Peter M. Todd’s research while affiliated with Indiana University Bloomington and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (253)


Sample characteristics
The size of circles shows the number of respondents. We created Fig 1 with OpenStreetMap® (https://www.openstreetmap.org). OpenStreetMap is open data, licensed under the Open Data Commons Open Database License (ODbL) by the OpenStreetMap Foundation (OSMF). According to OpenStreetMap “You are free to copy, distribute, transmit and adapt our data, as long as you credit OpenStreetMap and its contributors".
Inclinations to cooperate from March-August 2020
Black lines show the marginal effect of time, blue lines show the Loess curve (i.e., non-parametric line of best fit), and gray lines show the person-specific effect of time. Shaded bands show 95% CIs. Willingness to help (1 = not at all willing, 7 = very willing) neighbors (a) and humanity (c), as well as need-based helping attitude (1 = do not agree at all, 7 = strongly agree) toward neighbors (b) and humanity (d) decreased by a small margin over time.
Perceived interdependence from March-August 2020
Black lines show the marginal effect of time, blue lines show the Loess curve (i.e., non-parametric line of best fit), and gray lines show the person-specific effect of time. Shaded bands show 95% CIs. Emotional (a) and perceived (b) shared fate with neighbors (1 = do not agree at all, 7 = strongly agree), and perceived shared fate with humanity (d) increased by a small margin over time. Emotional shared fate with humanity (c) decreased by a small margin over time.
Changes in willingness to help neighbors
Willingness to help neighbors (Someone from your neighborhood is having their residence fixed, so it isn’t livable. How willing would you be to let them move into your residence for a week?; 1 = not at all willing, 7 = very willing) decreased over time when people experienced higher (blue; a), but not when people experienced lower (red; a), COVID-19 prevalence. Willingness to help decreased over time for people who lived in areas with high (blue; b), but not low (red; b), COVID-19 prevalence. Willingness to help decreased more over time for people with high (blue; c), than for people with low (red; c), trait perceived infection risk. Willingness to help decreased over time for people with low (red; d), but not for people with high (blue; d), perceived shared fate with neighbors. Shaded bands show 95% CIs.
Changes in need-based helping attitude toward neighbors
Need-based helping attitude (Helping someone from my neighborhood when they are in need is the right thing to do; 1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree) decreased by a small margin over time for people with high trait perceived infection risk (blue; a), but not for people with low trait perceived infection risk (red; a). Need-based helping attitude decreased over time for people with low emotional shared fate with neighbors (red; b), but increased over time for people with high emotional shared fate with neighbors (blue; b). Shaded bands show 95% CIs.

+7

Shared fate was associated with sustained cooperation during the COVID-19 pandemic
  • Article
  • Full-text available

September 2024

·

45 Reads

·

·

·

[...]

·

Did the COVID-19 pandemic bring people together or push them apart? While infectious diseases tend to push people apart, crises can also bring people together through positive interdependence. We studied this question by asking an international sample (N = 1,006) about their inclinations to cooperate, perceptions of interdependence (i.e., shared fate), and perceived risk as well as local prevalence of COVID-19 infection across 14 time points from March to August, 2020. While perceived interdependence with others tended to increase during this time period, inclinations to cooperate decreased over time. At the within-person level, higher local prevalence of COVID-19 attenuated increases in perceived interdependence with others, and was associated with lower inclinations to cooperate. At the between-person level, people with high perceived interdependence with others reported more stable, or increasing, inclinations to cooperate over time than people with low perceived interdependence. Establishing a high sense of perceived interdependence with others may thus allow people to maintain cooperation during crises, even in the face of challenging circumstances such as those posed by a highly transmissible virus.

