Rachna B. Reddy’s research while affiliated with Harvard University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (22)


Children’s reasoning about a basic aspect of consciousness
  • Article

February 2024

·

6 Reads

European Journal of Developmental Psychology

Rachna B. Reddy

·

Henry M. Wellman


The evolution of the adolescent growth spurt: Urinary biomarkers of bone turnover in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)

March 2023

·

52 Reads

·

9 Citations

Journal of Human Evolution

Life history theory addresses how organisms balance development and reproduction. Mammals usually invest considerable energy into growth in infancy, and they do so incrementally less until reaching adult body size, when they shift energy to reproduction. Humans are unusual in having a long adolescence when energy is invested in both reproduction and growth, including rapid skeletal growth around puberty. Although many primates, especially in captivity, experience accelerated growth in mass around puberty, it remains unclear whether this represents skeletal growth. Without data on skeletal growth in nonhuman primates, anthropologists have often assumed the adolescent growth spurt is uniquely human, and hypotheses for its evolution have focused on other uniquely human traits. The lack of data is largely due to methodological difficulties of assessing skeletal growth in wild primates. Here, we use two urinary markers of bone turnover-osteocalcin and collagen-to study skeletal growth in a large, cross-sectional sample of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda. For both bone turnover markers, we found a nonlinear effect of age, which was largely driven by males. For male chimpanzees, values for osteocalcin and collagen peaked at age 9.4 years and 10.8 years, respectively, which corresponds to early and middle adolescence. Notably, collagen values increased from 4.5 to 9 years, suggesting faster growth during early adolescence compared to late infancy. Biomarker levels plateaued at 20 years in both sexes, suggesting skeletal growth continues until then. Additional data, notably on females and infants of both sexes, are needed, as are longitudinal samples. However, our cross-sectional analysis suggests an adolescent growth spurt in the skeleton of chimpanzees, especially for males. Biologists should avoid claiming that the adolescent growth spurt is uniquely human, and hypotheses for the patterns of human growth should consider variation in our primate relatives.


Dog ‘experimenters’ Fiona (left), Henry (center) and Seymour (right).
Child-dog interaction setup.
Proportion of events in which children (N = 97) gave a pet dog an out-of-reach object when the dog showed interest in the object by attempting to access it themselves (n = 236) vs. ignored the item (n = 102).
Odds ratio plot showing results of a mixed-effects logistic regression where the outcome variable was whether a child gave a dog an out-of-reach object in a given event. Fixed effect predictor estimates and their standard errors are shown. Iteration and participant ID were included as random intercepts (N = 97 child participants, 338 events).
Do children help dogs spontaneously?
  • Article
  • Full-text available

January 2023

·

112 Reads

Two capacities considered foundational in human cooperation are prosocial motivation and goal-reading abilities that enable helping. Children exhibit both proclivities by age 2 in interactions with other humans, but interactions with nonhuman species on whom we have been interdependent for millennia are unstudied. We tested the hypothesis that children’s goal-reading and prosocial propensities extend to other animals. We predicted children would help pet dogs access objects that dogs attempted to reach but could not reach themselves. We studied 97 children between 2 and 3 years of age living in a small mid-western US city, 44 of whom had dogs as household pets. In a quasi-naturalistic setting, we introduced children to 1 of 3 friendly pet dogs who remained within a small, porous enclosure while a treat or toy was placed outside it. Dogs reacted naturally, either showing interest in accessing the item (e.g., pawing, begging) or ignoring it. Measures of dog and child behavior during sessions were coded blindly with high reliability. Children provided dogs with out-of-reach items twice as often when dogs showed interest rather than ignored items, indicating sensitivity to the dog’s goals. Additionally, children were more generally likely to provide dogs with items if children lived with pet dogs, if dogs were more lively and engaged rather than subdued and if the item was a treat rather than a toy. These findings lend support to our hypothesis that children’s early-developing proclivities for goal-reading and prosociality extend beyond humans to other animals.

