recalling negative autobiographical events, people tend to overestimate the intensity of experienced negative emotional experience with the degree of bias influenced by current feelings and well-being.


Emotion Language Use in Narratives of the 9/11 Attacks Predicts Long-Term Memory

October 2022

·

88 Reads

Despite considerable cognitive neuroscience research demonstrating that emotions can influence the encoding and consolidation of memory, research has failed to demonstrate a relationship between self-reported ratings of emotions collected soon after a traumatic event and memory for the event over time. This secondary analysis of data from a multi-site longitudinal study of memories of the 9/11 terrorist attacks, asked the question of whether emotional language use could predict memory over time. In the two weeks following the 9/11 attacks, participants (N = 691) wrote narratives about how they learned of the attacks and the impact of the attacks on them. Language features of these narratives were extracted using the Linguistic Inquiry Word Count program and used to predict three types of memory: (1) flashbulb memory consistency, (2) event memory accuracy, and (3) emotion memory consistency. These outcomes were assessed at the time of writing, one, three, and 10 years after the 9/11 attacks. Least absolute shrinkage and selection operator regressions were used to narrow down language predictors examined. Results of linear mixed-effects models indicate that greater use of emotion words in narratives predicts decreased flashbulb memory consistency, event memory accuracy, and emotion memory consistency over time. Similarly, the use of cognitive processing words predicted decreased event memory accuracy, and emotional memory consistency. We also report other exploratory linguistic predictors associated with memory over time. These findings suggest that assessing language usage may serve as a sensitive means of measuring emotion for predicting its influence on memory over time.


Figure 1. Changes in the content of retelling over time (word-based analysis). (A) Correspondence analysis map of Words (not plotted) and Timepoint (red dots) along the first and second dimensions, with confidence ellipses (grey circles). (B) Plot of specificity scores of Words by Timepoint, for the 7 words statistically significant at one timepoint or more. (C) Correspondence analysis map of Details (not plotted) and Timepoint (red dots) along the first and second dimensions, with confidence ellipses (grey circles). (D) Plot of specificity scores of Details by Timepoint. Red line: significance threshold.
Figure 2. Influence of intensity of emotions felt upon hearing the attack on report for details. Plot of specificity scores of Details by (Timepoint *Emotionintensity). Red line: significance threshold.
Occurrence of words and details by timepoint.
Changes over 10 years in the retelling of the flashbulb memories of the attack of 11 September 2001

July 2021

·

157 Reads

·

12 Citations

Memory

A flashbulb memory is a highly detailed and vivid autobiographical memory for the circumstances in which one first learned of a surprising, consequential and emotionally arousing event. How retelling of different features of a flashbulb memory changes over time is not totally understood. Moreover, little is known about how the emotional feeling experienced by individuals when they learned about the event modulates these changes. In this study, we explored changes over time in American individuals’ retelling of their flashbulb memories of the terrorist attack of 11 September 2001. We conducted textual analysis of 824 testimonies collected from the same 206 individuals 1 week, 11, 25 and again 119 months after the attack. Results showed individuals were more likely to report temporal and emotional details in their retelling early after the event and spatial details in their long-term retelling. In addition, the intensity of emotions felt upon hearing the news about the attack influenced how individuals reported their flashbulb memories over time. Overall, this study provides further support for theories suggesting different rates of forgetting for different canonical features of emotional arousal events.


