Miriam Spering’s research while affiliated with University of British Columbia and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (46)


Rapid adaptation to acceleration during interceptive hand movements
  • Article

September 2024

·

2 Reads

Journal of Vision

·

Miriam Spering



Figure 2. Set up of the testing room and schematic of dependent variables. (a) Participant
Figure 3. Frequency of each error type across all participants. There were five possible error types,
Figure 4. Accuracy across conditions and across participants. * p < .05, ** p < .01, *** p < .001 after
Figure 5. Constant and variable error across conditions. The effect of the different trajectories on
Figure 6. Scores on timing dependent variables across conditions. The effect of the trajectory
Target interception in virtual reality is better for natural versus unnatural trajectory shapes and orientations
  • Preprint
  • File available

August 2024

·

26 Reads

Human performance in perceptual and visuomotor tasks is enhanced when stimulus motion follows the laws of gravitational physics, including acceleration consistent with Earth's gravity, g. Here we used a manual interception task in virtual reality to investigate the effects of trajectory shape and orientation on interception timing and accuracy. Participants punched to intercept a ball moving along one of four trajectories that varied in shape (parabola or tent) and orientation (upright or inverted). We also varied the location of visual fixation such that trajectories fell entirely within the lower or upper visual field. Reaction times were faster for more natural shapes and orientations, regardless of visual field. Overall accuracy was poorer and movement time was longer for the inverted tent condition than the other three conditions, perhaps because it was imperfectly reminiscent of a bouncing ball. A detailed analysis of spatial errors revealed that interception endpoints were more likely to fall along the path of the final trajectory in upright vs. inverted conditions, suggesting stronger expectations regarding the final trajectory direction for these conditions. Taken together, these results suggest that the naturalness of the shape and orientation of a trajectory contributes to performance in a virtual interception task.

Download

Perception–eye movement dissociation. (A) Trial timeline in a binocular rivalry flash suppression task with a 1500-msec adaptation period (with cue timing shown for feature-based attention task; adapted from Spering & Carrasco, 2012). Figure panel shows two alternative ways to indicate responses, either via a 10-point interval scale or continuous arrow rotation to point in the direction of perceived motion. (B) Perception (orange) follows the unadapted component. (C) Eye movements (blue) follow the pattern (adapted from Spering et al., 2011 and Spering & Carrasco, 2015).
Schematic overview of brain pathways for the processing of conscious and unconscious visual (orange) and emotional (green) information and vision-guided eye movement control (orange/blue). Anatomical location of areas is approximate, and only selected areas and their connections are shown. dLGN = dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus of the thalamus; FEF = frontal eye field; LIP = lateral intraparietal area; MT+ = medial temporal area (V5); V1 = primary visual cortex; SC = superior colliculus.
Perception-action Dissociations as a Window into Consciousness

July 2024

·

25 Reads

·

1 Citation

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

Understanding the neural correlates of unconscious perception stands as a primary goal of experimental research in cognitive psychology and neuroscience. In this Perspectives paper, we explain why experimental protocols probing qualitative dissociations between perception and action provide valuable insights into conscious and unconscious processing, along with their corresponding neural correlates. We present research that utilizes human eye movements as a sensitive indicator of unconscious visual processing. Given the increasing reliance on oculomotor and pupillary responses in consciousness research, these dissociations also provide a cautionary tale about inferring conscious perception solely based on no-report protocols.




Eye Movements as a Window into Decision-Making

June 2022

·

119 Reads

·

30 Citations

Annual Review of Vision Science

For over 100 years, eye movements have been studied and used as indicators of human sensory and cognitive functions. This review evaluates how eye movements contribute to our understanding of the processes that underlie decision-making. Eye movement metrics signify the visual and task contexts in which information is accumulated and weighed. They indicate the efficiency with which we evaluate the instructions for decision tasks, the timing and duration of decision formation, the expected reward associated with a decision, the accuracy of the decision outcome, and our ability to predict and feel confident about a decision. Because of their continuous nature, eye movements provide an exciting opportunity to probe decision processes noninvasively in real time. Expected final online publication date for the Annual Review of Vision Science, Volume 8 is September 2022. Please see http://www.annualreviews.org/page/journal/pubdates for revised estimates.