Download

Fig. 4. Male chimpanzees form more strong bonds with non-family members as they pass from early adolescence to young adulthood (n = 30 males; 9 -20 years old). Each data point indicates a social bond formed by the male on the y axis and another group member (maternal kin including mothers and siblings, non-kin mature males and nonkin mature females). All pairs shown (n = 401) are strongly bonded pairs, defined as pairs whose affiliation indices, a composite measure of bondedness comprising the frequencies of time spent grooming and in spatial proximity, fell in the top 10% of the distribution of affiliation indices of all possible pairs in the community. Pairs farthest from the origin along the x axis are the strongest of these strongly bonded pairs. Males are shown in ascending age order on the y axis, with the youngest male near the origin at age 9 years and the oldest at the top at age 20 years. Reproduced from Reddy and Mitani (2020).
Fig. 5. A moment of social anxiety engendered by the features of fission-fusion group life: Adult male chimpanzee Richmond mounts and holds on to his brother Hutcherson when they hear calls of group members they have not yet seen this day gathering nearby. Richmond and Hutcherson must now decide whether to join or avoid this gathering. Photo by Nathan Chesterman. Ngogo, Kibale National Park, 2014.
Fig. 6. An example of prosocial behaviors in chimpanzee adolescence. A young adolescent chimpanzee, Damien, demonstrates behavior that benefitsothers: A) At age 11, Damien (right) grooms his younger sister Etta (left) to whom he has acted as an adoptive caregiver since their mother's death. B) At age 12, Damien (right) joins his older brother Rollins (left) on a "war-like" territorial boundary patrol, a dangerous activity which can provide food resources to Damien's community. In stills from this video sequence, Damien looks back toward Rollins before moving forward. Photos by Kevin Lee, Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda, 2017. Video by A.S. Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda, 2018.
Fig. 7. Examples of social exploration and social rejection during chimpanzee adolescence. a) Rosa, an adolescent female chimpanzee (left) approaches a young adult male, Haden (right), while exploring outside her neighborhood. Haden grooms his arm and does not respond to Rosa's approach. b) Gus, an adolescent male chimpanzee sits at the outskirts of a social gathering. c) Damien, an adolescent male chimpanzee observes in close proximity as two adult males groom each other, without receiving grooming himself. Photos by R.R., Kevin Lee, and A.S. Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda 2015 -2018.
Fig. 8. A conceptual framework for investigating (1) how social status sensitivity can guide social learning during chimpanzee adolescence while simultaneously exposing adolescents to vulnerabilities (i.e. rejection), (2) how social touch (Box 2) can buffer against such vulnerabilities and promote persistence and learning in uncertain situations, and (3) how both learning, vulnerability and proclivities for social touch during adolescence can be shaped by early life experiences and contribute to later life social outcomes.
Puberty initiates a unique stage of social learning and development prior to adulthood: Insights from studies of adolescence in wild chimpanzees

November 2022

·

174 Reads

·

7 Citations

Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience

In humans, puberty initiates a period of rapid and formative neurobehavioral development. Brain and behavior changes during this maturational window contribute to opportunities for social learning. Here we provide new insights into adolescence as a unique period of social learning and development by describing field studies of our closest living relatives, chimpanzees. Like humans, chimpanzees have a multiyear juvenile life stage between weaning and puberty onset followed by a multiyear adolescent life stage after pubertal onset but prior to socially-recognized adulthood. As they develop increasing autonomy from caregivers, adolescent chimpanzees explore and develop many different types of social relationships with a wide range of individuals in a highly flexible social environment. We describe how adolescent social motivations and experiences differ from those of juveniles and adults and expose adolescents to high levels of uncertainty, risk, and vulnerability, as well as opportunities for adaptive social learning. We discuss how these adolescent learning experiences may be shaped by early life and in turn shape varied adult social outcomes. We outline how future chimpanzee field research can contribute in new ways to a more integrative interdisciplinary understanding of adolescence as a developmental window of adaptive social learning and resilience.


