Nathan L. Arbuckle’s research while affiliated with Canisius College and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (13)


Interaction of Condition with OtherEV on the decision to take a gamble. Participants in the Mutual Exchange condition were more likely to take gambles that were good for the other and pass on gambles that were bad for the other, while those in the One-Way condition did not use OtherEV when making decisions.
Interaction of Motivations and Desire to Outperform with OtherEV on the decision to take a gamble. Participants who scored high on Motivations were more likely to make good decisions for the other, while those who scored high on Desire to Outperform were less likely to make good decisions for the other.
Estimates and p's, Study 1.
Considerations of Mutual Exchange in Prosocial Decision-Making
  • Article
  • Full-text available

May 2019

·

86 Reads

·

4 Citations

Suraiya Allidina

·

Nathan L. Arbuckle

·

William A. Cunningham

Research using economic decision-making tasks has established that direct reciprocity plays a role in prosocial decision-making: people are more likely to help those who have helped them in the past. However, less is known about how considerations of mutual exchange influence decisions even when the other party’s actions are unknown and direct reciprocity is therefore not possible. Using a two-party economic task in which the other’s actions are unknown, Study 1 shows that prosociality critically depends on the potential for mutual exchange; when the other person has no opportunity to help the participant, prosocial behavior is drastically reduced. In Study 2, we find that theories regarding the other person’s intentions influence the degree of prosociality that participants exhibit, even when no opportunity for direct reciprocity exists. Further, beliefs about the other’s intentions are closely related to one’s own motivations in the task. Together, the results support a model in which prosociality depends on both the social conditions for mutual exchange and a mental model of how others will behave within these conditions, which is closely related to knowledge of the self.

Download

An Indirect Measure of Discrete Emotions

March 2019

·

286 Reads

·

10 Citations

Emotion

·

·

Nathan L. Arbuckle

·

[...]

·

B. Keith Payne

Experiences of discrete emotion play important roles in a variety of psychological domains. Yet, current measures of discrete emotion face significant limitations. Biological and behavioral measures often do not capture subjective experiences related to discrete emotions, while self-reports are susceptible to reporting biases. An indirect measure of discrete emotions would help address the limitations of existing measures; however, few such measures exist. Across 4 studies, we offer an indirect measure of discrete emotion. Our results provide evidence that our measure can distinguish between participants' experiences of same-valenced emotions (Study 1), is relatively less susceptible to deliberate attempts to suppress emotional responses (Studies 2 and 3), and is also relatively less susceptible to the influence of social norms (i.e., gender stereotypes) in self-reported discrete emotions than an explicit measure (Study 4). Overall, these findings demonstrate that our measure of discrete emotions can capture discrete emotional responses above and beyond affective valence, is indirect, and measures affective processes contributing to discrete emotional responses. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).


Using experience sampling to examine links between compassion, eudaimonia, and prosocial behavior

July 2018

·

275 Reads

·

29 Citations

Journal of Personality

Compassion has been associated with eudaimonia and prosocial behavior, and has been regarded as a virtue, both historically and cross‐culturally. However, the psychological study of compassion has been limited to laboratory settings and/or standard survey assessments. Here, we use an experience sampling method (ESM) to compare naturalistic assessments of compassion with standard assessments, and to examine compassion, its variability, and associations with eudaimonia and prosocial behavior. Participants took a survey which included standard assessments of compassion and eudaimonia. Then, over four days, they were repeatedly asked about their level of compassion, eudaimonia, and situational factors within the moments of daily life. Finally, prosocial behavior was tested using the Dual Gamble Task and an opportunity to donate task winnings. Analyses revealed within‐person associations between ESM compassion and eudaimonia. ESM compassion also predicted eudaimonia at the next ESM time point. While not impervious to situational factors, considerable consistency was observed in ESM compassion in comparison with eudaimonia. Further, ESM compassion along with eudaimonia predicted donating behavior. Standard assessments did not. Consistent with virtue theory, some individual's ESM reports displayed a probabilistic tendency toward compassion, and ESM compassion predicted ESM eudaimonia and prosocial behavior toward those in need. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved.



