August 2023
·
12 Reads
Academy of Management Proceedings
This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.
August 2023
·
12 Reads
Academy of Management Proceedings
August 2023
·
9 Reads
Academy of Management Proceedings
August 2023
·
29 Reads
Academy of Management Proceedings
August 2022
·
82 Reads
·
4 Citations
Current Opinion in Psychology
In everyday life, people often have opportunities to improve others’ lives, whether offering well-intentioned advice or complimenting someone on a job well done. These are opportunities to provide “prosocial input” (information intended to benefit others), including feedback, advice, compliments, and expressions of gratitude. Despite widespread evidence that giving prosocial input can improve the well-being of both givers and recipients, people sometimes hesitate to offer their input. The current paper documents when and why people fail to give prosocial input, noting that potential givers overestimate the costs of doing so (e.g., making recipients uncomfortable) and underestimate the benefits (e.g., being helpful) for at least four psychological reasons. Unfortunately, the reluctance to give prosocial input results in a short supply of kindness.
August 2022
·
10 Reads
Academy of Management Proceedings
August 2022
·
34 Reads
Academy of Management Proceedings
June 2022
·
43 Reads
·
6 Citations
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
How does the self-relevance of a social movement shape individuals' engagement with it? We examined the decision-making processes that underlie support for Black Lives Matter (BLM) among Black, Hispanic, Asian, and White Americans. We find significant between-group differences in levels of support for BLM, both in terms of past behavior (Study 1) and in terms of future intentions to support the movement (Study 2). These differences notwithstanding, thinking about how one's decisions impact others - which we label impact mindset - explains support for BLM across racial groups, cross-sectionally as well as longitudinally (over 8 months later). Our findings underscore the equivalence of the impact mindset construct across racial groups and its predictive power in the context of BLM. We conclude that, although the struggle for racial justice has different meanings for different racial groups, the same mindset underlies both in-group advocacy and allyship in the context of BLM.
January 2022
·
108 Reads
·
32 Citations
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
Allyship is a growing phenomenon in many organizational contexts, and the involvement of advantaged group allies in identity-oriented social movements (e.g., men in the feminist movement) is ubiquitous. However, the impression that these advantaged group allies make on their intended beneficiaries is unclear. Over the course of four studies, we explore disadvantaged group activists' attitudes toward their advantaged group allies. We find converging evidence that disadvantaged group activists prefer advantaged group allies who engage in actions that demonstrate high levels of trustworthiness (e.g., selflessness, loyalty) and low levels of influence (e.g., centrality, power) in the movement, whereas non-activists show only a significant preference on the influence dimension. This evidence was observed in a survey of 117 social movement activists (Study 1), and in three experiments with 752 liberal women and nonbinary individuals (Study 2), 305 feminist social movement activists (Study 3), and a separate sample of 805 feminist social movement activists (Study 4). Taken together, our research documents the causal effects that different allyship behaviors have on beneficiaries' attitudes toward advantaged group allies (Studies 2, 3, & 4) while recruiting samples of currently engaged movement activists to solicit their unique perspectives (Studies 1, 3, & 4). We thereby identify the specific ways of being an advantaged group ally that elicit the most positive impressions from their intended beneficiaries, which have direct implications for supporting intergroup coalitions and social change.
November 2021
·
51 Reads
·
3 Citations
Psychological Science
Whom do individuals blame for intergroup conflict? Do people attribute responsibility for intergroup conflict to the in-group or the out-group? Theoretically integrating the literatures on intergroup relations, moral psychology, and judgment and decision-making, we propose that unpacking a group by explicitly describing it in terms of its constituent subgroups increases perceived support for the view that the unpacked group shoulders more of the blame for intergroup conflict. Five preregistered experiments ( N = 3,335 adults) found support for this novel hypothesis across three distinct intergroup conflicts: the Israeli-Palestinian conflict, current racial tensions between White people and Black people in the United States, and the gender gap in wages in the United States. Our findings (a) highlight the independent roles that entrenched social identities and cognitive, presentation-based processes play in shaping blame judgments, (b) demonstrate that the effect of unpacking groups generalizes across partisans and nonpartisans, and (c) illustrate how constructing packed versus unpacked sets of potential perpetrators can critically shape where the blame lies.
April 2020
·
158 Reads
·
1 Citation
Allyship is a growing phenomenon in many organizational contexts, and the involvement of allies in identity-based social movements (e.g., men in the feminist movement) is ubiquitous. However, the impression that these allies have on their intended beneficiaries is unclear. Over the course of three studies, we explore how different types of allyship behaviors are perceived by their beneficiaries. We find converging evidence that beneficiaries make critical judgments of their allies when their allies engage in actions that demonstrate lower levels of trustworthiness (e.g., selflessness, loyalty) and higher levels of influence (e.g., centrality, power) in the movement. This evidence was observed in a survey of 117 social movement activists (Study 1), and in two experiments sampling 752 liberal women and nonbinary individuals (Study 2), and 305 feminist social activists (Study 3). Taken together, our research documents the causal effects that different allyship behaviors have on beneficiaries’ attitudes toward allies (Studies 2 & 3) while recruiting samples of currently engaged movement activists to solicit their unique perspectives (Studies 1 & 3). We thereby identify the specific ways of being an ally that elicit the most positive impressions from their intended beneficiaries, which can reinforce intergroup coalitions, prosociality, and ultimately, downstream societal change.
... Our work has important practical implications for leaders' family members as well. Although employees may hesitate to express gratitude at work because it may feel awkward or risky (e.g., Abel et al., 2022;Kumar, 2022;Kumar & Epley, 2018), expressing gratitude within the safe and familiar environment of the home may be less uncomfortable (e.g., Grandey & Krannitz, 2016). Our work suggests that gratitude expressions at home may be beneficial not only for the recipient leader but also for their colleagues because experienced gratitude motivates prosocial acts. ...
August 2022
Current Opinion in Psychology
... Do Black Americans have more empathy for their fellow Black Americans than White Americans have sympathy for them? It appears, for instance, that a racial empathy gap exists between White and Black Americans in support for punitive approaches to crime (Unnever their feeling towards social movements like Black Lives Matter (Drakulich et al., 2021;Luttrell, 2019;Vani et al., 2023), whether they believe the violence perpetrated by the police against Black Americans is justified , and whether they have empathy for those Black victims (J. Johnson & Lecci, 2020). ...
June 2022
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin
... However, we believe that our theoretical argument about intergroup attribution bias has a more general scope, and we expect it to apply to other contexts in which clearly delineated groups have perpetrated violence (including intrastate wars). Attribution biases have been documented in Burundi, Indonesia, Israel, Turkey, and the United States, among other countries (Ariyanto et al., 2009;Bilali et al., 2012;Halevy et al., 2022). While limited studies examine the impact of such biases on attitudes toward specific peace provisions, several studies did document how war-related social identities are correlated with political attitudes more broadly. ...
November 2021
Psychological Science
... In particular, Radke et al. (2022) found that when allies to Black Americans and women used more dominant styles of communication in their allyship efforts (e.g., speaking over others), they were perceived as more motivated to make themselves the center of attention and in turn were evaluated as less desirable candidates for collaborative advocacy. Similarly, in a series of experimental studies, Park et al. (2022) found that activists from gender minoritized groups perceived allies from advantaged groups (e.g., cisgender men) less favorably when they believed these allies to be both less trustworthy (e.g., less selfless) and more influential (i.e., attempting to center their power in the movement). Nonactivists from disadvantaged groups perceived advantaged group allies less favorably when they believed them to be more influential and focused on gaining attention. ...
January 2022
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology