Paul Sauseng’s research while affiliated with University of Zurich and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (130)


Alpha Traveling Waves during Working Memory: Disentangling Bottom-up Gating and Top-down Gain Control
  • Article

November 2024

·

22 Reads

The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience

·

Paul Sauseng

·

While previous works established the inhibitory role of alpha oscillations during working memory maintenance, it remains an open question whether such an inhibitory control is a top-down process. Here, we attempted to disentangle this issue by considering the spatio-temporal component of waves in the alpha band, i.e., alpha traveling waves. We reanalyzed two pre-existing and open-access EEG datasets (N = 180, 90 males, 80 females, 10 unknown) where participants performed lateralized, visual delayed match-to-sample working memory tasks. In the first dataset, the distractor load was manipulated (2, 4, or 6), whereas in the second dataset, the memory span varied between 1, 3, and 6 items. We focused on the propagation of alpha waves on the anterior-posterior axis during the retention period. Our results reveal an increase in alpha-band forward waves as the distractor load increased, but also an increase in forward waves and a decrease in backward waves as the memory set size increased. Our results also showed a lateralization effect: alpha forward waves exhibited a more pronounced increase in the hemisphere contralateral to the distractors, whereas the reduction in backward waves was stronger in the hemisphere contralateral to the targets. In short, the forward waves were regulated by distractors, whereas targets inversely modulated backward waves. Such a dissociation of goal-related and goal-irrelevant physiological signals suggests the co-existence of bottom-up and top-down inhibitory processes: alpha forward waves might convey a gating effect driven by distractor load, while backward waves may represent direct top-down gain control of downstream visual areas. Significance Statement When exploring the functional role of alpha band neural oscillations during working memory, mostly amplitude modulations have been considered so far, with relatively limited exploration of spatial-temporal dynamics of this rather global brain oscillatory signature. The present study seeks to address this gap by examining the directionality of alpha wave propagation during working memory retention. Our findings offer novel insights into the well-established inhibitory role of alpha waves, demonstrating that this function is manifested differently according to their propagation directions: forward waves seem to facilitate bottom-up gating, while backward waves might mediate top-down gain control.


Experimental Paradigm. In each trial of this delayed match-to-sample task a rapid serial visual presentation constituted the encoding phase. Spatial positions in a square matrix were sequentially highlighted at a rate of 6 Hz (stimulus onset asynchrony of roughly 167 ms). The majority of stimuli were gray filler items. Five of the frames (highlighted in green) were targets that had to be selected and encoded in short-term memory in order to compare their positions to a retrieval probe. Three stimulation frames in the rapid serial visual presentation stream were salient (red) distractors.
ERP results on source level. When the source montage derived from target ERPs was applied to all three stimulus conditions (A) one cluster (at the dipole source marked in blue) stretching between 172 and 223 ms after stimulus onset dissociated between targets and distractors/fillers. Note, there was no significant difference between distractors and filler items. Largely identical results were obtained when a source montage based on distractor ERPs (B) or filler ERPs (C) was applied to all the conditions. There was always only one significant cluster identified dissociating between the three stimulus categories (driven by a larger negative wave for targets); and the cluster always stretched over an interval from roughly 170 to close to 230 ms. In none of these cases there was any significant difference between distractors and fillers. Colored dots (blue, red, pink or green) represent the locations of dipole sources (SDs). The dipole sources displaying significant differences between conditions (as indicated by pink shading in the amplitude and F-value graphs) are marked by a circle around the dot (i.e. in A the red dipole source would be SD-1, the blue dipole source is SD-2. The same accounts for B and C.). ERP, event-related potential; L, left; R, right; A, anterior; P, posterior.
ERP results on scalp level. Comparing targets, distractors and filler items on scalp level revealed one significant (p < 0.001) spatio-temporal cluster covering occipital, parietal and temporal EEG recording sites (indicated with three asterisks each). The three conditions differed from each other in a time interval comparable to source level, again with targets eliciting a stronger negative wave than distractors and fillers, but distractors and fillers not significantly differing from each other. The lower panels in the figure depict scalp ERPs at recording site PO8 and cluster F-values. Red shading indicates the time interval of the significant effect. EEG, electroencephalography.
Event-related Potentials Indicate Target Processing in the Absence of Distractor Suppression during Rapid Serial Visual Presentation
  • Article
  • Full-text available

