April 2024
·
95 Reads
Journal of Scientific Exploration
This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.
April 2024
·
95 Reads
Journal of Scientific Exploration
October 2023
·
1,206 Reads
·
2 Citations
Journal of Scientific Exploration
The sense of being stared at, or scopaesthesia, is very common, and its existence is supported by experimental evidence. However, it contravenes the standard scientific assumption, dating back to Kepler’s discovery of retinal images in 1604, that vision involves only the inward movement of light – intromission – but not the outward movement of images or attention – extramission. From this point of view, scopaesthesia is impossible. Yet, paradoxically, the conventional explanation of virtual images in mirrors is still based on Euclid’s (c. 300 BC) extramission theory, and most people implicitly believe in visual extramission, which could help provide a basis for scopaesthesia. If scopaesthesia depends only on the detection of another’s attention, it could conceivably be a scalar phenomenon, with a magnitude but not direction, analogous to telephone telepathy, in which people feel who is calling but do not know where they are. In this case, scopaesthesia would tell us little about the nature of vision. But if scopaesthesia is normally directional, enabling those stared at to detect the direction from which the look is coming, it would be more like a vector phenomenon, with both magnitude and direction and would provide evidence for visual extramission. Experimental tests of scopaesthesia have so far been devoted to establishing its existence and have not looked at its directionality. Here, we examine the natural history of the phenomenon based on a collection of 960 case histories collected over 25 years involving both humans and non-human animals. This collection includes more than 80 interviews with surveillance officers, detectives, martial arts teachers, celebrity photographers, wildlife photographers, and hunters who have extensive experience of watching people or non-human animals. In 466 (49%) of the cases, directional effects were explicit, in that the person or animal looked at responded by turning and looking directly back at the looker rather than searching at random for the source of attention. In 186 (19%) of the cases directional effects were implicit. In most of the other cases, directional effects were not mentioned, usually because they were general statements lacking detail. In online surveys, including a survey of a group of skeptics, the great majority of respondents said they had experienced directional scopaesthesia. We conclude that directionality is a normal feature of scopaesthesia in real-life situations and suggest that this finding supports the idea that minds are extended beyond brains and that this extension involves some kind of visual extramission. We quote from more than 40 case histories and, in the online Supplementary Material make the entire collection of 960 cases available to those who would like to look at the data for themselves.
April 2015
·
619 Reads
·
11 Citations
EXPLORE
To carry out automated experiments on mobile phones to test for telepathy in connection with telephone calls. Subjects, aged from 10 to 83, registered online with the names and mobile telephone numbers of three or two senders. A computer selected a sender at random, and asked him to call the subject via the computer. The computer then asked the subject to guess the caller׳s name, and connected the caller and the subject after receiving the guess. A test consisted of six trials. The effects of subjects׳ sex and age and the effects of time delays on guesses. The proportion of correct guesses of the caller׳s name, compared with the 33.3% or 50% mean chance expectations. In 2080 trials with three callers there were 869 hits (41.8%), above the 33.3% chance level (P < 1 × 10(-15)). The hit rate in incomplete tests was 43.8% (P = .00003) showing that optional stopping could not explain the positive results. In 745 trials with two callers, there were 411 hits (55.2%), above the 50% chance level (P = .003). An analysis of the data made it very unlikely that cheating could explain the positive results. These experiments showed that automated tests for telephone telepathy can be carried out using mobile phones. Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.
December 2008
·
153 Reads
Journal of Scientific Exploration
The sense of being stared at, or scopesthesia, was investigated experimentally with participants working in pairs. Two participants were tested repeatedly and the effect of attentional transition was investigated. In some tests, in the pre-trial period the starer stared at the staree, who was blindfolded, and in others the starer did not stare during the pre-trial period. Their overall hit rate in these attentional transition tests was 52.8% (2,800 trials; p¼0.002), but there was no significant difference in hit rates between the two kinds of test. Participants were given trial-by-trial feedback, so if there was any learning, there should have been a progressive increase in hit rates. This did not happen. The participants also took part in a control test in which there was no staring at all. In these tests hit rates were at chance levels, indicating that other forms of ESP, such as telepathy and clairvoyance, could not account for the results in scopesthesia tests. There were only 3 recording errors in 2,800 trials (0.1%), and two of these cancelled out, leaving a net error rate of 0.04% Keywords: scopesthesia—sense of being stared at—attentional transitions— response bias—error rates—feedback
January 2006
·
408 Reads
·
25 Citations
This study investigated possible telepathic communication in connection with e-mails. On each trial, there were four potential e-mailers, one of whom was selected at random by the experimenter. One minute before a prearranged time at which the e-mail was to be sent, the participant guessed who would send it. 50 participants (29 women and 21 men) were recruited through an employment web site. Of 552 trials, 235 (43%) guesses were hits, significantly above the chance expectation of 25%. Further tests with 5 participants (4 women, 1 man, ages 16 to 29) were videotaped continuously. On the filmed trials, the 64 hits of 137 (47%) were significantly above chance.