Download

Figure 1. Conceptual model showing the antecedents of cultivating empathic concern. Wealth buffers the frequency of resource-related challenges, and allows people to absorb the costs of challenges when they arise. Experiencing challenges motivates individuals to invest in social safety nets by way of helping others in times of need. Over time, helping people in need affords opportunities to deploy, and hence strengthen, a disposition towards feeling empathic concern.
Figure 2. Sequential mediation model predicting trait empathic concern (Study 1). Panel a: Indirect effects showed that (1) wealthier people experienced fewer challenges, in turn helping others less often, (2) people who experienced more challenges help others more often, in turn reporting higher trait empathic concern, and (3) wealthier people experienced fewer challenges and helped others less often, in turn reporting lower trait empathic concern. Panel b: Wealthier people experienced fewer challenges over the past year. Panel c: People who experienced more challenges helped others more often. Panel d: People who helped others more often reported higher trait empathic concern. Note. Panel a: Solid lines indicate statistically significant paths, dashed lines indicate not statistically significant paths (p > 0.05), and brackets show 95% CIs. *** = p < 0.001, ** = p < 0.01, * = p < 0.05.
Figure 4. Sequential mediation model predicting changes in trait empathic concern (Study 2). Panel a: Among people who helped less often (region to the left of the blue dashed line in panel d), indirect effects show that (1) wealthier people had fewer challenges, in turn helping less often, (2) people who had more challenges gave help more often, in turn reporting higher trait empathic concern, and (3) wealthier people had fewer challenges, in turn helping less often and reporting lower trait empathic concern. However, (4) wealthier people also helped more often, in turn reporting higher trait empathic concern. Among people who helped more often (region to the right of the red dashed line in panel d), indirect effects show that (1) wealthier people, and (2) people who had more challenges, helped others more often, in turn reporting lower trait empathic concern. Panel b: Participants who reported higher wealth had fewer challenges over the past year. Panel c: People who had more challenges helped others more often (y-axis shows mean help given). Panel d: Among people who helped others less often, people who helped more often reported higher trait empathic concern (region to the left of the blue dashed line), while among people who helped others more often, people who helped more often reported lower empathic concern (region to the right of the red dashed line) (x-axis shows the standardized mean help given). Note. Panel a: Black lines show latent paths, gray lines show covariances. Solid lines indicate statistically significant paths, dashed lines indicate not statistically significant paths (p > 0.05), and brackets show 95% CIs. *** = p < 0.001, ** = p < 0.01, * = p < 0.05. Panels b-d: Solid blue lines indicate linear effects, and the solid red line indicates curvilinear effect.
Figure 5. Giving help is associated with state-level empathic concern. Panel a: Among people who helped others less often (region to the left of the blue dashed line), more helping was associated with higher state-level empathic concern. Among people who helped others more often (region to the right of the red dashed line), more helping was associated with lower empathic concern. Panel b: Giving more help than usual, relative to one’s own overall level of helping, was associated with higher state-level empathic concern up to a point (region to the left of the blue dashed line). Beyond this point (region to the right of the blue dashed line), giving more help was not associated with increases in empathic concern. Panel c: State-level empathic concern decreased by a small margin over time (the blue line represents the Loess function, and gray lines show each person’s linear trajectory over time). Panel d: State-level empathic concern decreased over time in months in which, relative to one's own overall level of helping, people gave less help than usual (red), but not in months in which people gave more help than usual (blue). Note. Panels a-b: Solid blue lines indicate linear effects, and solid red lines indicate curvilinear effects.
Helping in Times of Need Cultivates Empathic Concern Over Time

September 2024

·

58 Reads

We test the hypothesis that wealth is associated with lower empathic concern because wealthier people experience fewer challenges and give help less often to people in need. As predicted, Study 1 (N = 513; cross-sectional) showed wealth was negatively associated with trait empathic concern through experiencing fewer challenges and providing less help. Study 2 (N = 915; longitudinal) replicated this while also revealing unexpected curvilinear associations. At low levels of helping (measured monthly across one year), wealth led to lower empathy through experiencing fewer challenges and less helping. However, more helping led to decreasing empathy at higher levels of helping. Moreover, person-centered analyses showed that people experienced higher state-level empathic concern in months in which they provided moderate amounts of help relative to when they helped less than their usual amount of helping. Results suggest people can cultivate empathy through helping, but only for those who help a moderate amount.