Viruses associated with ill health in wild chimpanzees

January 2022

·

74 Reads

·

13 Citations

American Journal of Primatology

Viral infection is a major cause of ill health in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), but most evidence to date has come from conspicuous disease outbreaks with high morbidity and mortality. To examine the relationship between viral infection and ill health during periods not associated with disease outbreaks, we conducted a longitudinal study of wild eastern chimpanzees (P. t. schweinfurthii) in the Kanyawara and Ngogo communities of Kibale National Park, Uganda. We collected standardized, observational health data for 4 years and then used metagenomics to characterize gastrointestinal viromes (i.e., all viruses recovered from fecal samples) in individual chimpanzees before and during episodes of clinical disease. We restricted our analyses to viruses thought to infect mammals or primarily associated with mammals, discarding viruses associated with nonmammalian hosts. We found 18 viruses (nine of which were previously identified in this population) from at least five viral families. Viral richness (number of viruses per sample) did not vary by health status. By contrast, total viral load (normalized proportion of sequences mapping to viruses) was significantly higher in ill individuals compared with healthy individuals. Furthermore, when ill, Kanyawara chimpanzees exhibited higher viral loads than Ngogo chimpanzees, and males, but not females, exhibited higher infection rates with certain viruses and higher total viral loads as they aged. Post-hoc analyses, including the use of a machine-learning classification method, indicated that one virus, salivirus (Picornaviridae), was the main contributor to health-related and community-level variation in viral loads. Another virus, chimpanzee stool-associated virus (chisavirus; unclassified Picornavirales), was associated with ill health at Ngogo but not at Kanyawara. Chisavirus, chimpanzee adenovirus (Adenoviridae), and bufavirus (Parvoviridae) were also associated with increased age in males. Associations with sex and age are consistent with the hypothesis that nonlethal viral infections cumulatively reflect or contribute to senescence in long-lived species such as chimpanzees. Research Highlights • Some viruses of wild primates are relatively benign but may be useful as biomarkers of physiology and health. • We used metagenomic methods to characterize viral communities in feces of wild chimpanzees in two long-term study communities (Kanyawara and Ngogo) in Kibale National Park, Uganda. • Chimpanzees exhibited higher viral loads (normalized abundances of viral genetic sequences) when they were ill than when those same individuals were previously healthy. • When ill, Kanyawara chimpanzees exhibited higher viral loads than Ngogo chimpanzees, and males, but not females, exhibited higher viral loads as they aged.


A preliminary analysis of wound care and other-regarding behavior in wild chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda

July 2021

·

125 Reads

·

6 Citations

Primates

Caring for others is a key feature of human behavior. Mothers, fathers, siblings, grandparents, and other group members provide care in the form of provisioning, protection, and first aid. To what extent is other-regarding behavior present in our primate relatives? Here we describe an unusual incident of other-regarding behavior toward an injured juvenile female chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) at Ngogo in Kibale National Park, Uganda. After the juvenile received a mild head wound from an adult female, several adolescent and juvenile chimpanzees gathered to touch, lick, and peer at the wound. One adolescent male wiped a leaf across the cut. Another adolescent male later groomed the injured female and briefly carried her. Across a 5-year period, we observed only three other instances of other-directed wound care in chimpanzees, occurring in 4% (4/100) of cases in which we observed individuals with fresh wounds, and 57 other instances of allomaternal carrying. Despite the infrequency of such behaviors, our study adds another chimpanzee field site to the list of those where other-directed wound care has been observed. Observations from wild chimpanzees provide insight into empathy and may inform our understanding of the evolution of other-regarding behavior in humans.


Figure 1. An adult male mounts another adult male, while a third adult male is vigilant to the left (photo by Nathan Chesterman).
Figure 3. Several adult males mount one another immediately after hearing a neighbouring group of chimpanzees, and just prior to an inter-group encounter (photo by Aaron Sandel).
Figure 5. Difference in dominance rank between mounter and mountee. Dotted lines represent ±50 Elo rating difference from zero, indicating comparable ranks.
Sociosexual behaviour between different age and sex combina- tions in chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda.
Contexts of sociosexual behaviour across age-and sex-classes.
Sociosexual behaviour in wild chimpanzees occurs in variable contexts and is frequent between same-sex partners

February 2021

·

1,285 Reads

·

11 Citations

Behaviour

Many animals engage in sociosexual behaviour, including that between same-sex pairs. Bonobos ( Pan paniscus ) are famous for their sociosexual behaviour, but chimpanzees ( Pan troglodytes ) apparently do not engage in sociosexual behaviour frequently. However, sociosexual behaviour in chimpanzees may have been overlooked. We observed 584 instances of sociosexual behaviour in chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda during three years of study. All ages and sexes engaged in sociosexual behaviour, which included mounting, touching of genitals, and pressing genitals together. Most sociosexual behaviour was between adult males. Sociosexual behaviour was often during tense contexts, such as subgroup reunions and during territorial behaviour. Among males, grooming and dominance rank relationships do not explain patterns of sociosexual behaviour. Although sociosexual behaviour may be less frequent in chimpanzees than in bonobos, and bonobos remain distinct in their genito-genital rubbing, our findings suggest that sociosexual behaviour is a regular part of chimpanzee behaviour.


The development of affiliative and coercive reproductive tactics in male chimpanzees

January 2021

·

51 Reads

·

11 Citations

Like many animals, adult male chimpanzees often compete for a limited number of mates. They fight other males as they strive for status that confers reproductive benefits and use aggression to coerce females to mate with them. Nevertheless, small-bodied, socially immature adolescent male chimpanzees, who cannot compete with older males for status nor intimidate females, father offspring. We investigated how they do so through a study of adolescent and young adult males at Ngogo in Kibale National Park, Uganda. Adolescent males mated with nulliparous females and reproduced primarily with these first-time mothers, who are not preferred as mating partners by older males. Two other factors, affiliation and aggression, also influenced mating success. Specifically, the strength of affiliative bonds that males formed with females and the amount of aggression males directed toward females predicted male mating success. The effect of male aggression toward females on mating success increased as males aged, especially when they directed it toward females with whom they shared affiliative bonds. These results mirror sexual coercion in humans, which occurs most often between males and females involved in close, affiliative relationships.


Social relationships between chimpanzee sons and mothers endure but change during adolescence and adulthood

December 2020

·

385 Reads

·

22 Citations

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

Mothers provide indispensable care for infants in many mammalian species. In some long-lived species, the maternal-offspring bond persists after infancy with mothers continuing to provide resources and social support to their adult progeny. Maternal presence is associated with fitness benefits through adolescence for male chimpanzees despite the fact that mature males dominate females and form their strongest bonds with other males. How mothers support grown sons is unknown, because few studies have examined developmental shifts in mother-son relationships during adolescence and adulthood. We investigated social interactions between 29 adolescent (9–15 years) and young adult male (16–20 years) chimpanzees and their mothers at Ngogo in Kibale National Park, Uganda, over 3 years. All males under 12 years old had their mother as their top grooming and proximity partner, as did one-third of the young adult males. As males grew older, the amount of time they associated with, maintained proximity to, groomed with, and kept track of their mothers while traveling decreased. When males were together in the same party as their mothers, however, young adult males affiliated with their mothers as frequently as did adolescent males, with sons initiating the majority of these interactions. In contrast to adult sons, however, adolescent sons became distressed when separated from mothers and relied on their mothers for agonistic support and reassurance after conflicts. These findings indicate that the chimpanzee maternal-offspring bond continues but changes through adolescence and adulthood, with mothers remaining occasional social companions for most adult sons and frequent companions for some. Significance statement Mammalian mothers protect and provision their infants, and in some species, mothers provide social support for their adult offspring. The importance of mothers in the lives of adult sons is clear in humans and in one of our closest relatives, bonobos. Here we show mothers are also important social partners for sons throughout adolescence and into young adulthood in chimpanzees. Despite prevailing knowledge that adult males are key alliance partners for male chimpanzees, we demonstrate that male chimpanzees maintain social bonds with their mothers into adulthood. We also suggest that social bonds reflect not only the total time spent together, but what animals do during those times, as many young adults rarely groomed their mothers, but at times when they were in the same subgroup with her, they did so frequently. Future research will assess the physiological, psychological, and reproductive benefits of such mother-son bonds.


Citations (17)


... For example, chimpanzee juveniles 87 begin facing threats from adult males soon after weaning around the age of 4 to 5 years, 88 whereas bonobos experience such threats closer to puberty around the age of 8 (Hohmann et 89 al., 2019; Kuroda, 1989). Furthermore, chimpanzee mothers in Ngogo, Uganda, have been 90 observed to actively support their offspring, intervening more frequently during infant attacks compared to bonobo mothers in Kokolopori, Democratic Republic of Congo (DRC), who 92 intervene less often (Reddy et al., 2024). This difference may reflect lower aggression levels in 93 bonobo societies and a reduced need for maternal protection. ...

Reference:

Great Ape Childhoods: Development of infant bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii) in the wild
Maternal conflict intervention is more frequent in chimpanzee compared to bonobo development
  • Citing Article
  • December 2023

Animal Behaviour

... Longitudinal data on linear growth in non-human primates is relatively rare and presents challenges when comparing between studies due to variation in methodology and analysis (See Berghaenel et al. 2023). An alternative approach for attempting to assess growth parameters by Sandel et al. (2023) uses urinary biomarkers of bone turnover, osteocalcin, and Type I collagen, to examine growth rates in a sample of 109 wild chimpanzees aged between 2.5-66 years over a duration of 2 years. In this study, a clear peak in osteocalcin and collagen was found at 9.4 and 10.8 years in male chimpanzees, coinciding with early and middle adolescence, but this pattern was not observed in females. ...

The evolution of the adolescent growth spurt: Urinary biomarkers of bone turnover in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)
  • Citing Article
  • March 2023

Journal of Human Evolution

... This aligns with existing research suggesting that prolonged exposure to trauma has lasting effects on chimpanzee behavior (Kalcher et al. 2008;Kalcher-Sommersguter et al. 2013), which have been compared with complex posttraumatic stress disorder (C-PTSD) in humans with behaviors such as social withdrawal and self-harm (Ferdowsian et al. 2011;Herman 1992;Dyer et al. 2009;Lopresti-Goodman, Kameka, and Dube 2012;Bradshaw et al. 2008;Cantor and Price 2007). The implications of this finding reinforce the well-established importance of socialization during infancy (Lopresti-Goodman, Kameka, and Dube 2012;Crailsheim et al. 2020;Kalcher-Sommersguter et al. 2015;Snyder et al. 1984) and adolescence (Reddy, Sandel, and Dahl 2022) for animal psycho-social development. ...

