Robert D. Nebes’s research while affiliated with Duke University Medical Center and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (21)


The effect of age on hemispheric asymmetry in visual and auditory identification
  • Article

February 1983

·

14 Reads

·

43 Citations

Experimental Aging Research

Robert D. Nebes

·

David J. Madden

·

William D. Berg

On psychometric tests, spatial scores typically decline more with age than do verbal scores. Since in humans, visuo-spatial information is more efficiently processed by the right hemisphere (RH) and verbal information by the left (LH), this behavioral pattern could reflect a greater age decline in RH than in LH abilities. To test this possibility, the speed with which young and old subjects identified stimuli in their right and left visual fields was measured. Since each half field, projects to the opposite hemisphere, by presenting stimuli in one half field, RH and LH abilities can be measured relatively independently. Words were identified faster in the right field (i.e., LH), pictorial stimuli in the left (RH). This was equally true for both young and old. Similarly, a right ear advantage in the identification of dichotically presented syllables was of equal magnitude in both age groups. Thus, there was no evidence on these tasks for a selective decline with age in RH processing efficiency.


The use of focused attention in visual search by young and old adults

February 1983

·

14 Reads

·

34 Citations

Experimental Aging Research

A deficit in focused attention has recently been suggested to underlie some of the cognitive decrements seen in the elderly. This hypothesis was tested in two visual search studies. Subjects had to decide whether or not a given target was present among an array of six digits, three of which were red, the rest black. For the "yes" trials, prior information regarding target color substantially reduced search time in comparison to warning signal that did not provide color information. This reduction in search time was equivalent in the young and the old, both at long (ad lib) and shorter (250-1000 msec) array durations. This result suggests that both age groups are equally proficient in focusing their attention on the digits of the relevant color. On the "no" trials, prior color-information was not effective in reducing search time. There was some evidence in experiment two that the older subjects were using a different search strategy on these trials.


Signal-Detection Analysis of Hemispheric Differences in Visual Recognition Memory

January 1982

·

10 Reads

·

5 Citations

Cortex

In the present experiment, subjects decided on each trial whether or not a unilaterally presented probe digit was a member of a previously memorized set of two, three, or four digits. The probe was presented at a near-threshold duration and followed by a visual pattern mask. Signal-detection estimates of detectability and response bias were obtained from subjects' confidence ratings regarding their decisions. The detectability of the probe in memory was significantly better when the probe was presented to the right visual field-left hemisphere than when presented to the left visual field-right hemisphere. The response criterion became significantly more lax as memory-set size increased, but only for probes presented to the left visual field-right hemisphere. The present results are consistent with reaction-time studies using verbal stimuli that indicate a left hemisphere advantage in the efficiency of memory comparison.


Memory for verbal and spatial information as a function of age

July 1980

·

13 Reads

·

40 Citations

Experimental Aging Research

The present study investigated whether there is a differential decline with age in verbal and spatial memory, by measuring the ability of 24 young (mean age = 18.8) and 24 elderly (mean age = 69.5) subjects to remember verbal and spatial information under identical task conditions. Subjects recalled either the identities or spatial locations of seven letters arranged randomly within a 5 x 5 grid. To determine whether subjects actually encoded the verbal and spatial characteristics of the array differently, verbal and spatial interference tasks were administered during the retention interval. Results showed that the memory decrement in the elderly was not greater for the spatial aspects of the stimulus array than for its verbal aspects. Thus, there was no evidence for a greater decline with age in spatial memory than in verbal memory. Limited support was found for the utility of the selective interference paradigm to demonstrate separate and independent verbal and spatial memory codes.


Vocal Versus Manual Response As a Determinant of Age Difference in Simple Reaction Time

December 1978

·

16 Reads

·

60 Citations

Journal of Gerontology

The decrease in response speed usually found to occur with advancing age is felt by many investigators to be a phenomenon common to all speeded tasks, regardless of the exact nature of the sensory stimulus or motor response involved. In the present experiment, however, the type of response the subject made determined whether or not an age difference existed in simple reaction time to a visual stimulus. When subjects responded manually, by releasing a key, the usual age decrement in response latency was found. When the same subjects responded vocally, by saying a word, there was no significant age difference in reaction time. This result suggests that slowing of psychomotor latency with age is not a general phenomenon, but may be limited to certain neural systems.


Intellectual Differences in Relation to Personal and Family Handedness
  • Article
  • Full-text available

December 1976

·

415 Reads

·

80 Citations

Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

Right, mixed and left-handed college students were given the complete WAIS, and a series of cognitive factor tests. Results showed left- and mixed-handed individuals to have a significantly lower full scale I.Q. than right-handers. There was no difference between the mixed and left-handers. In all three handedness groups, subjects with a positive family history of sinistrality had a lower full scale I.Q. than did subjects without left-handed relatives. Neither handedness nor family history differentially affected the Verbal or Performance subscales, nor did they have a significant effect on scores in the other cognitive tests. These results are discussed with respect to Levy's theory of hemispheric specialization, and to the role of inheritance and brain damage in the causation of left- and mixed-handedness.Presently at the Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of MedicinePresently at the Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.