December 2005
·
16 Reads
·
1 Citation
This study investigated possible telepathic communication in connection with e-mails. On each trial, there were four potential e-mailers, one of whom was selected at random by the experimenter. One minute before a prearranged time at which the e-mail was to be sent, the participant guessed who would send it. 50 participants (29 women and 21 men) were recruited through an employment web site. Of 552 trials, 235 (43%) guesses were hits, significantly above the chance expectation of 25%. Further tests with 5 participants (4 women, 1 man, ages 16 to 29) were videotaped continuously. On the filmed trials, the 64 hits of 137 (47%) were significantly above chance.
July 2003
·
1,432 Reads
·
47 Citations
Journal of the Society for Psychical Research
Many people claim to have known who was calling before they picked up the telephone, or to have thought about someone for no apparent reason, who then called. We carried out a series of experiments to test whether or not people really could tell who was telephoning. Each participant had four potential callers, and when the telephone rang had to guess who was calling before the other person spoke. By chance the success rate would have been 25%. In a total of 571 trials, involving 63 participants, the overall success rate was 40%, with 95% confidence limits from 36 to 45%. This effect was hugely significant statistically (p = 4 x 10-16). We obtained similar positive effects when the calls were made at randomly chosen times, and when the calls were made at fixed times known to the subject in advance. With 37 participants, we compared the success rates with familiar and unfamiliar callers and found a striking difference. With familiar callers, 53% of the guesses were correct (n = 190; p = 1 x 10-16). With unfamiliar callers, only 25% of the guesses were correct, exactly at the chance level. This difference between the responses with familiar and unfamiliar callers was highly significant (p = 3 x 10-7). We also investigated the effects of distance between the callers and participants. With overseas callers at least 1,000 miles away, the success rate was 65% (n = 43; p = 3 x 10-8). With callers in Britain, the success rate was lower (35%). In most cases, the overseas callers were people to whom the participants were closely bonded. For the successful identification of callers, emotional closeness seemed to be more important than physical proximity.
March 2003
·
418 Reads
·
35 Citations
Journal of Parapsychology
The authors tested whether participants (N = 4) could tell who was calling before answering the telephone. In each trial, participants had 4 potential callers, one of whom was selected at random by the experimenter. Participants were filmed on time-coded videotape throughout the experimental period. When the telephone began ringing, the participants said to the camera whom they thought the caller was and, in many cases, also how confident they felt in their guesses. The callers were usually several miles away, and in some cases thousands of miles away. By guessing at random, there was a 25% chance of success. In a total of 271 trials, there were 122 (45%) correct guesses (p = 1 × 10-12). The 95% confidence limits of this success rate were from 39% to 51%. In most trials, some of the callers were familiar to the participants and others were unfamiliar. With familiar callers there was a success rate of 61% (n = 100; p = 1 × 10-13). With unfamiliar callers the success rate of 20% was not significantly different from chance. When they said they were confident about their guesses, participants were indeed more successful than when they were not confident.
December 2000
·
404 Reads
·
10 Citations
Many dog owners claim that their animals know when a member of the household is coming home, typically showing their anticipation by waiting at a door or window. In previous trials with a dog called Jaytee, recorded on videotape, it was found that he anticipated his owner's arrival more than ten minutes in advance, even when she was returning in unfamiliar vehicles such as taxis, when the people at home did not know when to expect her, and when she set off at randomly-selected times. This paper describes the results of a pre-planned series of ten videotaped trials with a dog called Kane, a Rhodesian ridgeback, who was said to wait by a window while his owner was on the way home. The window-area was filmed continuously while the dog's owner went to places more than 8 km away and came home at a variety of non-routine times, some of which were selected at random and communicated to her by a telephone pager. The time-coded videotapes were scored blind by a third party. In nine out of ten trials Kane spent most time at the window when his owner was on the way home. On average he was at the window 26 percent of the time while she was returning, and only one percent of the time throughout the rest of her absence. This difference was highly significant statistically. Possible explanations for this behavior are discussed.