April 2024

·

18 Reads

Background In our modern world we are exposed to a steady stream of information containing important as well as irrelevant information. Therefore, our brains have to constantly select relevant over distracting items and further process the selected information. Whereas there is good evidence that even in rapid serial streams of presented information relevant targets can be actively selected, it is less clear whether and how distracting information is de-selected and suppressed in such scenarios. Methods To address this issue we recorded electroencephalographic activity during a rapid serial visual presentation paradigm in which healthy, young human volunteers had to encode visual targets into short-term memory while salient visual distractors and neutral filler items needed to be ignored. Event-related potentials were analyzed in 3D source space and compared between stimulus types. Results A negative wave between around 170 and 230 ms after stimulus onset resembling the N2pc component was identified that dissociated between target stimuli and distractors as well as filler items. This wave appears to reflect target selection processes. However, there was no electrophysiological signature identified that would indicate an active distractor suppression mechanism. Conclusions The obtained results suggest that unlike in situations where target stimuli and distractors are presented simultaneously, targets can be selected without the need for active suppression of distracting information in serial presentations with a clear and regular temporal structure. It is assumed that temporal expectation supports efficient target selection by the brain.

Download

Figure 1, the experimental paradigms and wave computation method. (A) The change detection task of dataset 1 (Feldmann-Wüstefeld & Vogel, 2019). Participants were required to keep in mind the colors of targets (squares) while ignoring the other distracting stimuli (circles). After a short retention interval, participants indicated whether the probe’s color matched with the target presented at the same location. (B) The lateralized whole-report task of dataset 2 (Adam et al., 2018). Participants were instructed to pay attention to and remember the stimuli in the hemifield cued by the green triangle while ignoring the rest. Then, they reported all the colors of the cued stimuli by clicking the color within the matrix corresponding to the items previously presented. (C) Wave quantification method. We selected three electrode axes; midline, left and right hemisphere. The channels within each axis were linearly interpolated into seven channels and sorted according to their spatial location (from posterior to anterior). We employed a 500-ms sliding window at the step of 100 ms, and the resulting segments were fed into two-dimensional fast Fourier Transform. In the power spectrum, the power of forward waves falls into the top-left (or down-right) quarter, whereas that of the backward waves falls into the down-left (or top-right) quarter. For a given temporal frequency and traveling direction, we defined the power as the maximum value in the corresponding column of the respective quarter, normalized by the averaged fast Fourier Transform power of the same temporal frequency. A: anterior, P: posterior, 2DFFT: two-dimensional fast Fourier Transform, FFT: fast Fourier Transform, FW: forward waves, BW: backward waves.
Figure 2, Midline alpha traveling waves during retention periods. Backward waves increased with distractor load in dataset 1 and decreased with set size in dataset 2. BF: the inclusion Bayes factor, D2/D4/D6: distractor load 2, 4, or 6; SS 1/3/6, set size 1, 3, or 6; FW, forward waves; BW, backward waves.
Figure 3, Bilateral alpha traveling waves in dataset 1. Line plots: the power of bilateral alpha traveling waves across time. Below, the color bar indicates the Bayes factors of the power difference between two lateral axes for each time point. Bar plots: the wave power averaged over the retention period. The inclusion Bayes factors above the plots show the main effect of distractor load.
Figure 4, Bilateral alpha traveling waves in dataset 2. The line plots and bar plots represent the bilateral wave power across time and averaged over the retention period, respectively. For more details please refer to the legend of Figure 3.
Figure 5. Schematic summary of the results. The size of the arrows indicates the power of traveling waves. We denote the effects of interests with less transparency. D: distractors, #D: distractor load, T: targets, #T; set size.
Alpha Traveling Waves during Working Memory: Disentangling Bottom-up Gating and Top-down Gain Control

