May 2017
·
50 Reads
·
1 Citation
This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.
May 2017
·
50 Reads
·
1 Citation
January 2014
·
228 Reads
·
7 Citations
January 2011
·
151 Reads
·
4 Citations
Journal of Motor Behavior
November 2010
·
655 Reads
·
28 Citations
“Unintended-acceleration” automobile accidents typically begin when the driver first enters the car, starts the engine, and intends to press his/her right foot on the brake while shifting from Park to a drive gear (Drive or Reverse). The driver reports an unintended (uncommanded) full-throttle acceleration, coupled with a loss of braking, until the episode ends in a crash. Pedal misapplications – where the right foot contacts the accelerator instead of the brake that was intended – have been linked to these accidents (Schmidt, 1989, 1993) which, in the 1980s, were thought to occur only at the start of a driving cycle (and/or with the car in Park). But, in 1997, we identified over 200 pedal errors as the cause of accidents reported in the North Carolina database; these crashes occurred during the driving cycle (Schmidt et al., 1997), and/or with the vehicle in a gear other than Park. Our present work provides a more thorough analysis of these North Carolina Police Accident Reports from 1979 to 1995. The vast majority of pedal misapplications (over 92%) (a) occurred during the driving cycle, (b) were generally in “unhurried” conditions, and (c) were categorically separate from those events referred to as unintended-acceleration episodes at start-up. These ideas are explanatory for the recent (2009–2010) surge of unintended-acceleration reports, perhaps even suggesting that all of these crashes are caused by pedal errors, and that none of them are based on some vehicle defect(s).
August 2010
·
2,908 Reads
·
124 Citations
Aphasiology
The present study was designed to examine whether applying principles of motor learning to a commonly used treatment approach for acquired apraxia of speech (AOS) would enhance relearning of speech production skills. In particular, we examined one main principle, random practice, and compared it to blocked practice. Of importance is the fact that many speech treatments utilize blocked practice, but the literature on motor learning clearly shows that blocked practice facilitates acquisition of target behaviours, but not retention and transfer. The latter two are considered true indices of learning. In two subjects with severe AOS, results showed that random practice facilitated retention whereas blocked practice did not. The present study provides preliminary evidence that these principles may have a similar effect on learning of skilled speech motor acts as they do on learning of limb movements. The results reported here are encouraging and provide justification and focus for further investigation.
January 2009
·
286 Reads
·
74 Citations
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology
Recent evidence suggests that massive amounts of practice of the basketball free throw (a "set shot") results in the development of a specific memory representation that is unique to this one shot distance and angle, and that is distinct from set shots taken at locations other than the free throw line. We termed this unique capability an especial skill. In this article, we review the evidence and provide new data regarding the existence of especial skills. These findings are discussed in terms of their implications for motor control theory and in terms of the broader context of specificity versus generality in the learning of motor skills.
September 2008
·
222 Reads
·
97 Citations
Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research
Two studies examined speech skill learning in persons with apraxia of speech (AOS). Motor-learning research shows that delaying or reducing the frequency of feedback promotes retention and transfer of skills. By contrast, immediate or frequent feedback promotes temporary performance enhancement but interferes with retention and transfer. These principles were tested in the context of a common treatment for AOS. Two studies (N = 4, N = 2) employed single-subject treatment designs to examine acquisition and retention of speech skills in adults with AOS under different feedback conditions. Reduced-frequency or delayed feedback enhanced learning in 3 participants with AOS. Feedback manipulation was not an influential variable in 3 other cases in which stimulus-complexity effects may have masked treatment effects. These findings demonstrate that individuals with AOS can benefit from structured intervention. They provide qualified support for reduction and delay of feedback, although interaction with other factors such as stimulus complexity or task difficulty needs further exploration. This study adds to the growing body of literature investigating the use of principles of motor learning in treating AOS and provides impetus for consideration of pre-treatment variables that affect outcome in treatment studies.
September 2008
·
23,468 Reads
·
541 Citations
American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology
There has been renewed interest on the part of speech-language pathologists to understand how the motor system learns and determine whether principles of motor learning, derived from studies of nonspeech motor skills, apply to treatment of motor speech disorders. The purpose of this tutorial is to introduce principles that enhance motor learning for nonspeech motor skills and to examine the extent to which these principles apply in treatment of motor speech disorders. This tutorial critically reviews various principles in the context of nonspeech motor learning by reviewing selected literature from the major journals in motor learning. The potential application of these principles to speech motor learning is then discussed by reviewing relevant literature on treatment of speech disorders. Specific attention is paid to how these principles may be incorporated into treatment for motor speech disorders. Evidence from nonspeech motor learning suggests that various principles may interact with each other and differentially affect diverse aspects of movements. Whereas few studies have directly examined these principles in speech motor (re)learning, available evidence suggests that these principles hold promise for treatment of motor speech disorders. Further research is necessary to determine which principles apply to speech motor (re)learning in impaired populations.
March 2006
·
35 Reads
·
7 Citations
Journal of Applied Biomechanics
The timing of glove movements used by baseball pitchers to catch fast approaching balls (i.e., line drives) was examined in two tests to determine the responses and temporal characteristics of glove movements in high school and college baseball pitchers. Balls were projected toward the head of participants at 34.8 m.s-1 (78 mph) on average in an indoor test and at speeds approaching 58.1 m.s-1 (130 mph) in a field test. Pitchers caught over 80% and 15% of the projected balls in the indoor and field tests, respectively. Analyses of glove responses indicated that all pitchers could track the line drives and produce coordinated glove movements, which were initiated 160 ms (+/-47.8), on average, after the ball was launched. College pitchers made initial glove movements sooner than high school pitchers in the field test (p=0.012). In contrast, average glove velocity for pitchers increased from 1.33 (+/-0.61) to 3.45 (+/-0.86) m.s-1 across the tests, but did not differ between experience levels. Glove movement initiation and speed were unrelated, and pitchers utilized visual information throughout the ball's flight to catch balls that approached at speeds exceeding the estimated speeds in competitive situations.