Neural evidence of switch processes during semantic and phonetic foraging in human memory

October 2023

·

151 Reads

·

16 Citations

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

Humans may retrieve words from memory by exploring and exploiting in “semantic space” similar to how nonhuman animals forage for resources in physical space. This has been studied using the verbal fluency test (VFT), in which participants generate words belonging to a semantic or phonetic category in a limited time. People produce bursts of related items during VFT, referred to as “clustering” and “switching.” The strategic foraging model posits that cognitive search behavior is guided by a monitoring process which detects relevant declines in performance and then triggers the searcher to seek a new patch or cluster in memory after the current patch has been depleted. An alternative body of research proposes that this behavior can be explained by an undirected rather than strategic search process, such as random walks with or without random jumps to new parts of semantic space. This study contributes to this theoretical debate by testing for neural evidence of strategically timed switches during memory search. Thirty participants performed category and letter VFT during functional MRI. Responses were classified as cluster or switch events based on computational metrics of similarity and participant evaluations. Results showed greater hippocampal and posterior cerebellar activation during switching than clustering, even while controlling for interresponse times and linguistic distance. Furthermore, these regions exhibited ramping activity which increased during within-patch search leading up to switches. Findings support the strategic foraging model, clarifying how neural switch processes may guide memory search in a manner akin to foraging in patchy spatial environments.


Food scarcity and disease concern reduce interdependence when people eat together

July 2023

·

126 Reads

·

1 Citation

European Journal of Social Psychology

Food sharing is central to the human experience, serving biological and sociocultural functions. Historically, food sharing has allowed people to manage food shortages, creating positive interdependence among those who pool calories and other sources of risk. However, sharing food may lead to negative outcomes when food is scarce, or when there is a threat of disease. We found that sharing food (compared with sharing pencils) led to reduced cooperation with an experiment partner (Study 1) and that perceived scarcity partially mediated a negative association between zero-sum orientation and perceived interdependence with people involved in a recently shared meal (Studies 2-3). Disgust was also associated with lower perceived interdependence toward people involved in a shared meal (Study 3; Ntotal = 1126). Our results suggest that scarcity and disgust can interfere with the positive feelings people might otherwise experience when eating together, warning against lay beliefs that 'breaking bread’ necessarily brings people together.


Timeline of self-reported mask wearing and perceived social norms in the United States during the COVID-19 pandemic. (a) Points and line ranges indicate means ± two standard errors for the self-reported mask wearing item. This item was measured across all 18 time points on a 5-point Likert scale, with higher values indicating increased frequency of personal mask wearing during in-person interactions. (b) Points and line ranges indicate means ± two standard errors for perceived injunctive mask wearing norms (green) and perceived descriptive mask wearing norms (blue). These items were measured across eleven time points on a 7-point Likert scale, with higher values indicating stronger perceived social norms. (c) Smoothed data for daily new COVID-19 cases in the United States, displayed on the log scale (data retrieved from Our World in Data; https://ourworldindata.org/). Across all panels, gray dashed lines represent significant pandemic-related events in the United States, such as vaccine approval from the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) and public health recommendations from the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC).
Path diagram of ten-wave time-varying random-intercept cross-lagged panel model. Circles represent data collection time points. Arrows represent within-person autoregressive effects (on one horizontal level) and cross-lagged effects (across levels) for mask wearing and perceived descriptive and injunctive norms, partitioning out stable between-person individual differences and controlling for factual beliefs, personal normative beliefs, demographics, and political orientation. Arrow thickness is scaled according to standardized effect size. Bolded arrows indicate significantly positive parameters, p < 0.05. Gray arrows indicate non-significant parameters.
Standardized cross-lagged coefficients for descriptive norms and injunctive norms predicting future mask wearing in the ten-wave time-varying random-intercept cross-lagged panel model. Points are standardized estimates, lines are 95% confidence intervals.
Descriptive norms caused increases in mask wearing during the COVID-19 pandemic

July 2023

·

56 Reads

·

10 Citations

Human sociality is governed by two types of social norms: injunctive norms, which prescribe what people ought to do, and descriptive norms, which reflect what people actually do. The process by which these norms emerge and their causal influences on cooperative behavior over time are not well understood. Here, we study these questions through social norms influencing mask wearing during the COVID-19 pandemic. Leveraging 2 years of data from the United States (18 time points; n = 915), we tracked mask wearing and perceived injunctive and descriptive mask wearing norms as the pandemic unfolded. Longitudinal trends suggested that norms and behavior were tightly coupled, changing quickly in response to public health recommendations. In addition, longitudinal modeling revealed that descriptive norms caused future increases in mask wearing across multiple waves of data collection. These cross-lagged causal effects of descriptive norms were large, even after controlling for non-social beliefs and demographic variables. Injunctive norms, by contrast, had less frequent and generally weaker causal effects on future mask wearing. During uncertain times, cooperative behavior is more strongly driven by what others are actually doing, rather than what others think ought to be done.


