Rindy C. Anderson’s research while affiliated with Florida Atlantic University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (44)


Ornamentation and body condition, but not glucocorticoids, predict wild songbird cloacal microbiome community and diversity
  • Article

December 2024

·

26 Reads

Oikos

·

·

·

[...]

·

Rindy C. Anderson

Animal populations can exhibit dramatic variation in individual fitness, and microbiota are emerging as a potentially understudied factor influencing host health. Bacterial diversity and community structure of the gut microbiome are associated with many aspects of fitness in animals, but relatively little is known about the generality of these relationships in wild populations and non‐mammalian taxa. We studied the northern cardinal Cardinalis cardinalis , a member of a taxon that is ecologically important but underrepresented in microbiome research: songbirds. To test for relationships between the microbiota and host fitness, we sampled the cloacal microbiomes of wild cardinals and measured body condition index, assessed coloration of sexual ornaments (beak and plumage), and collected blood to estimate the glucocorticoid response to stress. Both alpha and beta bacterial diversity were related to individual variation in body condition and several sexual ornaments, but not glucocorticoid concentrations. Our results from a free‐living songbird population add to a growing body of research linking avian host fitness to internal bacterial community characteristics. This study sets the stage for manipulative experiments to determine how challenges to fitness and microbiomes may upset these relationships.


A diagram showing the relationship between phonological syntax, lexical syntax, sequential syntax, and dialectic syntax. Syntax exists as an orderly arrangement of parts. If the structure of the syntax is not associated with a semantic meaning, it is considered sequential syntax. Dialectic syntax is a type of sequential syntax that is population specific. If syntax does convey a semantic meaning, it is considered phonological syntax at the first level and if the resulting meaningful units are combined again, it is considered lexical syntax.
An example of sequential and dialectic syntax in the songs of Bachman's sparrows. (A) The organization of notes into syllables is an example of sequential syntax. (B) The organization of an introductory note and syllables to create a song type is an example of second‐level sequential syntax. (C) The organization of specific song types into a preferred transition pattern that is highly shared within a population is an example of dialectic syntax.
Syntax in animal communication: its study in songbirds and other taxa
  • Article
  • Full-text available

October 2024

·

73 Reads

·

1 Citation

Many building blocks of human language can be found within the vocal communication systems of other species, most notably songbirds. One of the most prominent of these building blocks is syntax. While studies of syntax are abundant, a lack of consensus on the definition of syntax in non‐human animal communication studies has led to much debate. Consistent and deliberate use of terminology is needed to facilitate understanding across disciplines. In addition, new terminology may better describe syntactic structure found in vocal signals that are devoid of semantic associations, such as birdsong. Here, we propose two terms to describe the types of syntax commonly found in birdsong: sequential syntax and dialectical syntax. Sequential syntax can be defined as the rules that govern the patterns of sound without regard to semantic meaning. Dialectic syntax can be defined as sequential syntax that is distinct among different populations or groups with behavioral significance for those groups. Taken together, these two terms can describe the type of syntax seen in ornamental signals, such as birdsong.

Download


Effects of anthropogenic noise on cognition, bill color, and growth in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata)

December 2022

·

86 Reads

·

7 Citations

acta ethologica

There is growing concern that anthropogenic noise has various deleterious effects on wildlife in urban environments. In humans, it has been suggested that anthropogenic noise exposure during childhood can have long-term effects on cognitive processes in adulthood. Here we tested if urban noise impacts avian cognitive performance by testing adult zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata) on several cognition tasks in the presence or absence of urban noise playback. We also tested if urban noise impacts growth and cognitive development by testing adult zebra finches on cognition tasks after exposing them to urban noise, pink noise, or no noise during their first 90 days of life. We found that urban noise exposure marginally reduced cognitive performance during tests of a novel motor skill but did not reduce performance during tests of color association learning or spatial memory tasks. We found that urban noise exposure during development marginally affected cognitive performance in adulthood on a color association task. While urban noise exposure during development did not affect adult body size, treated males and females developed less bright bill coloration and redder bills, respectively, than untreated birds. Our results suggest that urban noise exposure may affect morphological traits, such as bill color, that influence social interactions and mate choice. Future studies should examine how noise exposure affects other cognitive behaviors, such as social behavior, and how social behavior in turn might exacerbate or mitigate negative effects of noise.