Puberty initiates a unique stage of social learning and development prior to adulthood: Insights from studies of adolescence in wild chimpanzees

Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience

... The QIamp MinElute Virus Spin Kit (Qiagen) was employed to extract nucleic acids according to the manufacturer's protocol but omitting carrier RNA. Then, cDNA was synthesized using the Superscript IV kit (Thermo Fisher) and random hexamer priming and purified with Agencourt AMPure XP beads (Beckman Coulter) as described previously [25][26][27][28]. The Illumina Nextera XT kit (Illumina) was employed to produce genomic libraries, which were sequenced on an Illumina MiSeq using 300×300 cycle (V3) paired-end chemistry. ...

Viruses associated with ill health in wild chimpanzees
  • Citing Article
  • January 2022

American Journal of Primatology

... However, licking (Volta's blood) and biting softly, which have not been reported as behaviors toward collapsed and inanimate subjects, were observed toward Volta. Chimpanzees show curiosity about new injuries in other individuals and perform inspecting behavior, as well as grooming and licking wounds 2,12,23 . Volta was temporarily collapsed by definition when discovered by M. S., but even then multiple adult males licked Volta's blood. ...

A preliminary analysis of wound care and other-regarding behavior in wild chimpanzees at Ngogo, Kibale National Park, Uganda

Primates

... Although much more attention has been placed on bonobo sexuality, there is evidence that chimpanzees also engage in sociosexual behaviour in similar contexts. Mixed-sex and same-sex genital touching and mounting have been observed across ages in chimpanzees [30] and, like bonobos, occur frequently during chimpanzee play [31]. Genital touching-hand or foot contact to another's anogenital region-is used often in greetings [32], where it appears to represent a form of reassurance, occurring often during post-conflict (PC) affiliation with uninvolved bystanders and reconciliation with aggressors too [33][34][35]. ...

Sociosexual behaviour in wild chimpanzees occurs in variable contexts and is frequent between same-sex partners

Behaviour

... 5,9 A few recent studies have investigated the benefits of coercion to males, 10 or the sources of variation among males in coercive tendencies. 11 Much less is known, however, about the strategies used by females to mitigate their exposure to coercion and its associated costs. ...

The development of affiliative and coercive reproductive tactics in male chimpanzees

... While most feeding skills are acquired by weaning age 44 , which typically occurs between 3 and 5 years 45 , several skills involving tool use can take 10-15 years to master 18,46 , and may involve sensitive learning periods 47,48 . Chimpanzees also have a protracted dependency and remain in association with their mothers for much of the first decade of life 49 . During this time, mothers may provide unique learning opportunities compared to other role models, particularly in complex tasks, such as nut-cracking, termite fishing, and ant dipping 46,50,51 . ...

Social relationships between chimpanzee sons and mothers endure but change during adolescence and adulthood

Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology

... Adulthood is often linked to social rather physical changes. Male chimpanzees in the wild are considered adults at around 16 years when they dominate adult females, become integrated into the social world of adults, and appear to reach full body size (Pusey, 1990;Muller et al., 2004;Reddy and Mitani, 2020;Sandel et al., 2022). Females are considered adults when they have their first offspring. ...

Adolescent and young adult male chimpanzees form affiliative, yet aggressive, relationships with females
  • Citing Article
  • July 2020

Journal of Human Evolution

... Tomasello's "shared intentionality" hypothesis and his ontogenetic inferences continue to meet skepticism from many primatologists. More observational attention paid to mother-offspring interactions in other apes (e.g., Reddy and Wellman, 2020;Tkaczynski et al., 2020;Heesen et al., 2021;Behringer et al., 2022) will continue to both complicate and clarify cross-species variation. As that work proceeds, the importance and explanatory reach of life history evolution is expanded by Sarah Hrdy's insights about novel survival challenges faced by infants ancestral to us (Hrdy, 2005(Hrdy, , 2009(Hrdy, , 2014(Hrdy, , 2016Hawkes, 2014) Hawkes and Finlay (2018), Finlay (2019), and Hawkes (2020a). ...

Caregiving relationships as evolutionary and developmental bases of obligation
  • Citing Article
  • April 2020

Behavioral and Brain Sciences