Timeline of survey questions asked throughout the longitudinal study. Each circle represents a month with the larger, white-filled ones indicating months where survey data were collected and the small black ones indicating intermediate months
Average 2019 environmental/occupational SEV values across the United States (left), for our respondents (center), and average PUMR of our participants (right). States without data in our study are marked in gray
Correlations matrix between perceptions of mortality risk from specific causes and perception of uncontrollable mortality risk
Scatter plot and best fit line between PUMR measured in March (X axis) and PUMR measured at the later dates: May (Red) and July (Green)
Objective risk exposure, perceived uncontrollable mortality risk, and health behaviors

July 2023

·

67 Reads

·

5 Citations

Journal of Public Health

Aim Perceived uncontrollable mortality risk (PUMR) refers to people's beliefs regarding their risk of death due to factors outside of their control. Previous theoretical models and empirical studies provide evidence that those with greater PUMR are less motivated to invest in preventative health behaviors, but little is known about how accurately people estimate PUMR compared to objective measures of risk exposure, an important consideration for interventions designed to address the link between PUMR and health behavior. Here, we explore how objective risk indices and personal characteristics relate to PUMR. Subject and methods We performed a series of pre-registered analyses on a US-representative longitudinal study (N = 915), connecting these results to external data from the Global Burden of Diseases, Injuries, and Risk Factors Study. Results We show that (Study 1) PUMR is associated with objective measures of risk exposure, and that (Study 2) perceptions of risk due to disease drive PUMR, and more educated individuals report less perceived risk. Additionally, we find that (Study 3) estimates of PUMR are relatively stable over a 4-month period (R = 0.7), indicating that behaviors influenced by PUMR are likely to persist over time. Finally, we show that (Study 4) those who believe they are at greater risk of dying due to factors outside of their control (i.e., greater PUMR) are less likely to engage in general health behaviors. Conclusion By assessing the determinants of PUMR, we can create data-driven policy solutions that lead individuals to more accurate mortality risk assessments and improved health behavior.


COVID-19 and friendships: Agreeableness and neuroticism are associated with more concern about COVID-19 and friends' risky behaviors

June 2023

·

21 Reads

·

5 Citations

Personality and Individual Differences

Given the importance of friendships during challenging times and the mixed associations between personality traits and disease-related behaviors, we investigated the correlations between personality traits and perceptions of friendships during the COVID-19 pandemic. Data were collected as part of a longitudinal investigation of the correlations between the pandemic and various cooperative relationships. In this investigation, we found that agreeableness and neuroticism predicted participants being more concerned about COVID-19 and bothered by friends' risky behavior, and extraversion predicted enjoying helping friends during the pandemic. Our results suggest that personality differences are associated with how individuals cope with friends' risky behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic.


Unpredictable Needs are Associated with Lower Expectations of Repayment

March 2023

·

82 Reads

·

1 Citation

Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology

Sometimes people help one another expecting to be repaid, while at other times people help without an expectation of repayment. What might underlie this difference in expectations of repayment? We investigate this question in a nationally representative sample of US adults (N = 915), and find that people are more likely to expect repayment when needs are perceived to be more predictable. We then replicate these findings in a new sample of US adults (N = 417), and show that people have higher expectations of repayment when needs are perceived to be more predictable because people assign greater responsibility to others for experiencing such predictable needs (e.g., needing money for utilities). This is consistent with previous work based on smaller-scale societies, which shows that the predictability of needs influences expectations of repayment. Our results also add to this previous work by (1) showing that the positive relationship between predictability of needs and expectations of repayment previously found in smaller-scale communities is generalizable to the US population, and (2) showing that attributions of responsibility partially mediate this relationship. This work shows that the predictability of needs and attributions of responsibility for that need are important factors underlying the psychology of helping in times of need.


Wishing our friends would take it seriously: What predicts concern about friends’ behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic?