Song repertoires, song type sharing, and sharing of preferred song transitions in male Bachman's Sparrows (Peucaea aestivalis)

September 2022

·

29 Reads

·

2 Citations

The Wilson Journal of Ornithology

Bachman's Sparrows (Peucaea aestivalis) have unusually large song repertoires for New World sparrows. Answering questions about their function, evolution, and development requires thorough description of these repertoires in multiple populations. Here, we quantified repertoire size, song type sharing, and sequence sharingspecifically, the sharing of preferred song type transitionsof primary songs within each of 2 populations of Bachman's Sparrows at northeastern and southeastern ends of the species' breeding range. We recorded 20 males in southern North Carolina (NC) and 18 males in eastern Florida (FL). Individual repertoire size had a mean of 4849 song types in both populations. Within each population, males shared many song types, with 80% repertoire overlap between any 2 males in NC and 49% in FL. This within-population song sharing was independent of distance between males' territories. The sequence in which males sang song types was neither stereotyped nor random, and within each population, preferred song transitions were shared by all (NC) or most (FL) pairs of males. The sharing of preferred song transitions was also not correlated with the distance between territories, suggesting that birds do not adjust repertoires or song sequences in adulthood to match more closely those of territory neighbors. The function and ontogeny of the repertoire features documented here in Bachman's Sparrowslarge repertoires with high sharing of both song types and preferred song transitionsinvite further study. Los gorriones Peucaea aestivalis tienen repertorios de canto inusualmente grandes entre los gorriones del Nuevo Mundo. La respuesta a preguntas sobre su funcin, evolucin y desarrollo requiere descripciones completas de dichos repertorios en mltiples poblaciones. Aqu cuantificamos el tamao del repertorio, intercambio de tipos de canto y secuencias en especfico, el intercambio de transiciones de tipos de canto preferidas de cantos primarios al interior de 2 poblaciones de este gorrin en los extremos noreste y sureste del rango reproductivo de esta especie. Grabamos 20 machos en North Carolina (NC) y 18 machos en el este de Florida (FL). El tamao promedio del repertorio fue de 4849 tipos de canto en ambas poblaciones. Dentro de cada poblacin, los machos compartieron muchos tipos de canto, con 80% de traslape de repertorios en cualquiera de 2 machos en NC y 49% en FL. Estos cantos compartidos al interior de la poblacin fue independiente de la distancia entre territorios de los machos. La secuencia en la que los machos cantaron los diferentes tipos de canto no fue estereotipado o aleatorio, y al interior de cada poblacin, las transiciones preferidas de canto fueron compartidas por todos (NC) o la mayora (FL) de los pares de machos. Las transiciones de cantos preferidas tampoco estuvieron correlacionadas con la distancia entre territorios, lo que sugiere que las aves no ajustan sus repertorios o secuencias de canto en la edad adulta para concordar ms cercanamente con las de sus vecinos de territorio. La funcin y ontogenia de las caractersticas del repertorio de este gorrin que documentamos aqu repertorios grandes con gran parte de cantos compartidos de tipos de canto y transiciones de cantos preferidas nos invitan a realizar ms estudios. Palabras clave: bioacstica, canto de aves, Passerellidae, secuencias vocales, variacin especial.