March 2024

·

151 Reads

While previous works established the inhibitory role of alpha oscillations during working memory maintenance, it remains an open question whether such an inhibitory control is a top-down process. Here, we attempted to disentangle this issue by considering the spatio-temporal component of waves in the alpha band, i.e., alpha traveling waves. We reanalyzed two pre-existing and open-access EEG datasets where participants performed lateralized delayed match-to-sample working memory tasks. In the first dataset, the distractor load was manipulated (2, 4, or 6), whereas in the second dataset, the memory span varied between 1, 3, and 6 items. In both datasets, we focused on the propagation of alpha waves on the anterior-posterior axis during the retention period. Our results reveal an increase in alpha-band forward waves as the distractor load increased, but also an increase in forward waves and a decrease in backward waves as the memory set size increased. Notably, our results also showed a lateralization effect: alpha forward waves exhibited a more pronounced increase in the hemisphere contralateral to the distractors, whereas the reduction in backward waves was stronger in the hemisphere contralateral to the targets. In short, the forward waves were regulated by distractors, whereas targets inversely modulated backward waves. Such a dissociation of goal-related and goal-irrelevant physiological signals suggests the co-existence of bottom-up and top-down inhibitory processes: alpha forward waves might convey a gating effect driven by distractor load, while backward waves may represent direct top-down gain control of downstream visual areas. Significance Statement When exploring the functional role of alpha band neural oscillations during working memory, significant attention has been directed towards its amplitude modulation, with relatively limited exploration of spatial-temporal dynamics of this rather global brain oscillatory signature. The present study seeks to address this gap by examining the directionality of alpha wave propagation during working memory retention. Our findings offer novel insights into the well-established inhibitory role of alpha waves, demonstrating that this function is manifested differently according to their propagation directions: forward waves seem to facilitate bottom-up gating, while backward waves might mediate top-down gain control.


Dynamic alpha power modulations and slow negative potentials track natural shifts of spatio‐temporal attention

December 2023

·

92 Reads

·

1 Citation

Psychophysiology

Alpha power modulations and slow negative potentials have previously been associated with anticipatory processes in spatial and temporal top‐down attention. In typical experimental designs, however, neural responses triggered by transient stimulus onsets can interfere with attention‐driven activity patterns and our interpretation of such. Here, we investigated these signatures of spatio‐temporal attention in a dynamic paradigm free from potentially confounding stimulus‐driven activity using electroencephalography. Participants attended the cued side of a bilateral stimulus rotation and mentally counted how often one of two remembered sample orientations (i.e., the target) was displayed while ignoring the uncued side and non‐target orientation. Afterwards, participants performed a delayed match‐to‐sample task, in which they indicated if the orientation of a probe stimulus matched the corresponding sample orientation (previously target or non‐target). We observed dynamic alpha power reductions and slow negative waves around task‐relevant points in space and time (i.e., onset of the target orientation in the cued hemifield) over posterior electrodes contralateral to the locus of attention. In contrast to static alpha power lateralization, these dynamic signatures correlated with subsequent memory performance (primarily detriments for matching probes of the non‐target orientation), suggesting a preferential allocation of attention to task‐relevant locations and time points at the expense of reduced resources and impaired performance for information outside the current focus of attention. Our findings suggest that humans can naturally and dynamically focus their attention at relevant points in space and time and that such spatio‐temporal attention shifts can be reflected by dynamic alpha power modulations and slow negative potentials.