October 2005
·
261 Reads
·
120 Citations
Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Perception & Performance
Differing viewpoints concerning the specificity and generality of motor skill representations in memory were compared by contrasting versions of a skill having either extensive or minimal specific practice. In Experiments 1 and 2, skilled basketball players more accurately performed set shots at the foul line than would be predicted on the basis of the performance at the nearby locations, suggesting considerable specificity at this distance. This effect was replicated even when the lines on the court were obscured (in Experiment 2). However, the effect was absent when jump shots were executed in Experiment 3. The authors argue that massive levels of practice at 1 particular member of a class of actions produce specific effects that allow this skill to stand out from the other members of the class, giving it the status of an especial skill. Various theoretical views are proposed to account for the development of these skills.
... In particular, drivers' braking-related behaviors are considered to be an important cause of accidents (Ren et al. 2011). For example, some inexperienced drivers may use the gas pedal instead of the brake and cause an accident (Schmidt et al. 1997). ...
October 1997
Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting
... The volume of feedback during its delivery was a necessary variable [9]. It is believed that extrinsic feedback is effective since it provides the learner with adequate response, reduces errors and maintains goal orientation. ...
October 1996
Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting
... In general, bimanual movements with the same temporal structure are supposed to be coordinated easily, while movements with different temporal structures are not (Klapp, 1979). Thus, rapid bimanual movements might be controlled by one common underlying GMP (Heuer, Schmidt, & Ghodsian, 1995). ...
September 1995
Biological Cybernetics
... The feedback can be provided either as the action is being performed, called concurrent feedback, or after the action has been completed, called terminal feedback. Concurrent feedback has been demonstrated to increase performance in terms of skill acquisition during training for the novice learner [12]. In the context of martial arts or similar forms of posture or movement training, such as yoga or dance, feedback also takes the form of movement guidance through a process of demonstration and mimicry [19]. ...
December 1997
Human Factors The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society
... Since feedback has to be processed itself, potential conflicts with concurrent 111 cognitive processing seem self-evident. Moreover, providing feedback can deteriorate intrinsic 112 performance monitoring when agents 'delegate' all performance monitoring to the feedback 113 instead of relying on internal monitoring (Swinnen et al., 1990). Thus, while explicit feedback 114 is probably no sure-fire success when it comes to performance-oriented strategy choice, a care-115 ful implementation should have the potential to improve choice. ...
July 1990
Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition
... A fading feedback schedule means that a learner receives a greater frequency of feedback during early stages of learning and the frequency of feedback diminishes as training continues. There is evidence that fading feedback can result in greater retention of motor skills (Nicholson & Schmidt, 1991). Fading feedback addresses the issue of dependency at the expense of intrinsic feedback. ...
September 1991
Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting
... It is well recognised that performance can be improved by augmented feedback (feedback from an external source provided as knowledge of performance or result) [22,23]. This type of feedback is often used during resistance training to enhance acute physical performance and has shown promise as a method for improving chronic physical adaptation [24][25][26][27]. ...
January 1999
... . 또한, 일부 연구자 들은 기존의 페달배치를 지양하고 브레이크 반응시간 (brake reaction time)감소로 야기되는 페달 오동작의 사고율을 감소시키기 위한 획기적인 디자인의 변화가 필요하다고 하였다[7]. 그러나 스웨덴의 볼보자동차 회사와 NTSB의 연구자 들은 제품을 만들어 실험한 결과 페달 디자인과 페달 오작동과의 상관성을 도출하는데 실패하였다[8][9]. 이와 같이 피로를 유발시키지 않은 상태에서 숙련도 와 제어시스템에 따라 하지자세가 차이가 있으므로 사 고 유발 가능성이 더 많은 피로한 경우의 운전 숙련도 와 자동차 페달시스템의 유형에 따라 상이한 페달 제 어동작을 야기해 사고의 위험성이 증가할 수 있다. 지금까지 페달 오작동과 페달디자인 및 배치와의 통 계적 상관유무는 학자들 간 상이한 견해를 가지고 있 어 첨예한 논쟁거리가 되고 있으며 [5],[6] 여러 사회집 단의 이해관계와 맞물려 복잡한 양상을 띠고 있다. ...
September 2005
Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society Annual Meeting
... The pudendal nerve has the role of the efferent arc of the reflex arousal pathway, mainly through one of its branches, the dorsal genital nerve, carrying the sensations from penis or clitoris [8], [9]. Nonetheless, even if some degree of evidence is available from animal studies [10], [11], there is no conclusive proof of the effectiveness of its electrical stimulation on triggering arousal in humans. Percutaneous [12] and laparoscopic [13] surgical approaches to pudendal nerve are well described. ...
January 1983
Neurourology and Urodynamics
... On the other hand, motor variability has been suggested as an essential feature of motor learning that is actively regulated for more efficient motor learning (Wu et al. 2014b;Dhawale et al. 2017). In this case, motor variability can be regarded as an action exploration that facilitates motor learning (Wulf and Schmidt 1988;Lin et al. 2019;Tang et al. 2019), and a greater motor variability may promote generalization of motor skills (Wulf and Schmidt 1988). For instance, previous studies in healthy adults demonstrated superior retention and transfer of learned skills with random practice compared to repetitive practice under the same conditions (Shea and Morgan 1979;Kantak et al. 2010a; Thorp et al. 2017). ...
August 1988
Journal of Motor Behavior