Up-regulation of neural indicators of empathic concern in an offender population

June 2016

·

323 Reads

·

20 Citations

Social Neuroscience

Empathic concern has traditionally been conceived of as a spontaneous reaction to others experiencing pain or distress. As such, the potential role of more deliberate control over empathic responses has frequently been overlooked. The present fMRI study evaluated the role of such deliberate control in empathic concern by examining the extent to which a sample of offenders recruited through probation/parole could voluntarily modulate their neural activity to another person in pain. Offenders were asked to either passively view pictures of other people in painful or non-painful situations, or to actively modulate their level of concern for the person in pain. During passive viewing of painful versus non-painful pictures, offenders showed minimal neural activity in regions previously linked to empathy for pain (e.g., dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and bilateral insula). However, when instructed to try to increase their concern for the person in pain, offenders demonstrated significant increases within these regions. These findings are consistent with recent theories of empathy as motivational in nature, and suggest that limitations in empathic concern may include a motivational component.


Figure 1. Scatter plot of participants' resource allocation bias and musical reactivity scores in Study 1. 
Figure 2. Scatter plot of participants' IAT accuracy bias and reactivity scores in Study 2, with separate prediction lines for participants in the music and picture conditions. IAT implicit association test. 
Figure 3. Mean reactivity in Study 7 as a function of social belonging condition and reactivity assessment stimulus type. Error bars represent the 95% confidence interval. 
Table 4 Correlations Among Factor Scores in Study 4 
Unraveling the Mystery of Music: Music as an Evolved Group Process

July 2013

·

3,026 Reads

·

62 Citations

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

As prominently highlighted by Charles Darwin, music is one of the most mysterious aspects of human nature. Despite its ubiquitous presence across cultures and throughout recorded history, the reason humans respond emotionally to music remains unknown. Although many scientists and philosophers have offered hypotheses, there is little direct empirical evidence for any perspective. Here we address this issue, providing data which support the idea that music evolved in service of group living. Using 7 studies, we demonstrate that people's emotional responses to music are intricately tied to the other core social phenomena that bind us together into groups. In sum, this work establishes human musicality as a special form of social cognition and provides the first direct support for the hypothesis that music evolved as a tool of social living. In addition, the findings provide a reason for the intense psychological pull of music in modern life, suggesting that the pleasure we derive from listening to music results from its innate connection to the basic social drives that create our interconnected world. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2013 APA, all rights reserved).


Table 1 . Regression Coefficients Predicting Personality Judgments, Experiment 2 
Figure 2. Proportion pleasant responses as a function of prime valence and passing-allowed versus no-passing conditions Note: Error bars reflect 1 SE. 
Intention Invention and the Affect Misattribution Procedure: Reply to Bar-Anan and Nosek (2012)

March 2013

·

517 Reads

·

100 Citations

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

A recent study of the affect misattribution procedure (AMP) found that participants who retrospectively reported that they intentionally rated the primes showed larger effect sizes and higher reliability. The study concluded that the AMP's validity depends on intentionally rating the primes. We evaluated this conclusion in three experiments. First, larger effect sizes and higher reliability were associated with (incoherent) retrospective reports of both (a) intentionally rating the primes and (b) being unintentionally influenced by the primes. A second experiment manipulated intentions to rate the primes versus targets and found that this manipulation produced systematically different effects. Experiment 3 found that giving participants an option to "pass" when they felt they were influenced by primes did not reduce priming. Experimental manipulations, rather than retrospective self-reports, suggested that participants make post hoc confabulations to explain their responses. There was no evidence that validity in the AMP depends on intentionally rating primes.


Understanding Everyday Psychopathy: Shared Group Identity Leads to Increased Concern for Others among Undergraduates Higher in Psychopathy

October 2012

·

1,904 Reads

·

33 Citations

Social Cognition

Psychopathy can be considered as a dimension anchored on one end by a lack of concern for others. Even in its milder forms, psychopathy can lead to everyday antisocial behavior, such as plagiarism and cheating or getting into fistfights. Although a lack of concern for others is central to the concept of psychopathy, it is unclear whether this stems from differences in ability or motivation. In two studies, participants made decisions for themselves and others simultaneously following a manipulation of shared identity, which is known to increase the motivation for cooperative behavior. When the others were described as in-group members, participants higher in psychopathy showed greater concern for those others. This indicates that the lack of concern for others produced by everyday psychopathy is due to a lack of motivation to care about others, rather than a lack of ability to do so.