Download

Verbal-pictorial Recoding in the Elderly

August 1976

·

5 Reads

·

11 Citations

Journal of Gerontology

Older individuals have been reported to use imagery mediation less often than the young in remembering verbal material. To determine whether this is due to a decrease with age in the speed with which verbal stimuli are recoded into pictorial representations, the reaction time of 12 old (63-78) and 12 young (17-25) subjects for matching verbal descriptions to geometric shapes was measured. While the elderly were generally slower, they were identical to the young in being faster at determining whether or not two simultaneously presented shapes were the same than they were a printed description of a shape and a shape. Also, in both age groups, when 1 or more sec elapsed between the presentation of the description and that of the shape, matching times were as quick as when both stimuli were shapes, suggesting that, for the old as well as the young, 1 sec was sufficient time to recode the description into a pictorial form.


The effect of age on the speed of sentence formation and incidetal learning

August 1976

·

15 Reads

·

15 Citations

Experimental Aging Research

To test the hypothesis that older persons tend not to use verbal mediators in paired-associate learning because it takes them too long to form an appropriate mediator, the time needed by young and old subjects to generate sentences incorporating given pairs of nouns was measured. Older subjects formed sentences just as rapidly as did the young. Despite their equivalent speed in creating these verbal associations when tested later for the occurrence of incidental learning of the noun pairs, the older subjects showed much poorer recall than did the younger subjects. This age difference in learning did not appear to be a function of any major dissimilarities in the generated sentences themselves, i.e., in the grammatical constructions used, or in the imageability of the relationships expressed by the sentences. The rated imagery value of the stimulus nouns was found to affect both the speed of sentence formation and the accuracy of incidental learning; in both younger and older subjects, formation time was less, and recall was better for the high imagery noun pairs.


The use of imagery in memory by right and left handers

February 1976

·

10 Reads

·

1 Citation

Neuropsychologia

Right and left handers were given a variety of memory tasks for which imagery ability has been demonstrated to be an important determinant of performance. Results showed no significant relationship between subjects' handedness and the efficiency with which they used imagery in memory.


The effects of handedness, family history and sex on the performance of a dichotic listening task

February 1976

·

9 Reads

·

98 Citations

Neuropsychologia

The effects of handedness, family history of handedness, sex, and ear differences on the performance of a dichotic listening task by 120 college students were studied. The sample, as a whole, showed a significant right ear superiority. Females made fewer errors than males. There were no significant main effects of family history or handedness nor were any of the interactions significant. It was suggested that right ear dominance is more prevalent than is generally supposed. A possible relationship between ear asymmetries and intellectual level was discussed.


Citations (20)


... Whether or not it is directly related to a dichotic cognitive system, the brain's lateralization into two hemispheres serves as an example of how parallel processing is more the rule than the exception in information processing. Taking the dual-process theory to an extreme, psychological research has evidenced that our brain's two hemispheres specialize in their own unique tasks (e.g., Nebes andSperry, 1971, Springer &Deutsch, 1985). Electroencephalogram (EEG) studies and split-brain research involving individuals whose corpus callosum has been severed shows a specific hemispheric specialization strongly corresponding with the principles of operation found in the two cognitive subsystems examined in this paper. ...

Reference:

Elucidating Need for Cognition Using a Procedural Learning Paradigm
Cerebral dominance in perception
  • Citing Article
  • January 1971

... During the same session, the older participants were assessed with the neuropsychological tests. We designed the control condition in order to assure that the potential differences between the two groups were not due to a generalized age-related decrease in response speed (see Nebes, 1978). A similar approach was applied in our previous studies on aging and motor cognition (Zapparoli et al., 2016(Zapparoli et al., , 2019. ...

Vocal Versus Manual Response As a Determinant of Age Difference in Simple Reaction Time
  • Citing Article
  • December 1978

Journal of Gerontology

... This large bias in both groups is rarely commented on by handedness researchers. In fact, more often than not, right-handers and left-handers do not differ statistically on dichotic listening tests, although the numerical differences are inevitably in the predicted direction of reduced REA in the left-handed participants (Briggs and Nebes 1976;Hugdahl et al. 2009;Hirnstein et al. 2014). When this difference achieves statistical significance, the effects tend to be small and depend on large samples (Bless et al. 2015;in Karlsson et al. 2019 compare E1 and E2). ...