The Mnemonic Consequences of Jurors’ Selective Retrieval During Deliberation

June 2019

·

94 Reads

·

4 Citations

Topics in Cognitive Science

The jury is a defining component of the American criminal justice system, and the courts largely assume that the collaborative nature of jury deliberations will enhance jurors’ memory for important trial information. However, research suggests that this kind of collaboration, although sometimes improving memory, can also lead to incomplete and inaccurate “collective” memories. The present research examines whether jury deliberations, where individuals collaboratively recall and discuss trial evidence to render unanimous verdicts, might shape jurors’ memories through the robust phenomena of Within‐Individual and Socially Shared Retrieval‐Induced Forgetting (WI‐RIF and SS‐RIF, respectively). The results revealed no WI‐RIF or SS‐RIF. However, we did find evidence in the direction of Within‐Individual and Socially‐shared Retrieval Induced Facilitation (WI‐RIFA and SS‐RIFA, respectively) in speakers’ and listeners’ narrative and open‐ended recall of evidentiary details. The present results are discussed in terms of whether jurors’ goals during deliberation and the deliberation structure (e.g., six or more discussants) protect against forgetting, or whether possible methodological issues (e.g., the vast amount of information presented) eliminated WI‐RIF and SS‐RIF and, in turn, make drawing conclusions surrounding the mnemonic impact of jury deliberation difficult. Regardless, the present results suggest jury deliberations are quite limited in terms of how much evidence is actually discussed compared to the total of what could be discussed, and our methodology provides an ecologically valid baseline for future research to better understand the mnemonic consequences associated with jury deliberations and, in turn, jury decision making.



Temporal evolution of memory for the attack of September 11, 2001: Lexical analysis of flashbulb memories through 10-year follow-up

June 2017

·

191 Reads

Flashbulb memories related to negative arousing event inform on traumatic memory processes, as well as trauma related disorder such as Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder. These memories are not immutable photographs of the past experience, and there is now a general consensus about their retention over time. However, little is known on whether contextual and emotional aspects of memories change over the long-term. This longitudinal study aims to directly look at the textual content of testimonies provided by 206 participants at four times from 1 week to 10 years after the attacks of September 11, 2001. Our results suggest progressive changes and reconstruction in flashbulb memories over time. Memory for temporal and emotional details decreased, whereas memory for spatial details increased. We observed that intensity of the emotional response at the time of the event modulates the content of flashbulb memories and their change over time. As described for autobiographical memories, heightened emotional arousal during the event leads to attention focus on emotional salient stimuli increasing their recall one week after the attacks. Beyond the theoretical aspects of flash memory and the traumatic memory generated by this study, we show that the textual analysis of testimonies allows the subtle exploration of the dynamic mnemonic processes.


Figure 1. 
Figure 2. 
Figure 3. 
Figure 4. 
Highly Accurate Prediction of Emotions Surrounding the Attacks of September 11, 2001 Over 1-, 2-, and 7-Year Prediction Intervals

April 2016

·

148 Reads

·

11 Citations

Journal of Experimental Psychology General

In the aftermath of a national tragedy, important decisions are predicated on judgments of the emotional significance of the tragedy in the present and future. Research in affective forecasting has largely focused on ways in which people fail to make accurate predictions about the nature and duration of feelings experienced in the aftermath of an event. Here we ask a related but understudied question: can people forecast how they will feel in the future about a tragic event that has already occurred? We found that people were strikingly accurate when predicting how they would feel about the September 11 attacks over 1-, 2-, and 7-year prediction intervals. Although people slightly under- or overestimated their future feelings at times, they nonetheless showed high accuracy in forecasting (a) the overall intensity of their future negative emotion, and (b) the relative degree of different types of negative emotion (i.e., sadness, fear, or anger). Using a path model, we found that the relationship between forecasted and actual future emotion was partially mediated by current emotion and remembered emotion. These results extend theories of affective forecasting by showing that emotional responses to an event of ongoing national significance can be predicted with high accuracy, and by identifying current and remembered feelings as independent sources of this accuracy. (PsycINFO Database Record


Citations (8)


... Currently, there is relatively little existing evidence regarding the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on autobiographical memory (Brown, 2021;Castillo et al., 2022;Öner et al., 2023;Rouhani et al., 2023). The pandemic started abruptly and the quarantines changed people's lives drastically, creating an inflection point that might have had an effect on how well events prior, during, and after the pandemic are remembered and on how the events are temporally distributed. ...