Alpha-tACS Alters Attentional Control but not Cognitive Functions as Video Games Do: A Psychophysical Investigation based on the Theory of Visual Attention

March 2023

·

119 Reads

European Journal of Neuroscience

Video game players' faster speed of information processing has been shown to coincide with altered posterior alpha power modulation, i.e., brain oscillatory activity around 10 Hz. Thus, it was proposed that improved cognitive processing in video game players may be related to differential alpha activity. However, a causal relationship thereof has not yet been established. We addressed this by conducting a non-invasive brain stimulation study to demonstrate that modulating alpha power using transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) may impact on speed of information processing. Furthermore, we aimed to show that this effect correlated with altered attentional control, e.g., visuospatial attention and/or top-down control processing, given that this has been suggested to contribute to video gaming effects. Therefore, we recruited 19 non-video game players to undergo one of five brain stimulation conditions while performing a visual short-term memory task at five different days, respectively. Thus, we applied tACS either at 10 Hz (alpha frequency) or at 16.18 Hz (control frequency) either over their left or right posterior parietal cortex (PPC) or a sham stimulation. Individuals' speed of information processing, visuospatial attention and top-down control processing were operationalized using a computational modeling approach based on the theory of visual attention. We found that alpha-tACS applied over individuals'left PPC altered their visuospatial attention orientation but not their speed of information processing. Thus, we were not able to establish a causal relationship between speed of information processing and altered visuospatial attention processing through alpha power modulation using non-invasive brain stimulation.


Oscillatory brain activity as unified control mechanism for working memory and mentalizing

February 2023

·

54 Reads

·

·

Elisabeth F Sterner

·

[...]

·

Paul Sauseng

It has long been thought that coordination of briefly maintained information (working memory) and higher social cognition (mentalizing) rely on mutually exclusive brain mechanisms. However, here we show that slow rhythmical brain activity in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex controls distributed networks associated with working memory as well as mentalizing during cognitively demanding visual and social tasks. Depending on the effort necessary for cognitive operations, the phase of slow frontal oscillations is used to precisely tune communication with posterior brain areas. For participants having low autistic personality traits, this mechanism is identical across tasks – no matter whether visual or social information is processed. This underpins a unified function of the mentioned oscillatory brain mechanism in working memory and mentalizing. Participants with high autistic personality traits – thus, with difficulty in social cognition – however, have an inability to efficiently tune brain communication depending on cognitive effort in visual information processing. Even more striking, in higher social cognition they fail to implement coordination of distributed brain networks by slow frontal oscillations completely. While these findings suggest a unified function of brain oscillations in cognitive coordination they also explain why individuals with high autistic personality traits can have difficulties with demanding cognitive processing across domains. Significance Statement Our findings revealed an interregional brain coupling mechanism based on rhythmical cortical activity to be responsible for successful social and visual working memory by tuning the fronto-parietal network depending on memory load. We suggest that this coupling mechanism can explain how communication between distant brain areas is effectively controlling cognitive functions, independent of the exact type of information that is processed. Importantly, participants with high autistic personality traits struggle with efficient tuning of fronto-parietal networks. Thus, a deficit in this coupling mechanism seems to be an underlying cause of impairments in social and visual working memory, which is often seen in individuals on the Autism Spectrum. These findings might even generalize to other mental disorders as broad cognitive control deficits and social problems are common in a variety of psychiatric and neurological conditions.


Spatio‐Temporal Dynamics of Oscillatory Brain Activity during the Observation of Actions and Interactions between Point‐light Agents

December 2022

·

52 Reads

·

3 Citations

European Journal of Neuroscience

Predicting actions from nonverbal cues and using them to optimize one’s response behavior (i.e., interpersonal predictive coding) is essential in everyday social interactions. We aimed to investigate the neural correlates of different cognitive processes evolving over time during interpersonal predictive coding. Thirty‐nine participants watched two agents depicted by moving point‐light stimuli while an electroencephalogram (EEG) was recorded. One well‐recognizable agent performed either a ‘communicative’ or an ‘individual’ action. The second agent either was blended into a cluster of noise dots (i.e., present), or was entirely replaced by noise dots (i.e., absent), which participants had to differentiate. EEG amplitude and coherence analyses for theta, alpha and beta frequency bands revealed a dynamic pattern unfolding over time: Watching communicative actions was associated with enhanced coupling within medial anterior regions involved in social and mentalizing processes and with dorsolateral prefrontal activation indicating a higher deployment of cognitive resources. Trying to detect the agent in the cluster of noise dots without having seen communicative cues was related to enhanced coupling in posterior regions for social perception and visual processing. Observing an expected outcome was modulated by motor system activation. Finally, when the agent was detected correctly, activation in posterior areas for visual processing of socially‐relevant features was increased. Taken together, our results demonstrate that it is crucial to consider the temporal dynamics of social interactions and of their neural correlates to better understand interpersonal predictive coding. This could lead to optimized treatment approaches for individuals with problems in social interactions.