The effects of handedness, family history and sex on the performance of a dichotic listening task
  • Citing Article
  • February 1976

Neuropsychologia

... Most investigators attribute these age-related declines in episodic memory to poor encoding techniques on the part of older adults. Older adults are more likely to use shallow encoding techniques (e.g., rehearsal), whereas younger adults are more likely to use deep encoding techniques (e.g., imagery, verbal elaboration; Hulicka and Grossman, 1967;Nebes and Andrew-Kulis, 1976;Whitbourne & Slevin, 1978), and these techniques have been shown to result in better retrieval. ...

The effect of age on the speed of sentence formation and incidetal learning
  • Citing Article
  • August 1976

Experimental Aging Research

... Other evidence indicates that the CVLT-II utilizes both the right hippocampus and right frontal lobe for word learning and retrieval (Johnson et al., 2001), therefore making it a difficult tool to rely on for hemispheric lateralization. Despite limitations in the neuropsychological measures utilized in this study, previous literature on handedness in people without MS does not consistently point to a main effect of handedness (e.g., Sahu et al., 2016;Briggs, Nebes, & Kinsbourne, 1976), thus findings from the current study suggest something novel about the MS brain differentiating it from the brains of people without MS. ...

Intellectual Differences in Relation to Personal and Family Handedness

Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

... In the following decades, several instruments have been devised, starting with Annett's [12] which then, in turn, inspired the development of further instruments. Among the most appreciated and used questionnaires, the following deserve to be mentioned: the Provins and Cunliffe's questionnaire [13]; the Briggs and Nebes's questionnaire [14]; the Handedness Questionnaire Cambridge [9]; the Stanley Coren's Lateral Preference Inventory [15]; the Waterloo Handedness and Footedness Questionnaire-revised [16,17], etc. Some of these questionnaires contain many items (e.g., Provins and Cunliffe's questionnaire: 31 items; Waterloo Handedness and Footedness Questionnaire -r: 36(39) + 10(13) items), some of which concern activities that can be considered anachronistic. ...

Patterns of Hand Preference in a Student Population
  • Citing Article
  • October 1975

Cortex

... Typically, long words take a longer time to articulate; hence, some traditional models of verbal WM, such as the phonological loop (see, Baddeley, 1986), suppose that the WLE reflects a disadvantage of long words in articulatory rehearsal. Such models assume that phonological representations are evoked through word perception, then decay within a few seconds, and that articulating a word can maintain decaying representations (e.g., Baddeley & Wilson, 1985;Nebes, 1975). Some other models suppose that the WLE reflects disadvantages of long words in retrieval processes, such as disadvantages in assembling the segments of the degraded phonological representations (e.g., Neath & Nairne, 1995), in planning phonological output processes (e.g., , and in distinguishing phonological representations of a word from other words in a list (e.g., Hulme, Suprenant, Bireta, Stuart, & Neath, 2004). ...

The nature of internal speech in a patient with aphemia
  • Citing Article
  • November 1975

Brain and Language

... Another, less reported, approach to studying age differences and similarities in memorial processing of pictures and words involves assessing the latencies of subprocesses concerned with semantic memory access. There have been two primary methodologies used: dual-stimulation comparison tasks (e.g., Elias & Kinsbourne, 1974;Nebes, 1976) and picture-naming tasks (e.g., Thomas, Fozard, & Waugh, 1977). Elias and Kinsbourne (1974) presented elderly and young Paul C. Amrhein, Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico; John Theios, Department of Psychology, University of Wisconsin-Madison. ...

Verbal-pictorial Recoding in the Elderly
  • Citing Article
  • August 1976

Journal of Gerontology

... The sequential and simultaneous cognitive processes have been associated with specific anatomical regions of the brain [49][50][51]. In general, the left hemisphere is responsible for processing language and performing sequential processing of information [52]. ...

Hemispheric Specialization in Commissurotomized Man

... Meanwhile, other studies have highlighted the different functional specializations of the left and right cerebral hemispheres (for review see Gazzaniga, 2005;and Zaidel, 1991). For example, the right hemisphere appears to be superior for face recognition, visuospatial tasks, processing of global/holistic forms, and performing mental rotation (Bogen & Gazzaniga, 1965;Nebes, 1973;Gazzaniga & Smylie, 1983;Corballis & Sergent, 1988;Robertson, Lamb & Zaidel, 1993). Similarly, in the vast majority of adults, the left hemisphere is specialized for most aspects of language (Lassonde, Bryden & Demers, 1990;Reuter-Lorenz & Baynes, 1992;Gazzaniga, 2000). ...

Perception of spatial relationships by the right and left hemispheres in commissurotomized man
  • Citing Article
  • August 1973

Neuropsychologia