(A) Localization of BESA Dipole modeling for latent variable. (B) LORETA source modeling result for latent variable.
Latent time courses for Black and White faces in the Pull and Push conditions. Because these values are projected from latent space, values on the y axis are scaled in arbitrary units with respect to underlying microvolts.
Mean amplitudes for the P100 for Black and White faces in the Pull and Push conditions.
Rapid social perception is flexible: approach and avoidance motivational states shape P100 responses to other-race faces

May 2012

·

225 Reads

·

49 Citations

Research on person categorization suggests that people automatically and inflexibly categorize others according to group memberships, such as race. Consistent with this view, research using electroencephalography (EEG) has found that White participants tend to show an early difference in processing Black versus White faces. Yet, new research has shown that these ostensibly automatic biases may not be as inevitable as once thought and that motivational influences may be able to eliminate these biases. It is unclear, however, whether motivational influences shape the initial biases or whether these biases can only be modulated by later, controlled processes. Using EEG to examine the time course of biased processing, we manipulated approach and avoidance motivational states by having participants pull or push a joystick, respectively, while viewing White or Black faces. Consistent with previous work on own-race bias, we observed a greater P100 response to White than Black faces; however, this racial bias was attenuated in the approach condition. These data suggest that rapid social perception may be flexible and can be modulated by motivational states.


Aspects of neuroticism and the amygdala: Chronic tuning from motivational styles (Reprinted from Neuropsychologia, vol 48, pg 3399-3404, 2010)

March 2011

·

189 Reads

·

19 Citations

Neuropsychologia

Recent research and theory has highlighted the dynamic nature of amygdala activation. Rather than simply being sensitive to a few limited stimulus categories, amygdala activation appears to be dependent on the goals of the perceiver. In this study, we extend this line of work by demonstrating that the means by which a person seeks to accomplish a goal also modulates the amygdala response. Specifically, we examine the modulatory effects of the aspects of neuroticism (volatility/withdrawal), a personality variable that has been linked to both generalized anxiety and differences in amygdala sensitivity. Whereas Neuroticism-Volatility is proposed to be associated with the fight-flight-freeze system (FFFS) and a sensitivity for any cues of negativity, Neuroticism-Withdrawal is proposed to be associated with the behavioral inhibition system (BIS) and a generalized tendency toward passive avoidance. During fMRI scanning, participants were presented with positive, negative, and neutral images and were required to approach (move perceptually closer) or avoid (move perceptually farther away) stimuli in different blocks of trials. Consistent with hypotheses proposing a dissociation between these two aspects of neuroticism, participants higher in Neuroticism-Volatility had increased amygdala activation to negative stimuli (regardless of whether they were approached or avoided), whereas participants higher in Neuroticism-Withdrawal had increased amygdala activation to all approached stimuli (regardless of stimulus valence). These data provide further support for the motivational salience hypothesis of amygdala function, and demonstrate that both the ends and means of goal pursuit are important for shaping a response.


Citations (12)


... Studies have outlined the malleable nature of prosocial behaviour, advocating for several training approaches to encourage the development of prosocial skills (Böckler et al., 2018;Paulus, 2018;Tountopoulou et al., 2021). Experiments with economic games and theoretical frameworks in controlled settings have shown that factors such as communication (Burton-Chellew and West, 2013;Caviola and Faulmüller, 2014;Zhao et al., 2016), reciprocity (Allidina et al., 2019;Hsieh et al., 2023), reputation and reward (Hauert, 2010;Wang et al., 2012), as well as time constraints (Haley and Fessler, 2005;Kümmerli et al., 2010;Yamagishi et al., 2017), can significantly impact prosocial actions. ...

Reference:

Advancing Prosociality in Extended Reality: Systematic Review of the Use of Embodied Virtual Agents to Trigger Prosocial Behaviour in Extended Reality
Considerations of Mutual Exchange in Prosocial Decision-Making

... These include standardized measures such as the Differential Emotion Scale (Izard et al., 1993), the PANAS, and the expanded version (PANAS-X; Watson & Clark, 1994;Watson et al., 1988), and the Profile of Mood States (Mcnair et al., 1971). These measures lack of phenomenological data of the emotions felt with music (Lee et al., 2020;Zentner & Eerola, 2010). Self-report measures require to choose from a predetermined category and are thus subject to report bias. ...

An Indirect Measure of Discrete Emotions

Emotion

... ESM studies prompt participants to repeatedly report their feelings and behaviors in real time over a certain amount of time ranging from a few days to weeks or even months (Mehl & Conner, 2012). Given that these measurement occasions happen within participants' natural contexts, they reduce recall biases and have the potential to alleviate social desirability effects (Runyan et al., 2019), allowing researchers to draw conclusions that are better situated in context and have greater ecological validity. An additional means of contextualizing studies is to employ studies around culturally salient beliefs, practices, and behaviors. ...