Reference:

2020 feels slow, long, and far away: Time distortion due to the COVID‐19 pandemic
Overestimating the intensity of negative feelings in autobiographical memory: evidence from the 9/11 attack and Covid-19 pandemic

... Firstly, the concept is only relevant to part of our sample: if flashbulb memory corresponds to the detailed recollection of the circumstances in which an individual is informed, by an external source, of an unexpected, emotionally charged and socially important event (Brown and Kulik, 1977), then this concept would only apply to circles 3 and 4 of our corpus, to people who were informed by an external source; everyone else experienced the event live. 9 Secondly, the flashbulb memory approach implies a longitudinal dimension in the analysis (e.g., Dégeilh et al., 2021), which is currently 8 However, the interviewers were given strict instructions never to intervene to correct a mistake. 9 It should be noted that some studies on flashbulb memories extend the spectrum of analysis to include people who have directly experienced the event (see Er, 2003). ...

Changes over 10 years in the retelling of the flashbulb memories of the attack of 11 September 2001

Memory

... In underscoring the role practice effects, WIRIF, and SSRIF can play in shaping the memories of those interacting with external sources, we are underscoring memory's social nature, an attribute that could be viewed as adaptive for creature as social as human being are (Hirst et al., 2019;Wimber et al., 2015, Schacter, 2012Storm et al., 2008). In the present study, we mainly focus on the simple act of listening and the more dynamic act of engaging in a conversation. ...

The Mnemonic Consequences of Jurors’ Selective Retrieval During Deliberation
  • Citing Article
  • June 2019

Topics in Cognitive Science

... It is difficult for people to forecast their future emotions accurately, no matter how hard they try (e.g., Doré et al., 2016;Buechel et al., 2017). This study provides a more habitual context to construct participants' occupational, emotional responses rather than general (i.e., Levine et al., 2012) and draw a picture of people's weaknesses in affective forecasting. ...

Highly Accurate Prediction of Emotions Surrounding the Attacks of September 11, 2001 Over 1-, 2-, and 7-Year Prediction Intervals

Journal of Experimental Psychology General

... Several studies have shown greater knowledge about the content area leads to a person being more influential and less knowledge about the area leads to the person conforming more (e.g., Gabbert, Memon, and Wright 2007;Williamson, Weber, and Robertson 2013). If people trust the other person and believe in their expertise they are more likely to be influenced by them than otherwise (Koppel et al. 2014). Our focus is on the evaluation and managerial aspects of power and whether they lead to different patterns of memory conformity. ...

The Effect of Listening to Others Remember on Subsequent Memory: The Roles of Expertise and Trust in Socially Shared Retrieval-Induced Forgetting and Social Contagion

Social Cognition

... The hospitalization of the Prime Minister Boris Johnson, for example, has created a febrile public atmosphere, the extent of which can be gauged by the claims of some right-wing commentators that fears over Johnson's personal health could be directly equated with that of the national collective health (a figure of thought with an extremely unpalatable historical lineage). As William Hirst et al. (2015) have shown, both flashbulb memories and memories of flashbulb events can become highly inconsistent over time, although the latter may be moderated somewhat by attention to media. Given that such memories are typically the prism through which a specific historical period is framed, this should give cause for concern that the complexities of global political and environmental processes might come to be memorialized in the longer term by way of wobbly, but vivid recollections of a specific controversial political figure. ...

A Ten-Year Follow-Up of a Study of Memory for the Attack of September 11, 2001: Flashbulb Memories and Memories for Flashbulb Events

Journal of Experimental Psychology General

... Findings from this research stream have shown differences regarding emotional valence when comparing private and public FBMs. Pillemer (2009) found that private FBMs generally exhibit higher consequentiality, personal importance, and rehearsal than public FBMs. Between positive and negative public events, although Talarico and Rubin (2018) concluded that research on emotional valence produced mixed findings on consistency and confidence, results of a recent study by Raw et al. (2023) conducted in the UK on FBMs of the 2016 Brexit Referendum suggest that negativity enhances accuracy, whereas positivity creates overconfidence. ...

Long-Term Memory for the Terrorist Attack of September 11: Flashbulb Memories, Event Memories, and the Factors That Influence Their Retention

Journal of Experimental Psychology General