Norman J. Slamecka’s research while affiliated with University of Toronto and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (28)


Normal Forgetting of Verbal Lists as a Function of Prior Testing
  • Article
  • Publisher preview available

October 1988

·

63 Reads

·

51 Citations

Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition

Norman J. Slamecka

·

Lilly T. Katsaiti

Three experiments investigated the effects of a prior test trial on the subsequent long-term forgetting of paired-associates lists. The pattern of findings was consistent with a learning hypothesis that views a prior test as promoting the overlearning of remembered items and as mediating an efficient learning strategy on the next study trial, but leaving the forgetting rate intact. The learning hypothesis was upheld when pitted against the contrasting predictions of a retrieval-based notion, and it also provided an interpretation of extant data from the method of successive relearning. The main conclusion was that, although a prior test has substantial performance consequences, there is no evidence that it influences the rate of forgetting. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)

View access options

The generation as an artifact of selective displaced rehearsal

December 1987

·

84 Reads

·

199 Citations

Journal of Memory and Language

Four experiments on the generation effect of free recall were reported. Experiment 1 used a within-lists design that showed the generation effect with unilingual, but not bilingual, pairs. Experiment 2 had a between-lists design and found no effect with either kind of pairs. Experiment 3 contracted the two design types with unilingual materials, and obtained generation effects only for the mixed-list arrangement. Experiment 4 prevented displaced rehearsal in a mixed-list arrangement, and found no generation effects. It was concluded that the generation effect of recall is an artifact of selective displaced rehearsal that strengthens generated items at the expense of read items. It was also argued that these data render extant theories of the effect untenable.


On Comparing Rates of Forgetting. Comment on Loftus (1985)

October 1985

·

30 Reads

·

69 Citations

Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition

Discusses G. R. Loftus's (see record 1986-05498-001) proposal for comparing relative forgetting rates by evaluating the horizontal interaction rather than the standard vertical interaction, as used by the present author and B. McElree (see record 1984-05823-001) in their study of forgetting of verbal lists as a function of their degree of learning. It is maintained that the proposed method entails an unavoidable confounding with the ages of the lists being compared, such that one list is measured at an earlier point in its forgetting curve than the other. This prevents an unequivocal assessment of the effect of the treatment variable of interest. The vertical interaction appears to be free of artifact. It is concluded that although the horizontal interaction has the advantage of circumventing the scaling problem, this is more than outweighed by the disadvantage of the list-age confound. (7 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)


Ebbinghaus: Some Rejoinders

July 1985

·

16 Reads

·

15 Citations

Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition

Responds to comments by W. K. Estes, W. Kintsch, G. Mandler, B. B. Murdock, T. O. Nelson, E. Tulving, and R. K. Young (see PA, Vol 73:10996, 11006, 11011, 11012, 11014, 11026, and 11032, respectively) on the contributions of H. Ebbinghaus to the history of psychology. Some of the ideas expressed by these authors include the following: (1) Because he was a true pioneer, it was argued that Ebbinghaus's influence had to be, on balance, positive. (2) It was maintained that there is no sharp line of demarcation between association and structure but rather that the presence of an association always implies structure. (3) It was reaffirmed that Ebbinghaus's passing glance at the manifestation of recollective experience suggests that this subjective indicant of memory was epiphenomenal with respect to an objective indicant. (4) It was emphasized that Ebbinghaus based his seminal work on the measurement of implicit memory, which is now undergoing a resurgence of experimental concern with respect to notions of dissociation and separate memory systems. (14 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)


Ebbinghaus. Some Associations

July 1985

·

123 Reads

·

109 Citations

Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition

In recognition of the 100th anniversary of the publication of H. Ebbinghaus's monograph Memory: A Contribution to Experimental Psychology (1885 [1964]), the present author comments on various aspects of the monograph in light of subsequent experimental and theoretical developments. Specific topics discussed are remote associations, the chaining hypothesis, the length–difficulty relation, the long list–short list problem, successive relearning, the law of contiguity, recollective experience, ecological validity, and sources of bias. In a general evaluation of the Ebbinghaus legacy, it is concluded that in spite of extensive changes that have occurred in the field, Ebbinghaus's work still stands as a model of excellence. (66 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)