Figure 2. (A) ERP traces for congruent, incongruent, and neutral conditions across all participants averaged for electrode sites C3, Cz, C4, CP1, CP2, and Cz. Note the interference effect for the N400 component between 350 and 500 ms post-stimulus. (B) Difference waves between incongruent and neutral conditions in L1 and L2 for German, Romance, and Slavic groups. Note the reduced N400 congruency effect in L1 and the enlarged congruency effect in L2 for the German group.
Sociodemographic and linguistic characteristic of participants by group
Influence of Bilingualism on Behavioral and Electrophysiological Parameters of Cognitive Control: No Clear Effects of Immersion, Stimulus Language, and Word Similarity

October 2022

·

87 Reads

·

1 Citation

Journal of Psychophysiology

Bilingualism has been associated with better cognitive control as compared to monolingualism. However, the robustness of the respective findings is subject to a recent debate, and moderators should be taken into consideration. We reasoned that groups immersed in their second language should show a greater bilingual advantage in cognitive control as compared to non-immersed participants. Further, stimulus language (first or second language), word similarity in the two languages (similar or dissimilar), as well as congruency between ink and word were varied. Forty-five participants from three different language groups (Romance, Slavic, and German) conducted a Stroop task while EEG was recorded. Higher cognitive control demand was operationalized as (1) longer reaction times, (2) higher error rates, (3) stronger N400, (4) increased Late Positive Complex (LPC), and (5) stronger Frontal Midline Theta activity. The classical Stroop interference effect was replicated for all dependent variables. Contrary to expectation, participants immersed in their second language did not exhibit any inhibition advantage in the Stroop task. Moreover, higher script similarity between first and second languages led to faster response times in general. Results are discussed in light of the current debate on the existence of a bilingual advantage in cognitive control.


Temporal dynamics of oscillatory brain activity during the observation of communicative interactions between point-light agents

August 2022

·

95 Reads

·

1 Citation

Predicting actions from nonverbal cues and using them to optimize one's response behavior (i.e., interpersonal predictive coding) is essential in everyday life social interaction. 39 participants watched two agents depicted by moving point-light stimuli while an electroencephalogram (EEG) was recorded. One well-recognizable agent performed either a communicative or an individual action. The second agent was either masked by a cloud of noise dots, or absent and replaced by noise dots, which participants had to differentiate. EEG amplitude and coherence analyses for theta, alpha and beta frequency bands revealed a dynamic pattern unfolding over time: Watching communicative actions was associated with enhanced coupling within medial anterior regions involved in social and mentalizing processes and with dorsolateral prefrontal activation indicating a higher deployment of cognitive resources. Trying to detect the masked agent without having seen communicative cues was related to enhanced coupling in posterior regions for social perception and visual processing. Observing an expected outcome was associated with increased motor system activation. Finally, when the masked agent was detected correctly, activation in posterior areas for visual processing and social cognition was increased. Taken together, our results demonstrate that it is crucial to consider the temporal dynamics of social interactions and of their neural correlates to better understand interpersonal predictive coding.


