Peter F. Lovibond’s research while affiliated with University of South Wales and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (119)


Occasion setting in humans: Norm or exception?
  • Preprint

January 2025

·

13 Reads

·

Julie Chow

·

Peter Lovibond

In this commentary, we propose that occasion setting in humans may be even more widespread than Leising and colleagues assume. In contrast to animal work, our research using the feature negative procedure (A+ AX-) reveals substantial individual differences in what people learn about the feature (X). We discuss findings showing that the majority of participants in our experiments learn something akin to occasion setting, and present reasons for why this may be the case. We conclude that occasion-setting may be the norm in humans because it allows existing learning to be preserved, and for the possibility that the effect of a cue is unique to its accompanying target.



Occasion Setting in Humans: Norm or Exception?
  • Article
  • Full-text available

January 2025

·

2 Reads

Comparative Cognition & Behavior Reviews

Download

The Role of Expectancy in Pavlovian Conditioning

December 2024

·

119 Reads

Psychological Review

A review of Pavlovian conditioning in animals and humans reveals a critical role for expectancy in the learning of an association between a conditioned stimulus (CS) and an unconditioned stimulus (US), as well as in the expression of this association in a conditioned response (CR). The automatic and involuntary nature of CRs has traditionally been explained in terms of the formation of excitatory or inhibitory links between representations of the CS and US. However, this view has difficulty accounting for the variety of CRs that are observed, some qualitatively different from those elicited by the US, depending on the imminence of the predicted US and the nature of the CS. Furthermore, in humans, the same anticipatory responses are seen when the CS–US relationship is instructed rather than experienced and when the imminent occurrence of the US is directly instructed, without a mediating CS. These findings suggest an alternative explanation in which CRs are anticipatory responses elicited automatically by a specific state of expectancy of the US. The similarity between Pavlovian conditioning in animals and humans in turn suggests a continuity of core mechanisms for learning and performance. We conclude that research and theory in Pavlovian conditioning should go beyond the search for direct CS–US connections and seek to understand the mechanisms that underlie CS–US contingency knowledge, expectancy states, and the generation of anticipatory responses.


Using Unobserved Causes to Explain Unexpected Outcomes: The Effect of Existing Causal Knowledge on Protection From Extinction by a Hidden Cause

December 2023

·

91 Reads

·

2 Citations

Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition

People often rely on the covariation between events to infer causality. However, covariation between cues and outcomes may change over time. In the associative learning literature, extinction provides a model to study updating of causal beliefs when a previously established relationship no longer holds. Prediction error theories can explain both extinction and protection from extinction when an inhibitory (preventive) cue is present during extinction. In three experiments using the allergist causal learning task, we found that protection could also be achieved by a hidden cause that was inferred but not physically present, so long as that cause was a plausible preventer of the outcome. We additionally showed complete protection by a physically presented cue that was neutral rather than inhibitory at the outset of extinction. Both findings are difficult to reconcile with dominant prediction error theories. However, they are compatible with the idea of theory protection, where the learner attributes the absence of the outcome to the added cue (when present) or to a hidden cause, and therefore does not need to revise their causal beliefs. Our results suggest that prediction error encourages changes in causal beliefs, but the nature of the change is determined by reasoning processes that incorporate existing knowledge of causal mechanisms and may be biased toward preservation of existing beliefs.


How do Participants Interpret Trials from Individual Cells in a Causal Illusion Task?

July 2023

·

32 Reads

·

1 Citation

In a causal illusion task, participants rate a cue that has an objectively null contingency with an outcome as causal. Trials are usually organized according to a 2x2 table representing the presence/absence of a binary cue and a binary outcome. Cell A outcomes (cue, outcome) can be attributed to the cue. But how do participants interpret trials from cell C (no cue, outcome), where the cause of the outcome is unspecified? In two experiments we asked participants to provide causal explanations for cell A and C trials in a medicine-recovery causal illusion task. Participants who reported that the cause of cell C outcomes (e.g., strong immunity, spontaneous recovery) did not also apply to cell A outcomes showed the strongest causal illusion. Such a causal reasoning process undermines the logic behind the delta P metric typically used to define a contingency, and thereby provides a potential normative account of causal “illusions”.