Figure 1. The relationship between aggressiveness principal component and body size principal component in male Bachman's sparrows at Jonathan Dickinson State Park in 2018. Positive body size and aggressiveness values indicate larger and more aggressive individuals, respectively. values <0.05, p values between 0.05 and 0.1 as non-significant trends, and values >0.1 as not significant. All statistical analyses were done in R 3.5.2 (R Core Team, 2013).
Figure 2. The mean ± SE effect of reproductive index on the aggressiveness principal component in male Bachman's sparrows at Jonathan Dickinson State Park in 2018. 1, Unpaired; 2, paired but no nesting; 3, paired and nesting but no fledglings; 4, fledged young (Winiarski et al., 2017b). Positive aggressiveness values indicate more aggressive individuals. Letters above the bars show the results of Tukey's honest significant difference post hoc test: bars that do not share the same letters are significantly different (p < 0.05).
Figure 3. The relationship of sparrow density on the aggressiveness principal component for male Bachman's sparrows at Jonathan Dickinson State Park in 2018. Positive aggressiveness values indicate more aggressive individuals.
Figure 4. Anselin's local indicators of spatial autocorrelation (LISA) for the aggressiveness principal component with respect to longitude and latitude for male Bachman's sparrows at Jonathan Dickinson State Park in 2018. Red circles denote positive deviations from the mean, black squares denote negative deviations from the mean, and filled points are significant local correlations (p < 0.05).
Linear models examining the relationships between the aggressiveness principal component and vegetation characteristics of Bachman's sparrow territories (N = 31) at Jonathan Dick- inson State Park in 2018.
Body size, habitat quality, and territory defense in Bachman’s sparrow

March 2021

·

183 Reads

·

5 Citations

Behaviour

Many wild populations of animals conform to the ideal despotic distribution (IDD) in which more competitive individuals exclude less competitive individuals from high quality resources. Body size and aggressiveness are two important traits for resource defense, and they positively covary so that larger individuals are usually more aggressive. Using Bachman’s sparrows, we tested the hypothesis that larger birds are more aggressive and are thus able to compete for the best quality territories. We found that larger males were more aggressive, and more aggressive birds fledged at least one young. However, we did not find consistent relationships between aggressiveness and habitat characteristics. Our results suggest that Bachman’s sparrows meet most of the predictions of the IDD. Frequent ecological disturbances, such as fires, might disrupt the IDD or make it difficult to detect. Additional studies are needed to test for relationships between ecological disturbances and territorial behaviour.


Top row: Distribution of competitiveness among breeders when (A) stochastic effects are weak (σc = 0) and (B) stochastic effects are strong (σc = 0.2). High frequency is indicated by darker gray. Bottom row: Population dynamics of breeders (dark line) and floaters (light line) when (C) stochastic effects are weak (σc = 0) and (D) stochastic effects are strong (σc = 0.2).
Predictions of the simulation model with open competition. Shading represents the distribution of competitiveness in the breeder population, with high frequency indicated by darker gray. Solid points represent average floater abundance. Results are averaged over 100 replicate runs of 10⁵ time steps for each value of σc. Shading represents the relative abundance of values of c during the last 10⁴ time steps. Similarly, floater abundance is the average over the last 10⁴ time steps. Error bars indicate the standard deviation of the average over the 100 replicate runs.
Equilibria and invasion analysis. (A) Floater equilibrium (solid line) and breeder equilibrium (dashed line). (B) Invasion plot for a mutant with competitive ability c invading a resident population with c* and deterministic competition (σc = 0.0). Dashed line denotes c = c*, where invader and resident traits are identical. Regions in which invasion by a mutant with c succeeds or fails are denoted by + and −, respectively. Territory quality R takes values between Rmin and Rmax in steps of ΔR = 0.001. If the outcome of competition is deterministic, an invader with c > c* obtains the ith best territory, where i is a random draw from a negative binomial with mean = (1 − μB)/μB, i.e., E(R|c,c*)=Rmax − ΔR(1 − μB)/μB. The invasion succeeds if g(c)E(R|c,c*) > 1, indicated by the upper solid line. An invader with c < c* can invade if c is below the lower solid line. Dotted lines mark values of c at which floaters are eliminated and the population goes extinct. Red line indicates a typical trajectory in which successive mutants replace residents. Competitiveness increases until invasion by c < c* and c > c* is possible (open red point). After this point, trait dynamics oscillate (as in Fig. 1A). (C) Invasion plot for stochastic competition with σc = 0.2. Symbols are as in (B). A resident with c* can be invaded by c > c* up to the value indicated by the filled red point. Beyond this point, only mutants with c below the solid line can invade.
Predictions of the simulation model with spatially constrained competition. (A) Offspring settle at random. (B) Offspring settle in the nearest territory with no floaters or other new recruits. Shading represents the distribution of competitiveness in the breeder population, with high frequency indicated by darker gray. Solid points represent average floater abundance, and open points represent average breeder abundance. Averages and error bars are calculated as described in the Fig. 2 legend.
Predicted correlation between breeder competitiveness (c) and territory quality (R). Points represent the correlation at the end of 10⁵ time steps averaged over 100 replicate runs and bars represent the standard deviation. Open points represent the predictions from the model in which offspring disperse at random. Solid points represent predictions from the model in which offspring avoid competitors.
Asymmetric competition and floater dynamics