January 2000
·
6,323 Reads
·
33 Citations
Journal of Scientific Exploration
Many dog owners claim that their animals know when a member of the household is about to come home, showing their anticipation by waiting at a door or window. We have investigated such a dog, called Jaytee, in more than 100 videotaped experiments. His owner, Pam Smart (PS) traveled at least 7 km away from home while the place where the dog usually waited for her was filmed continuously. The time-coded videotapes were scored "blind". In experiments in which PS returned at randomly-selected times, Jaytee was at the window 4 per cent of the time during the main period of her absence and 55 percent of the time when she was returning (p<0.0001). Jaytee showed a similar pattern of behavior in experiments conducted independently by Wiseman, Smith & Milton (1998). When PS returned at non-routine times of her own choosing, Jaytee also spent very significantly more time at the window when she was on her way home. His anticipatory behaviour usually began shortly before she set off. Jaytee also anticipated PS's return when he was left at PS's sister's house or alone in PS's flat. In control experiments, when PS was not returning, Jaytee did not wait at the window more and more as time went on. Possible explanations for Jaytee's behavior are discussed. We conclude that the dog's anticipation may have depended on a telepathic influence from his owner.
... In a prolonged series of experiments, Sheldrake and other researchers tested the degree to which people can sense they are being stared at from behind. Again, the results obtained are overwhelmingly positive, suggesting that not only the staring itself but also the direction of its origin can be detected (Sheldrake & Smart, 2023). ...
October 2023
Journal of Scientific Exploration
... The second phase of research, starting in 2007, involved a variety of automated tests, including internet-based automated tests for telepathy (Sheldrake & Beeharee, 2009;Sheldrake & Lambert, 2007), tests on telepathy in connection with SMS messages ) and emails ) and automated tests for telephone telepathy on mobile phones (Sheldrake et al., 2015;Sheldrake & Stedall, 2024). Independent investigations of telephone telepathy using an automated procedure were carried out in the US by Wahbeh et al. (2024) and in Italy by Tressoldi and Stedall (2025). ...
April 2015
EXPLORE
... By chance, their responses should have been right about 25% of the time; in fact, they were correct significantly more than this. In an initial unfilmed series of tests with 63 participants and 571 trials, the average hit rate was 40%, very significantly above the mean chance expectation of 25% (p <1x10 -15 ) (Sheldrake & Smart, 2003a). In a follow-up study in which four selected participants were filmed during the tests to minimize any possibility of cheating, the hit rate in a total of 271 trials was 45%, again very significantly above the chance level of 25% (p <1x10 -12 ) (Sheldrake & Smart, 2003b). ...
July 2003
Journal of the Society for Psychical Research
... A related phenomenon is the ability of many cats, dogs and other animals to anticipate when their owners are coming home, sometimes 15 minutes or more in advance, in a way that cannot be explained in terms of routine or sensory cues. The return-anticipating responses seem to be telepathic; the animals appear to be picking up their owners' intentions to come home (Sheldrake, 1999, Sheldrake & Smart, 1998, 2000a, 2000b. ...
January 1998
Journal of the Society for Psychical Research
... In an initial unfilmed series of tests with 63 participants and 571 trials, the average hit rate was 40%, very significantly above the mean chance expectation of 25% (p <1x10 -15 ) (Sheldrake & Smart, 2003a). In a follow-up study in which four selected participants were filmed during the tests to minimize any possibility of cheating, the hit rate in a total of 271 trials was 45%, again very significantly above the chance level of 25% (p <1x10 -12 ) (Sheldrake & Smart, 2003b). In addition, an experiment of this kind was filmed for television in Britain with a group of five sisters, the Nolan sisters, who were a well-known girl band; their hit rate was 50% in 12 trials (p = .05) ...
March 2003
Journal of Parapsychology
... A related phenomenon is the ability of many cats, dogs and other animals to anticipate when their owners are coming home, sometimes 15 minutes or more in advance, in a way that cannot be explained in terms of routine or sensory cues. The return-anticipating responses seem to be telepathic; the animals appear to be picking up their owners' intentions to come home (Sheldrake, 1999, Sheldrake & Smart, 1998, 2000a, 2000b. ...
January 2000
Journal of Scientific Exploration
... A related phenomenon is the ability of many cats, dogs and other animals to anticipate when their owners are coming home, sometimes 15 minutes or more in advance, in a way that cannot be explained in terms of routine or sensory cues. The return-anticipating responses seem to be telepathic; the animals appear to be picking up their owners' intentions to come home (Sheldrake, 1999, Sheldrake & Smart, 1998, 2000a, 2000b. ...
December 2000
... (Sheldrake et al., 2004). Sheldrake and Smart (2005) also investigated possible telepathy in connection with emails in tests with a similar design, and with similar results. In a total of 819 unfilmed trials, the hit rate was 42%, very significantly above the 25% expected by chance (p < 1 x 10 -18 ). ...
January 2006