Normal forgetting of verbal lists as a function of their degree of learning

July 1983

·

102 Reads

·

178 Citations

Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition

Three experiments, with 276 undergraduates, examined how degree of learning affects normal forgetting. Exp I varied learning of categorized lists and tested retention at 3 intervals (immediately and 1 or 5 days after presentation). Across all measures, study trials affected intercepts but not slopes of forgetting functions. Exp II varied learning of paired-associate lists and tested retention at the same 3 intervals. Across all measures, trials influenced intercepts but not forgetting slopes. Exp III varied learning of sentence lists and tested verbatim and gist memory at the same intervals. Again, trials affected intercepts but not slopes. Results suggest that the forgetting of verbal lists is independent of their degree of learning. No current theories of memory predict these outcomes, but neither does the pattern of results disconfirm any theory. It is argued that present memory theorizing neglects almost entirely the central problem of normal forgetting. (36 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)


Memorial consequences of generating nonwords: Implications for semantic-memory interpretations of the generation effect

June 1982

·

116 Reads

·

119 Citations

Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior

The memorial consequences of generating and reading nonwords were compared under a variety of input and test conditions. The findings obtained refuted the notion that the generation effect was due to inherent differences in the generate and read processes per se. The results suggest that generating yields superior retention only when semantic memory is engaged at the time of study. Explanations for the generation effect consistent with this conclusion were discussed.


The Generation Effect: Delineation of a Phenomenon

November 1978

·

19,404 Reads

·

1,574 Citations

Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Learning & Memory

Reports on 5 experiments with 96 undergraduates, comparing memory for words that were generated by the Ss themselves with the same words when they were simply presented to be read. In all cases, performance in the Generate condition was superior to that in the Read condition. This held for measures of cued and uncued recognition, free and cued recall, and confidence ratings. The phenomenon persisted across variations in encoding rules, timed or self-paced presentation, presence or absence of test information, and between- or within-Ss designs. The effect was specific to the response items under recognition testing but not under cued recall. A number of potential explanatory principles are considered and their difficulties enumerated. It is concluded that the generation effect is real and that it poses an interesting interpretative problem. (23 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)


A case for response-produced cues in serial learning

March 1977

·

6 Reads

·

16 Citations

Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Learning & Memory

Performed 3 experiments with 80 university students to test the prediction that the serial to derived paired-associates transfer task cannot be expected to yield more than relatively moderate degrees of transfer, primarily because one aspect of serial performance (i.e., utilization of response-produced cues) cannot transfer productivity to the 2nd task. Exp I tested serial to derived paired-associates transfer while minimizing biases from response guessing and backward associative interference; moderate transfer was found. Exp II modified the paradigm to encourage the productive transfer of response-produced cues; substantial positive transfer resulted. Exp III was designed to detect the utilization of response-generated stimuli in the absence of confounding with position information. It is concluded that serial anticipation learning is guided by a stimulus complex which includes the prior item on the memory drum together with cues produced by the correct prior anticipatory response. (23 ref) (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2012 APA, all rights reserved)


An analysis of double-function lists

September 1976

·

12 Reads

·

23 Citations

Memory & Cognition

Double-function list acquisition was analyzed in three experiments which used mixed-list designs and forced-choice recognition tests. Findings showed that specific associative interference occurred both during learning and performance phases, that its source was limited to the immediate backward associate during the learning phase, but that it also involved items further removed in both the forward and backward directions during the performance phase, probably by way of mediated chaining.


Citations (28)


... This suite of ideas follows consolidation theory, in that memories rely on physiological processes like Hebbian plasticity (Hebb, 1949) that make the memories susceptible for certain time periods after learning and become more robust thereafter (Duncan, 1949;M€ uller & Pilzecker, 1900). The second set of explanations proposes that interpolated learning causes RI due to enhanced competition between previously and newly learned responses at retrieval (Bower et al., 1994;Mensink & Raaijmakers, 1988;Postman et al., 1968;Saltz & Hamilton, 1967;Slamecka & Ceraso, 1960). One proposed mechanism of this competition comes from cue overload. ...

Reference:

Semantic Associates Create Retroactive Interference on an Independent Spatial Memory Task
Retroactive and proactive inhibition of verbal learning

Psychological Bulletin

... Numerous theories, concepts, frameworks and hypotheses have been developed and employed to explore factors that drive new immigrants to start business ventures in their host countries (Zhou, 2004). Some of these theories include the middleman minority theory (Bonacich, 1973), the discrimination hypothesis (Slamecka, 1960), the blocked mobility positions (Collins, 2002), the culture model (Kourtit et al., 2016), the enclave economy hypothesis (Portes & Jensen, 1992), the interactive model (or opportunity structure) (Aldrich & Waldinger, 1990) and mixed embeddedness (Kloosterman et al., 1999). They have been used effectively to explain the forces that drive IEEE in many of the advanced countries and from population ecology, anthropological, sociological and immigration perspectives (Collins, 2002). ...