Tracking and perceiving diverse motion signals: Directional biases in human smooth pursuit and perception

May 2022

·

30 Reads

Human smooth pursuit eye movements and motion perception behave similarly when observers track and judge the motion of simple objects, such as dots. But moving objects in our natural environment are complex and contain internal motion. We ask how pursuit and perception integrate the motion of objects with motion that is internal to the object. Observers (n = 20) tracked a moving random-dot kinematogram with their eyes and reported the object's perceived direction. Objects moved horizontally with vertical shifts of 0, +-3, +-6, or +-9 degs and contained internal dots that were static or moved +-90 degs up/down. Results show that whereas pursuit direction was consistently biased in the direction of the internal dot motion, perceptual biases differed between observers. Interestingly, the perceptual bias was related to the magnitude of the pursuit bias (r = 0.75): perceptual and pursuit biases were directionally aligned in observers that showed a large pursuit bias, but went in opposite directions in observers with a smaller pursuit bias. Dissociations between perception and pursuit might reflect different functional demands of the two systems. Pursuit integrates all available motion signals in order to maximize the ability to monitor and collect information from the whole scene. Perception needs to recognize and classify visual information, thus segregating the target from its context. Ambiguity in whether internal motion is part of the scene or contributes to object motion might have resulted in individual differences in perception. The perception-pursuit correlation suggests shared early-stage motion processing or perception-pursuit interactions.


Eye-hand coordination during online reach corrections is task-dependent

March 2022

·

33 Reads

·

7 Citations

Journal of Neurophysiology

To produce accurate movements, the human motor system needs to deal with errors that can occur due to inherent noise, changes in the body, or disturbances in the environment. Here, we investigated the temporal coupling of rapid corrections of the eye and hand in response to a change in visual target location during the movement. In addition to a 'classic' double-step task in which the target stepped to a new position, participants performed a set of modified double-step tasks in which the change in movement goal was indicated by the appearance of an additional target, or by a spatial or symbolic cue. We found that both the absolute correction latencies of the eye and hand and the relative eye-hand correction latencies were dependent on the visual characteristics of the target change, with increasingly longer latencies in tasks that required more visual and cognitive processing. Typically, the hand started correcting slightly earlier than the eye, especially when the target change was indicated by a symbolic cue, and in conditions where visual feedback of the hand position was provided during the reach. Our results indicate that the oculomotor and limb-motor system can be differentially influenced by processing requirements of the task and emphasize that temporal eye-hand coupling is flexible rather than rigid.


Citations (22)


... On the empirical side, Cohen and colleagues (Cohen, Sung, & Alaoui, 2024) present a new experiment using a creative paradigm to alter the visual input in the periphery with intriguing findings that may help to resolve a longstanding debate in the field of conscious perception: whether our subjective phenomenology is rich or sparse. Carrasco and Spering (Carrasco & Spering, 2024) review a series of studies demonstrating a striking dissociation between eye movement patterns and visual awareness, which has both theoretical and practical implications for studies of conscious perception. Schacter and Thakral discuss the relationships between memory recall, imagination and simulation, and conscious experiences (Schacter & Thakral, 2024). ...

Reference:

Integrating Consciousness Science with Cognitive Neuroscience: An Introduction to the Special Focus
Perception-action Dissociations as a Window into Consciousness

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

... They assess the integrity of visuomotor and cognitive processes respectively 12 and can also expose irregularities in pupil responses 13 and eye blinks 14 that reveal disease-related modulation of neural circuits. PD typically increases antisaccade error rates, especially in cognitively impaired patients 11 and slows correct antisaccade reaction times 10,[15][16][17] . Studies find both increased rates of short-latency prosaccades and prolonged prosaccade reaction time; this may be partially related to medication given findings that dopaminergic drugs slow prosaccade reaction time 18,19 and could indicate a speeding effect of the disease itself on visually-driven saccades. ...

Eye movements in Parkinson's disease: from neurophysiological mechanisms to diagnostic tools
  • Citing Article
  • December 2023

Trends in Neurosciences

... V ISION plays a pivotal role in our daily lives, surpassing other senses in importance for decision-making [1], [2]. It is essential for recognizing objects, navigating environments, avoiding collisions, and understanding detailed features like facial expressions and body language [3], [4]. ...