December 2020

·

33 Reads

·

2 Citations

In territorial species, nonterritorial floaters may be critical to population dynamics. One theoretical framework, based on the assumption that floating is a strategic decision to forego reproduction, predicts that selection maintains an abundant floater population even if low‐quality territories are available. However, existing models make two critical assumptions: all individuals have equal competitive ability, and every individual in a population has access to every available territory. We assess the consequences of relaxing these assumptions in a model of asymmetric competition with a trade‐off between investment in competitiveness and reproductive success. Our results demonstrate that selection for greater competitiveness eliminates floater production unless the outcome of territorial contests has a strong stochastic component. Next, we suppose individuals can compete for territories only within a fixed neighborhood. If this constraint is sufficiently strong, our model predicts that a population will produce floaters. Finally, we show that our model makes novel predictions regarding the maintenance of trait variation and the relationship between this variation and the distribution of competitors among unequal territories.


Evidence supporting the microbiota–gut–brain axis in a songbird

November 2020

·

97 Reads

·

21 Citations

Recent research in mammals supports a link between cognitive ability and the gut microbiome, but little is known about this relationship in other taxa. In a captive population of 38 zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata), we quantified performance on cognitive tasks measuring learning and memory. We sampled the gut microbiome via cloacal swab and quantified bacterial alpha and beta diversity. Performance on cognitive tasks related to beta diversity but not alpha diversity. We then identified differentially abundant genera influential in the beta diversity differences among cognitive performance categories. Though correlational, this study provides some of the first evidence of an avian microbiota–gut–brain axis, building foundations for future microbiome research in wild populations and during host development.


Citations (39)


... Heather Wolverton and Rindy Anderson discuss challenges and opportunities associated with the definition and study of 'syntax' in animal communication (Wolverton and Anderson 2024). This analysis includes some important considerations for research into birdsong in particular, as well as other avian vocalizations, summarizing important terminology, perspectives and contexts for ongoing investigations that attempt to understand the structure of animal signals. ...

Reference:

Introducing the mini‐review article category and the Journal of Avian Biology review award
Syntax in animal communication: its study in songbirds and other taxa

... Behavioural ecology deals with the ability of animals to reproduce and survive in their natural environment [84]. Noise can impact the behaviour of birds through 'Decrease of density/ Abundance of population' [85][86][87][88][89][90][91][92][93], 'Behavioural changes/response' [1,2] and 'Reduction of cognitive performance' [94,95]. ...

Effects of anthropogenic noise on cognition, bill color, and growth in the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata)
  • Citing Article
  • December 2022

acta ethologica

... repertoires of distinct song types, which they sing with 'immediate variety', usually changing song type with each successive song. During singing bouts, a bird does not cycle through its repertoire in a fixed sequence, but there is evidence of certain preferred transitions from one song type to another (Soha et al. 2022). The authors found that these song sequences exist in two distinct populations of Bachman's sparrow, and that sequences are highly shared among the males of each population, suggesting that sequences might be learned. ...

Song repertoires, song type sharing, and sharing of preferred song transitions in male Bachman's Sparrows (Peucaea aestivalis)
  • Citing Article
  • September 2022

The Wilson Journal of Ornithology

... It is not known if the ideal free distribution or the ideal despotic distribution better describes interannual patterns of habitat use over decades under natural fluctuations in abundance. Some studies have yielded evidence of populations conforming to the ideal free distribution (Haché, Villard, and Bayne 2013;Pagán, Martínez, and Calvo 2009;Petit and Petit 1996;Whitham 1980), while others have found evidence of ideal despotic distribution (Niederhauser et al. 2021;Zimmerman, LaHaye, and Gutiérrez 2003). Boyce et al. (2016) suggested that birds and mammals do not usually follow an ideal free distribution. ...