Using experience sampling to examine links between compassion, eudaimonia, and prosocial behavior

Journal of Personality

... Particularly brain imaging studies support this hypothesis. For example, Arbuckle and Shane (2017) presented criminal offenders images of people in painful and non-painful situations (i.e., within-subject design) while recording their neural response. As anticipated, no increased activation of brain regions related to empathic concern was found between non-painful and painful images. ...

Up-regulation of neural indicators of empathic concern in an offender population

Social Neuroscience

... Although a lack of concern for others is the point of focus of the concept of psychopathy, it is not clear whether this stems from differences in ability or motivation. (Arbuckle & Cunningham, 2012) Polaschek & Daly review past and recent research on psychopathy and treatment, concluding that a handful of newer, well-designed studies suggest grounds for optimism about psychopathy and treatability. Although psychopathy characteristics-along with other indices of criminal risksignal responsivity challenges, well-conceived programs for higher-risk offenders can decrease recidivism in highly psychopathic offenders and appear to do so by reducing dynamic risk factors. ...

Understanding Everyday Psychopathy: Shared Group Identity Leads to Increased Concern for Others among Undergraduates Higher in Psychopathy
  • Citing Article
  • October 2012

Social Cognition

... Interpersonal synchronous movement is a universal characteristic of human collective behaviour [1] and is often accompanied by a euphoric sense of union [2]. Rituals that incorporate collective movement play a key role in the evolution of human ultrasociality by facilitating the maintenance of large cohesive groups [2][3][4][5][6][7][8][9][10][11][12][13]. Previous research suggests that interpersonal synchrony expands the contours of the group beyond the confines of familial relationships [14] and physical similarity [15] by utilising behavioural movement patterns as cues of similarity for in-group bond formation [14][15][16][17][18][19][20][21]. ...

Unraveling the Mystery of Music: Music as an Evolved Group Process

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

... According to one of the studies conducted precisely on the overlapping complexity of social identity on the attitude toward the outgroups in America, it was found that the perception of complexity and overlapping of social categorization is positively associated with the favorable evaluation of outgroups and support for diversity and multiculturalism (Miller et al., 2009). It is pertinent to mention that the perception of diversity in the in-group is related to the favorable evaluation of out-groups, including religious identitydriven groups such as sects and other religions. ...

Social Identity Complexity: Its Correlates and Antecedents
  • Citing Article
  • January 2009

Group Processes & Intergroup Relations

... et al., 2012;Znanewitz et al., 2018). As a result, the AMP has been used to study many issues, like prejudice (e.g., Cooley et al., 2014;Steele et al. 2018), alcohol consumption (e.g., Payne et al., 2008), eating behavior (e.g., Hofmann et al., 2010), or still health-related issues (e.g., Hood et al., 2021;Seligman et al., 2023). ...

Automatic attitudes and alcohol: Does implicit liking predict drinking?

... One limitation to the generalizability of our findings is related to the operationalization and measurement of our constructs. Although some research defines implicit processes as those that constitute any of the "four horsemen" of automaticity (viz., Fast, efficient, unintentional, unaware; see Bargh, 1994;Moors, 2016), recent work in social cognition tends to define it in terms of fast and unintentional (e.g., Ferguson et al., 2023;Kurdi et al., 2023;Payne et al., 2005Payne et al., , 2013. However, research is ongoing about how exactly the AMP works (e.g., Hughes et al., unpublished manuscript;Kurdi et al., 2023;Mann et al., 2019), and about the potential similarities and differences across indirect measures (Corneille & Hütter, 2020). ...

Intention Invention and the Affect Misattribution Procedure: Reply to Bar-Anan and Nosek (2012)

Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

... At the neural level, the selective processing of an instructed threat cue can be observed through the activity of a distributed fear network, for example including the amygdala, anterior cingulate cortex, and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (Mechias et al., 2010;Olsson & Phelps, 2007). The Late Positive Potential (LPP) serves as an electrocortical correlate of this network activity and reflects the interplay between limbic and extrastriate visual processing (de Rover et al., 2012;Liu et al., 2012). ...

Rapid social perception is flexible: approach and avoidance motivational states shape P100 responses to other-race faces