Alpha-tACS Alters Attentional Control but not Cognitive Functions as Video Games Do: A Psychophysical Investigation based on the Theory of Viusal Attention

July 2022

·

17 Reads

We aimed to validate a recent approach to explain cognitive enhancements in video game players according to which transfer effects may be related to differential attentional control functions as a result of video game playing. For this, we developed an experimental design where 19 non-video game players performed a visual short-term memory paradigm at five different days and experienced one of five different stimulation protocols on each day, respectively. Stimulation protocols comprised transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS) applied either at 10 Hz (alpha frequency) or 16.18 Hz (control frequency) over either the left or right posterior parietal cortex (PPC) or a sham stimulation. Individuals' speed of information processing and short-term memory capacity were modeled by means of a computational modeling approach based on the theory of visual attention (TVA) and served to operationalize transfer effects. Moreover, their visusopatial attentional processing and top-down control were modeled by applying the same approach and served as indicators for which attentional control functions may be related to transfer effects. We hypothesized that alpha-tACS would modulate these functions given that alpha brain oscillatory activity likely represents a neuronal substrate of attentional control. In fact, alpha-tACS applied to the left PPC modulated participants’ visuospatial attentional processing. However, this effect did not cause transfer effects. Thus, our results do not support the hypothesis that differential attentional control may account for video gaming effects.


Citations (77)


... Sequential presentation of targets and distractors implicate that in addition to features such as spatial location, color, shape, salience, etc., temporal information might be used in processing and dissociating targets from distractors. It has been shown that visuospatial and temporal attention can go hand in hand, and thus, support attentional processing of incoming sensory information [19]. Recently, it was reported that temporal predictability can even protect information stored in working memory from distraction [20,21]. ...

Reference:

Event-related Potentials Indicate Target Processing in the Absence of Distractor Suppression during Rapid Serial Visual Presentation
Dynamic alpha power modulations and slow negative potentials track natural shifts of spatio‐temporal attention

Psychophysiology

... A recent study by Vöröslakos et al. (2018) suggests that stimulation intensities of 1 mA peak-to-peak as used in this study might be not sufficient to achieve stimulation effects. Further, Minarik et al. (2016) emphasized the importance of sample size in studies in the related field of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS, Minarik et al., 2016). Due to small or intermediate effect sizes, a rather large sample is required in order to reveal a statistically significant effect. ...

Citation: The Importance of Sample Size for Reproducibility of tDCS Effects
  • Citing Article
  • September 2016

... Our presented feature engineering model is accurate with lower computational complexity than the deep learning models. EEG depicts the spatiotemporal electrical activation of underlying brain regions recorded using a set of surface electrodes placed at standardized positions over the scalp (Friedrich et al. 2022). It has been used to study diverse neuropsychiatric conditions, including AD (Bouwman et al. 2022). ...

Spatio‐Temporal Dynamics of Oscillatory Brain Activity during the Observation of Actions and Interactions between Point‐light Agents
  • Citing Article
  • December 2022

European Journal of Neuroscience

... It is proven in the research [22], learners who follow language immersion programs come through communication apprehensions in four communication contexts, group discussions, meetings, conversations, and public speaking, which in turn may bring negative impact on learners' performance, participation, course grades, cognitive processing, and motivation. From the other psychological aspect, there is no benefit explicitly seen from language immersion participants in the Stroop task [23] and executive functions [24]. In addition, double demand in paying attention to language and content in language immersion is often resolved by favoring the content. ...

Influence of Bilingualism on Behavioral and Electrophysiological Parameters of Cognitive Control: No Clear Effects of Immersion, Stimulus Language, and Word Similarity

Journal of Psychophysiology

... Gene expression studies have revealed the role of the MPFC [32], located in the downstream region of information [33], in long-term memory [34,35]. The frontal midline theta rhythm is a distinct theta activity of the EEG in the frontal midline area that controls activity in the parietal cortex associated with memory maintenance [36]. Following theta-shaking exercise, FNDC5 was highly expressed in the MPFC, strengthening cortical theta oscillations and allowing neurons to compute and communicate top-down control in a wide range of networks. ...