Tests of the Discrimination Hypothesis
  • Citing Article
  • July 1960

The Journal of General Psychology

... Because weaker memories are closer to the floor of the forgetting curve (i.e., all forgetting functions are bound by zero), they would often exhibit less apparent forgetting over time than stronger memories-even if they have the same underlying forgetting rates. Interested readers may refer to several relevant papers (Bäuml, 1996;Bogartz, 1990;Loftus, 1985;Rivera-Lares et al., 2022;Rose, 1992;Slamecka, 1985;Wixted, 1990) to explore the mathematical measurement of forgetting over time. ...

On Comparing Rates of Forgetting. Comment on Loftus (1985)

Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition

... Items given overtly in an immediate-recall test prior to any 2sec pause on the part of the subject (inv mediate-recall/fast) have been hypothesized to reflect recall from STS, whereas items given subsequent to the first such pause (im-mediate-recall/slow) have been hypothesized to reflect recall from LTS. The relevance of interresponse times to the study of recall processes has been discussed by Murdock and Okada (1970) and Slamecka (1969). This specific method has been used by Bellezza and Walker (1974) and is based on the hypothesis that items residing in STS typically are output first on immediate-recall tests (Glanzer & Cunitz, 1966), since such retrieval constitutes a read-out of temporary memories that would be lost otherwise. ...

A temporal interpretation of some recall phenomena

Psychological Review

... How meaning influences memory has been examined in domains such as word recall (Bower et al., 1969;Calkins, 1898;Collins & Quillian, 1969;Dooling & Lachman, 1971;Ernest & Paivio, 1971;Sasson & Fraisse, 1972;Tulving, 1972), where it was found that meaningfulness appears to facilitate memory (Pezdek, 1977;Slamecka, 1985). This issue has also been studied in the visual domain for both short-term and long-term memory (Asp et al., 2021;Bellhouse-King & Standing, 2007;Brady & Störmer, 2022;Goldstein & Chance, 1971;Konkle et al., 2010;Kouststaal et al., 2003;Madigan, 2014;Shoval et al., 2022;Standing, 1973). ...

Ebbinghaus. Some Associations

Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition

... This work was the compilation of multiple experiments on memory conducted by Ebbinghaus between the years of 1879-1880 and 1883-1884 (Ebbinghaus, 1885(Ebbinghaus, /1913. It changed psychology by confirming that studying the higher mental functions was possible (Ebbinghaus, 1885(Ebbinghaus, /1913Fuchs, 1997;Murdock, 1985;Slamecka, 1985b). Ebbinghaus (1885Ebbinghaus ( /1913 based his experiments on multiple series of approximately 2,300 nonsense syllables that he created. ...

Ebbinghaus: Some Rejoinders

Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition

... Although the present study did not directly assess whether the delay between initial learning and interpolated testing impacted final test performance, we are confident that such a delay would result in a reduction in the commonly found testing effect (Slamecka & Katsaiti, 1988). Further, the results from Experiment 1 suggest that while the interpolated test may not have supported long-term retention of originally learned information, it did potentiate the learning of the post-event information. ...

Normal Forgetting of Verbal Lists as a Function of Prior Testing

Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition

... Additionally, the number of anticipatory responses, or the number of trials that the participant began to complete a given directive prior to receiving the directive on her Apple Watch, was calculated for each setting. During baseline and intervention probes, if the participant demonstrated an anticipatory response (i.e., initiated directive following before receiving the text directive on the Apple Watch and then completed the directive after receiving the message), the response was marked as a correct "anticipatory response" (Slamecka, 1977), and was not included as a correct response in the probe data. If she did not anticipate the directive before receiving it, the response was coded as an incorrect anticipatory response. ...

A case for response-produced cues in serial learning

Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Learning & Memory

... The generation effect is a classic memory phenomenon in which generating information about the studied words (e.g., solving an anagram, such as "loerwf" for "flower") leads to better memory than simply reading the words (Bertsch et al., 2007;Slamecka & Graf, 1978). ...

The Generation Effect: Delineation of a Phenomenon

Journal of Experimental Psychology Human Learning & Memory

... For instance, one such quirky finding was reported in the late 1960s and opened up a brandnew avenue for investigating memory inhibition. When some of the items from a studied list were given as cues to remind the participants of the rest of the items on the list, recall of the rest of the items was not facilitated as expected but actually inhibited (Slamecka, 1968(Slamecka, , 1969. A similar effect emerged in semantic memory, as well, when US states were the to-be-recalled items (Brown, 1968). ...

Testing for associative storage in multitrial free recall

Journal of Experimental Psychology