Figure 1: Screenshot of the distribution builder (Quentin, 2016) used to assess causal beliefs.
Figure 2. A) Mean proportion belief (i.e., allocated balls) for each option in the distribution builder, and B) mean causal ratings for each time point (prior to, and post-training) in Experiment 1. Error bars represent standard error of the mean.
Figure 3. A) Mean proportion belief (i.e., allocated balls) for each option in the distribution builder, and B) mean causal ratings for each time point (prior to, and post-training) in Experiment 2. Error bars represent standard error of the mean.
Assessing Distributions of Causal Beliefs in the Illusory Causation Task

·

·

·

[...]

·

Peter F. Lovibond

The illusory causation effect describes the tendency to judge an unrelated cue and outcome to be causally related. The standard procedure for assessing the illusion is based on the implicit assumptions that participants start as naïve observers with no prior beliefs about the likely relationship between the cue and outcome, and that learning can be adequately captured as a point-estimate causal rating after null contingency training. Here, we use a novel distributional measure to assess participants' beliefs over a range of causal relationships prior to, as well as after, exposure to non-contingent cues and outcomes. Across two experiments with different causal scenarios and 50% cue and outcome density, we show that participants have an initial bias towards expecting a causal relationship between the cue and outcome, and that this bias is mostly corrected after exposure to the null contingency. We conclude that distributional measures of causal beliefs can offer novel insights in understanding the illusory causation effect.


EXPRESS: Unidirectional rating scales overestimate the illusory causation phenomenon

April 2023

·

22 Reads

·

3 Citations

Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006)

Illusory causation is a phenomenon in which people mistakenly perceive a causal relationship between a cue and outcome even though the contingency between them is actually zero. Illusory causation studies typically use a unidirectional causal rating scale, where one endpoint refers to no relationship and the other to a strongly positive causal relationship. This procedure may bias mean causal ratings in a positive direction, either by censoring negative ratings or by discouraging participants from giving the normative rating of zero which is at the bottom extreme of the scale. To test this possibility, we ran two experiments that directly compared the magnitude of causal illusions when assessed with a unidirectional (zero - positive) versus a bidirectional (negative - zero - positive) rating scale. Experiment 1 used high cue and outcome densities (both 75%) whereas Experiment 2 used neutral cue and outcome densities (both 50%). Across both experiments, we observed a larger illusory causation effect in the Unidirectional group compared to the Bidirectional group, despite both groups experiencing the same training trials. The causal illusions in Experiment 2 were observed despite participants accurately learning the conditional probabilities of the outcome occurring in both the presence and absence of the cue, suggesting that the illusion is driven by the inability to accurately integrate conditional probabilities to infer causal relationships. Our results indicate that although illusory causation is a genuine phenomenon that is observable with either a undirectional or bidirectional rating scale, its magnitude may be overestimated when unidirectional rating scales are used.


A cognitive pathway to punishment insensitivity

April 2023

·

60 Reads

·

15 Citations

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

Individuals differ in their sensitivity to the adverse consequences of their actions, leading some to persist in maladaptive behaviors. Two pathways have been identified for this insensitivity: a motivational pathway based on excessive reward valuation and a behavioral pathway based on autonomous stimulus-response mechanisms. Here, we identify a third, cognitive pathway based on differences in punishment knowledge and use of that knowledge to suppress behavior. We show that distinct phenotypes of punishment sensitivity emerge from differences in what people learn about their actions. Exposed to identical punishment contingencies, some people (sensitive phenotype) form correct causal beliefs that they use to guide their behavior, successfully obtaining rewards and avoiding punishment, whereas others form incorrect but internally coherent causal beliefs that lead them to earn punishment they do not like. Incorrect causal beliefs were not inherently problematic because we show that many individuals benefit from information about why they are being punished, revaluing their actions and changing their behavior to avoid further punishment (unaware phenotype). However, one condition where incorrect causal beliefs were problematic was when punishment is infrequent. Under this condition, more individuals show punishment insensitivity and detrimental patterns of behavior that resist experience and information-driven updating, even when punishment is severe (compulsive phenotype). For these individuals, rare punishment acted as a "trap," inoculating maladaptive behavioral preferences against cognitive and behavioral updating.