Brain rhythms: How control gets into working memory
  • Citing Article
  • May 2022

Current Biology

... Neural entrainment remains, nonetheless, a controversial topic. Recently, there has been debate over whether the observed synchrony between neural activity and an entraining stimulus truly reflects a state of entrainment [19,22,38,60,84,99] or is simply a byproduct of evoked responses and transient resonance effects [12,24,25,31,34,43,61]. Trains of evoked responses to a periodic stimulus may indeed camouflage as synchrony with an external stimulus [12,42,43]. ...

Evoked responses to rhythmic visual stimulation vary across sources of intrinsic alpha activity in humans

... Of all task-positive regions, the bilateral intra-parietal sulcus, thalamus, and fusiform gyrus showed significant common effects between age, CBF and performance. The intraparietal sulcus and thalamus also showed a unique association between performance and BOLD activity, which is consistent with neurophysiological studies ( Tschentscher and Hauk, 2016 ;Tschentscher and Sauseng, 2022 ) and may suggest a neural origin of the effects in these regions. The processes contributing to coupling between baseline CBF and neural activity are multifaceted, probably comprising neurogenic vasodilation, cardiac output and arterial remodelling ( Gaballa et al., 1998 ;Li et al., 2015 ;Ohanian et al., 2014 ), all of which change with age and regulate baseline and stimulusevoked CBF ( Willie et al., 2014 ). ...

Spatio-Temporal Brain Dynamic Differences in Fluid Intelligence

... Crucially, empiricists were characterized by a prevalence of autistic vs. schizotypal traits, whereas believers showed the reversed pattern. These findings align with the expanding literature focused on identifying the neuro-behavioral signatures associated with Bayesian processing and interindividual differences in predictive mechanisms (Friedrich et al., 2022;Hein et al., 2023;Ivanova et al., 2024). Importantly, for the first time, we showed that individual's position along the ASD-SSD continuum influences the motor strategies they adopt. ...

Seeing a Bayesian Ghost: Sensorimotor Activation Leads to an Illusory Social Perception

iScience

... Converging evidence for the involvement of these brain regions in visuospatial working memory has been provided by functional MRI (fMRI) (Ferreira et al., 1998;Hillary et al., 2006;Olson et al., 2006;Chase et al., 2008;Kas et al., 2011;Jeneson et al., 2012;Bowren et al., 2020). The initial processing of visuospatial information is carried out by lowerand higher-order visual areas in the occipital and temporal lobes within 200 ms after stimulus onset (Vogel and Machizawa, 2004;Agam and Sekuler, 2007;Reinhart and Nguyen, 2019;Peylo et al., 2022). Investigators have further highlighted the causal role of the medial temporal lobe in visuospatial working memory encoding and maintenance, in addition to the formation and retrieval of long-term memory (Olson et Lugtmeijer et al., 2023). ...

Theta:Gamma Phase Coupling and Evoked Gamma Activity Reflect the Fidelity of Mental Templates during Memory Matching in Visual Perception
  • Citing Article
  • January 2022

Cerebral Cortex

... Studies with human participants have previously shown strong associations between the thetaband activity and working memory, such as increased theta-band power in proportion to increased memory load (e.g., number of items being remembered) (Gevins et al., 1997;Raghavachari et al., 2001;Jensen and Tesche, 2002;Cohen and Donner, 2013) and modulated working memory performance with theta-band-based brain stimulation (improvement studies: Polanía et al., 2012;Hoy et al., 2016;Albouy et al., 2017;Violante et al., 2017;Riddle et al., 2020; disruption studies: Lee and D'Esposito, 2012; no effect study: Biel et al., 2022). In monkeys, we as well observed that the scalp EEG theta-band oscillations were pronounced during the MGS task and, interestingly, exhibited transient memory-dependent activities after the anticipated distractor time that were only briefly disrupted by the distractor (Figure 5). ...

Modulating verbal working memory with fronto‐parietal transcranial electric stimulation at theta frequency: Does it work?
  • Citing Article
  • December 2021

European Journal of Neuroscience