Body size, habitat quality, and territory defense in Bachman’s sparrow

Behaviour

... If minimum resource conditions were unavailable to establish a territory within a tree vole's search radius (200-m; 1 hexagon), the individual was deemed a nonreproducing "floater" and incurred an additional survival penalty (survival rate = 0.1) after annual survival rates were applied. Floaters were retained in the model as they can make important contributions to population dynamics (Noonburg & Anderson, 2021). Occupancy and movements of simulated individuals were restricted to the current range of the species (i.e., within the last halfcentury; Forsman et al., 2016). ...

Asymmetric competition and floater dynamics

... Yet, the understanding of these links in wild animals remains limited (Hird, 2017;Davidson et al., 2020). Studies in birds associated exploratory behavior with microbiota diversity, while learning and memory performance have been correlated with compositional differences (Florkowski and Yorzinski, 2023;Slevin et al., 2020). The gut microbiota's links to stress have also been explored: in common toad tadpoles (Bufo bufo), elevated baseline corticosterone associates with higher microbial diversity (Gabor et al., 2022), while American red squirrels (Tamiasciurus hudsonicus) show lower alpha diversity and fewer gastrointestinal pathogens in response to elevated glucocorticoids (Petrullo et al., 2022). ...

Evidence supporting the microbiota–gut–brain axis in a songbird

... In birds, there is variation among species in the age at which chicks begin to respond to environmental challenges by activating the HPA axis and releasing corticosterone (e.g. Adams et al., 2008;Bebus et al., 2020;Wada et al., 2007), as well as variation in which stimuli can elicit an HPA axis response from nestlings (e.g. Kern, 2018, 2014). ...

Development of the corticosterone stress response differs among passerine species
  • Citing Article
  • February 2020

General and Comparative Endocrinology

... For song sparrows, two factors were extracted, and not all tasks loaded onto the first factor extracted. This may be due to the low reliability in performance across years on cognitive tasks in song sparrows (Soha et al. 2019). While the results from animals thus far are interesting and promising, there are some difficulties in comparing g across species. ...

Performance on tests of cognitive ability is not repeatable across years in a songbird
  • Citing Article
  • October 2019

Animal Behaviour

... Low amplitude song also occurs in other bird species, where it is usually termed soft song (Reichard and Anderson 2015). Such soft song is a context-dependent low amplitude signal that has been seen as an adaptation to prevent eavesdropping from distant individuals when the signal is intended only for a receiver nearby (Dabelsteen et al. 1998;Rice et al. 2013;Zollinger and Brumm 2015;Ali and Anderson 2018). Such soft song is indeed common in territorial bird species with otherwise loud territorial advertisement song (Anderson et al. 2008;Reichard and Anderson 2015). ...

Song and aggressive signaling in Bachman's Sparrow
  • Citing Article
  • July 2018

Ornithology

... Shultz & Dunbar, 2010;MacLean et al., 2009;Kverkov a et al., 2018;Weisbecker et al., 2015) and neocortex (Schillaci, 2008;Sandel et al., 2016) vastly outnumbered studies on other brain regions] prevented us from analysing this relationship at a finer scale than comparing cognitive tests to neuroanatomy. Additionally, given that the SIH is based on the cognitive demands of sociality, it was surprising to see how few studies utilise social tests of cognition such as tactical deception (Byrne & Corp, 2004), theory of mind (Devaine et al., 2017), social learning (Sewall et al., 2018;Lefebvre, Palameta & Hatch, 1996) and inequity aversion (Wascher, 2015). Many intraspecific tests of the relationship between sociality and cognition use domain-general tasks, rather than socio-cognitive tasks to measure cognition, despite the explicit prediction that social pressures have driven the evolution of cognition (Dunbar, 1998;Jolly, 1966;Humphrey, 1976;Chance & Mead, 1953). ...

Early life conditions that impact song learning in male zebra finches also impact neural and behavioral responses to song in females
  • Citing Article
  • November 2017

Developmental Neurobiology