Retardation of Acquisition After Conditioned Inhibition and Latent Inhibition Training in Human Causal Learning

April 2023

·

157 Reads

·

2 Citations

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition

Inhibitory stimuli are slow to acquire excitatory properties when paired with the outcome in a retardation test. However, this pattern is also seen after simple non-reinforced exposure: latent inhibition. It is commonly assumed that retardation would be stronger for a conditioned inhibitor than for a latent inhibitor, but there is surprisingly little empirical evidence comparing the two in either animals or humans. Thus, retardation after inhibitory training could in principle be attributable entirely to latent inhibition. We directly compared the speed of excitatory acquisition after conditioned inhibition and matched latent inhibition training in human causal learning. Conditioned inhibition training produced stronger transfer in a summation test, but the two conditions did not differ substantially in a retardation test. We offer two explanations for this dissociation. One is that learned predictiveness attenuated the latent inhibition that otherwise would have occurred during conditioned inhibition training, so that retardation in that condition was primarily due to inhibition. The second explanation is that inhibitory learning in these experiments was hierarchical in nature, similar to negative occasion-setting. By this account, the conditioned inhibitor was able to negatively modulate the test excitor in a summation test, but was no more retarded than a latent inhibitor in its ability to form a direct association with the outcome.


Citations (86)


... It is also worth noting that the present study involved instructing participants to evaluate a fictitious drug of which participants had no prior knowledge. Previous research has found an interactive effect of expectations and direct experience of cue-outcome events (Alloy & Tabachnik, 1984), with an asymmetrical contribution, such that people who had strong prior beliefs were less likely to change their causal judgements in light of contradictory covariational information (Fugelsang & Thompson, 2000), and were more likely to 'explain away' evidence that conflicts with their prior beliefs by invoking alternative unobserved causes (Chow et al., 2023;Luhmann & Ahn, 2007;Rottman et al., 2011). The influence of prior knowledge on causal beliefs is explicitly incorporated in Griffiths and Tenenbaum's (2009) theory based causal induction, where causal inference is thought to be guided by both prior knowledge about the causal relationship, and an estimation of the strength of that causal relationship based on experienced information. ...

Reference:

Instruction on the Scientific Method Provides (Some) Protection Against Illusions of Causality
Using Unobserved Causes to Explain Unexpected Outcomes: The Effect of Existing Causal Knowledge on Protection From Extinction by a Hidden Cause

Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition

... This correlation is supported by a longitudinal study by Baykiz et al. (2023) involving COVID-19 patients, where it was observed that the relative DASS-21 scores tended to maintain similar levels of severity over time. This finding is consistent with the conclusions of Lovibond (1998), indicating enduring stability in depression, anxiety, and stress syndromes over a period spanning from 2 to 8 years. ...

Long-Term Stability of Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Syndromes

Journal of Abnormal Psychology

... Although unidirectional scales have been habitually used in contemporary studies on causal illusions (e.g. Barberia et al., 2019;Moreno-Fernández et al., 2021;Vicente et al., 2023), Ng et al. (2024) recently showed that these scales, compared to their bidirectional analogues, can inflate the magnitude of the observed illusions. In our medical scenario, a bidirectional scale might involve values ranging from −100 to +100, the negative ones indicating a preventive relationship (i.e. a harmful influence of the drug over the disease). ...