February 2023

·

15 Reads

The COVID-19 pandemic drastically changed the ways in which social interactions happened. This is particularly true for valued social relationships such as friendships. Friendships provide many physical and mental health benefits, including buffering against loneliness during the pandemic, so contact and interactions with friends may have become more important during the pandemic. Additionally, an additional benefit of friendships is that they may have provided individuals with the ability to reduce their uncertainty and lower their risks during the pandemic. But, if individuals or their friends are not being careful about their risky behavior during this time, this could lead to conflict within these valued relationships. In this study, we investigated how COVID-19, demographics, and preferences for risk management influenced participants’ perceptions of their friendship during the pandemic. We found that participants who were more concerned about contracting COVID-19 and more likely to use risk retention reported having more conflict with their friends during the pandemic. These results suggest that there may be individual differences in risk tolerance of contracting COVID-19 and these differences fundamentally influence social relationships, loneliness, or social connectedness during the pandemic.


Descriptive, not injunctive, social norms caused increases in mask wearing during the COVID-19 pandemic

December 2022

·

16 Reads

Human sociality is governed by two types of social norms: injunctive social norms, which prescribe what people ought to do, and descriptive social norms, which reflect what people actually do. These norms enable people to cooperate in the face of group-wide challenges. While previous experimental work has shown that people’s behavior is influenced by social norms, several open questions remain about the natural emergence of injunctive and descriptive social norms within populations and their causal influences on cooperative behavior over time. To understand how cooperative behavior emerges and is shaped by changing social norms in a non-experimental setting, we studied mask wearing during the COVID-19 pandemic. Mask wearing has individual benefits, but it is also a cooperative behavior that provides collective benefits of reduced disease transmission in the community. Leveraging two years of longitudinal data from a representative sample of adults in the United States (18 time points; n = 915), we tracked people’s reported mask wearing and their perceived injunctive and descriptive mask wearing norms as the pandemic unfolded. Longitudinal trends of norm perceptions and self-reported mask wearing suggested that norms and behavior were tightly coupled and both changed quickly in response to recommendations from public health authorities. In addition, a random-intercept cross-lagged panel model revealed that perceived descriptive norms, but not perceived injunctive norms, caused future within-person increases in individuals' mask wearing. These findings show that, during uncertain times, cooperative behavior is driven by what others are actually doing, rather than what others think ought to be done.


Citations (75)


... Verbal fluency is a direct expression of cognitive flexibility, demonstrating the efficiency and effectiveness of cognitive processes involved in generating language (Amunts et al., 2020(Amunts et al., , 2021Filipe et al., 2023). It measures how well individuals can generate words based on semantic and phonemic cues, demonstrating their ability to switch between cognitive sets and retrieve information from memory (Kavé & Sapir-Yogev, 2020;Lundin et al., 2023). This study examines how the ecological and cultural practices of the Oroqen community, such as traditional hunting and associated practices, influence their cognitive abilities and verbal fluency. ...

Reference:

Eco-cultural influences on cognitive flexibility: a comparative analysis of verbal fluency in Oroqen hunters and Han populations
Neural evidence of switch processes during semantic and phonetic foraging in human memory
  • Citing Article
  • October 2023

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

... However, the COVID-19 pandemic might have also created negative interdependence by way of disease transmission. In contrast to shared fate, infection risk tends to induce avoidance, reducing shared fate and inhibiting cooperation [5][6][7][8]. Hence, not only did the COVID-19 pandemic evoke the duality of human nature observed during previous crises, but people also had to weigh their inclinations to cooperate against an increasing risk of infection [9][10][11]. ...

Food scarcity and disease concern reduce interdependence when people eat together

European Journal of Social Psychology

... The novelty and uncertainty surrounding the pandemic also contributed to a lack of clear guidelines for collective behavior (e.g., mask wearing) during the early periods of the pandemic [59]. And even when clear guidelines for collective behavior became widespread, adherence to such guidelines varied widely [25,[60][61][62]. The transmissibility of the disease and general uncertainty surrounding the pandemic posed a challenge to perceiving positive interdependence and engaging in cooperation because the risk of infection and the risk of defection leads to the possibility of negative fitness interdependence (e.g., becoming infected when helping a person in need, helping an exploitative person). ...