Eye Movements as a Window into Decision-Making
  • Citing Article
  • June 2022

Annual Review of Vision Science

... Additionally, the uniform response time between hands, which can benefit overall motor coordination and speed (Badau et al., 2018), suggests that training plans for skeet shooters should focus on enhancing coordination balance between hands. EHC is a focal point of current research into athletes' visuomotor abilities, emphasizing the synchronization of hand movements with visual stimuli (Bajaj et al., 2024;de Brouwer and Spering, 2022;Erickson, 2021;Jurkiewicz et al., 2024). Post-SVT, the EXP exhibited faster response speeds and greater accuracy, although improvements in response time for central and peripheral targets were not significant. ...

Eye-hand coordination during online reach corrections is task-dependent
  • Citing Article
  • March 2022

Journal of Neurophysiology

... The key cortical substrate for motion processing is middle temporal complex (MT+) and its impact on LLRs is especially notable since MT+ plays a crucial role in smooth pursuit eye movements (SPEM) used to track moving objects (9,10). During SPEM, an efference copy of the extraretinal signal associated with eye movements has been hypothesized to contribute to motion perception (12)(13)(14), to guide upper limbs actions such as interceptive hand movements (15), and anticipatory posture stabilization during interactions with moving objects (16,17). The broad influence of SPEM on limb motor control and the linkage of MT+ to LLR modulation suggests an important but untested possibility: LLRs may be modulated by gaze behavior. ...

The role of eye movements in manual interception: A mini-review
  • Citing Article
  • June 2021

Vision Research

... As a first dependent variable, we assessed the location of participants' final gaze fixation before movement execution, also known as the "quiet eye" in the far-aiming literature [44,45]. Extensive research on gaze behavior shows that humans typically fixate their task goal prior to initiating goal-directed movements, suggesting that the location of their final fixation provides a valid indicator of participants' planned aiming point before movement [46][47][48][49][50][51][52]. As a second dependent variable, we assessed the ball's impact location relative to the target center as a measure of movement outcome. ...

Functional Use of Eye Movements for an Acting System
  • Citing Article
  • January 2021

Trends in Cognitive Sciences

... Accommodation and vergence ensure rapid and stable visual focus across varying distances, while contrast sensitivity is indispensable in challenging conditions, such as during rain or fog [8]. Stereopsis facilitates three-dimensional environmental perception, allowing athletes to accurately estimate the distance and size of objects, which is vital for timing and spatial judgments [9,10]. ...

Eye movements as a readout of sensorimotor decision processes
  • Citing Article
  • March 2020

Journal of Neurophysiology

... Visually induced torsion typically has a gain of <0.1 (Sheliga et al., 2009), similar to what we observed here. Yet, these tiny responses appear to impact anticipatory horizontal pursuit, and might contribute to the perception of rotational motion illusions (Wu & Spering, 2019). ...

Ocular torsion is related to perceived motion-induced position shifts

Journal of Vision

... However, there is limited understanding of predictive abilities of autistic individuals in the realm of motor control, and why interacting with moving objects appears more affected than interacting with static objects (Ament et al., 2015;Arthur et al., 2021;Lidstone et al., 2020). Hence, we investigated a range of interception tasks as they rely heavily on prediction (as opposed to reaction) (Brenner & Smeets, 2018;Fiehler et al., 2019). This area of research is highly significant, as difficulties in interacting with moving objects in daily life may impact both social and motor development (e.g., play activities with peers), or even impact individuals' safety (e.g., crossing a busy street or driving a vehicle). ...

Prediction in goal-directed action
  • Citing Article
  • August 2019

Journal of Vision

... However, eye movement behavior is also affected by the predictability of the path of the moving object and visual certainty. In situations of high uncertainty, such as when the moving target bounces or is occluded, observers shift their gaze away from the moving object to the anticipated interception locations (de la Malla, Rushton, Clark, Smeets, & Brenner, 2019;Diaz, Cooper, Rothkopf, & Hayhoe, 2013;Fooken & Spering, 2019;Mann, Nakamoto, Logt, Sikkink, & Brenner, 2019). Thus, the coordination between eye and hand movements can be modulated by visuomotor task demands. ...

Decoding go/no-go decisions from eye movements

Journal of Vision