EXPRESS: Unidirectional rating scales overestimate the illusory causation phenomenon
  • Citing Article
  • April 2023

Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006)

... We also found that punishment resistance for cocaine or food could not be explained by decreased sensitivity to footshock. Finally, studies using a conditionedpunishment task for food rewards found little evidence that punishment resistance was related to reward dominance or aversion insensitivity; instead, punishment resistance in rats and humans seemed most causally related to a lack of learning the punishment contingency and understanding the relationship between actions and aversive outcomes [77,95]. ...

A cognitive pathway to punishment insensitivity
  • Citing Article
  • April 2023

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

... A preexposed stimulus is relatively less novel in comparison to nonpreexposed stimuli. This mechanism was presented as a criticism applied to human research on the phenomena, and was applied mostly in situations where many filler trials were used in human procedures (e.g., Evans et al., 2007;Granger et al., 2016;Lovibond et al., 2023;Rodríguez et al., 2019;Rodríguez & Hall, 2017) whereby there is ample opportunity to contrast the novelty of the stimuli in the situation. Nevertheless, in a typical study, the CS is preexposed (novelty decreases) in a context (novelty decreases) and conditioning is slower than with a stimulus or group for which a novel CS occurs in a relatively less-novel context. ...

Retardation of Acquisition After Conditioned Inhibition and Latent Inhibition Training in Human Causal Learning

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition

... In terms of information or statistics, the two types of correlation excitatory (positive) and inhibitory (negative) are equally informative (e.g., Castiello et al., 2022;Murphy et al., 2022). One might expect therefore, that they would be equally likely to be learned about, indeed the processes might be expected to be symmetrical (Baker & Mackintosh, 1977), but this does not always seem to be the case (Chow et al., 2023;White, 2006) We sought to investigate possible symmetrical causal human learning between generative and preventative contingencies while equating for the perceptual differences in the cues and learned relations. By symmetrical, we refer to learning how generative and preventative contingencies may follow a similar learning trajectory, based on a similar absolute value of magnitude. ...

Inhibitory Learning with Bidirectional Outcomes: Prevention Learning or Causal Learning in the Opposite Direction?

Journal of Cognition

... Lacking awareness or possessing erroneous causal beliefs about the adverse consequence of a behavior is not always problematic. In many individuals, lack of awareness or incorrect causal beliefs can be corrected to change behavior [59]. For example, explicit information about Action-Punisher contingencies changes the behavior and beliefs of some insensitive people, causing them to cease that behavior and avoid further punishment. ...

A cognitive pathway to punishment insensitivity
  • Citing Preprint
  • January 2023

... Participants provided online consent for their participation. The task procedure reported here closely followed previous work from our lab (e.g., Lovibond, Chow, Tobler & Lee, 2022). ...

Reversal of Inhibition by No-Modulation Training but Not by Extinction in Human Causal Learning

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition

... Rescorla-Wagner) or those specifying separate CS-US/CS-no US memories (e.g.,Pearce-Hall).Ongoing research could explore additional distinctions, such as whether inhibitors trained in appetitive or aversive learning contexts generalize inhibitory effects across modalities or are specific to the reward or threat outcome they were conditioned with(Pearce, 1987). If stimulus X becomes a safety signal, trained during stimulusshock conditioning, it might counteract aversive memory traces related to other unpleasant stimuli, including disgust-related or socially unpleasant outcomes(Chow et al., 2022). Another frustration/safety distinction might be found in the phenomena of 'preparedness'(Åhs et al., 2018;Hugdahl & Johnsen, 1989;R. ...

Inhibitory Summation as a Form of Generalization

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Learning and Cognition

... The Depression Anxiety Stress Scale-Youth Version (DASS-Y) (Szabo and Lovibond 2022) will be used to measure the negative emotional states of depression, anxiety, and stress. Adolescents rate how true each statement is for depression (7 items; e.g., "I did not enjoy anything"), anxiety (7 items; e.g., "My hands felt shaky"), and stress (7 items; e.g., "I got upset about little things) on a 3-point scale from 0 (not true) to 3 (very true). ...

Development and Psychometric Properties of the DASS-Youth (DASS-Y): An Extension of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) to Adolescents and Children