Descriptive norms caused increases in mask wearing during the COVID-19 pandemic

... Our specific focus on health behaviour (rather than energetic allocation to growth, reproduction or somatic maintenance) at the individual level (rather than the species level) differentiates the Uncontrollable Mortality Risk Hypothesis from the life history theory which inspired it. Furthermore, the behavioural ecological model from Nettle [19] underlying the Uncontrollable Mortality Risk Hypothesis, along with subsequent research [18,35,41,42], previously referred to 'extrinsic mortality risk', whereas 'uncontrollable mortality risk' has been used more recently to refer to mortality risk that cannot be reduced by behaviour [43][44][45][46]. This is because the definition of 'extrinsic mortality risk' employed by evolutionary models for understanding senescence [47][48][49][50][51] differs from that of human health behaviour literature relevant to the Uncontrollable Mortality Risk Hypothesis [15,18,19,41,44,46,[52][53][54]. ...

Objective risk exposure, perceived uncontrollable mortality risk, and health behaviors

Journal of Public Health

... The rationale for opting for the Donnellan scale lies in its brevity and efficiency, which aligns well with the busy and hectic work schedules typically encountered by IT professionals (Prabhu et al., 2023). Given that our target population often faces time constraints and demanding workloads (Gonibeed and Saqib, 2023), it's crucial to minimize the burden of survey participation while still capturing relevant personality traits (Ayers et al., 2023). An example statement from the scale is "I am the life of the party." ...

COVID-19 and friendships: Agreeableness and neuroticism are associated with more concern about COVID-19 and friends' risky behaviors
  • Citing Article
  • June 2023

Personality and Individual Differences

... Osotua partners should help only when they are asked to do so and only if they are able to do so, and they are expected to give only the amount that is actually needed 105 . Moreover, a recent study showed that unpredictable needs are associated with lower expectations of repayment 106 . The cultivation of long term, reliable relationships likely require a different technique for signaling prosociality than ostentatious, non-targeted acts of prosociality. ...

Unpredictable Needs are Associated with Lower Expectations of Repayment

Current Research in Ecological and Social Psychology

... Socioeconomic factors such as education, economic deprivation, and self-assessed socioeconomic status have been found to significantly predict attitudes toward vaccination. Generally, higher levels of education and socioeconomic status are associated with more positive attitudes toward vaccination [26][27][28], as individuals with higher socioeconomic status and educational attainment tend to place more trust in vaccines and recognize their importance for public health [29]. However, the relationship between education, socioeconomic status, and vaccine hesitancy is not always straightforward. ...

Identities as predictors of vaccine hesitancy during the COVID‐19 pandemic
  • Citing Article
  • November 2022

Journal of Social Issues

... Environmentally conscious food consumption might be motivated by social values (sustainability, environment protection, animal welfare, local community, etc.) and functional values (healthiness, quality, taste, etc.) (Kushwah et al. 2019;Schrank-Running 2018). Most recently, Waldman and co-authors also showed that the motivation for food sustainability also consists of convenience and selfinterest and not only the consideration of environmental outcomes (Waldman et al. 2023). ...

Eating sustainably: Conviction or convenience?
  • Citing Article
  • October 2022

Appetite

... Switching and clustering are classically studied in the context of verbal fluency, that is, goaldirected retrieval of specific information from long-term memory [31,65,71]. For example, on an animal verbal fluency task, switching reflects alternating between different subcategories of animals (e.g., birds, snakes, fish), whereas clustering reflects grouping related animals together (e.g., small animals; mice, rabbits, hamsters; [72]). ...

Neural Switch Processes Guide Semantic and Phonetic Foraging in Human Memory
  • Citing Preprint
  • August 2022

... foraging | memory search | verbal fluency | hippocampus | cerebellum Efficient storage and retrieval of information from memory are critical for adaptive human behavior. Just as animals forage for resources such as food and mates by strategically deciding where, when, and how to search in physical space, a growing body of research is probing the question of whether humans retrieve information from memory by employing similar strategies to search in "semantic space" (1,2). ...

Seeking Inner Knowledge: Foraging in Semantic Space
  • Citing Chapter
  • May 2022