September 2016
·
100 Reads
·
12 Citations
Journal of Vision
This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.
September 2016
·
100 Reads
·
12 Citations
Journal of Vision
August 2016
·
85 Reads
Perception
Owing to errors made by the authors, Jessica K. Witt, J. Eric T. Taylor, Mila Sugovic, and John T. Wixted, the main conclusion of the article was reached using the wrong formula. A reevaluation with the correct formula confirmed the main conclusion. Jessica K. Witt, J. Eric T. Taylor, Mila Sugovic, and John T. Wixted (2015)
March 2016
·
2,603 Reads
·
50 Citations
Acta Psychologica
Action abilities are constrained by physical body size and characteristics, which, according to the action-specific account of perception, should influence perceived space. We examined whether physical body size or beliefs about body size affect distance perception by taking advantage of naturally-occurring dissociations typical in people who are obese but believe themselves to weigh less. Normal weight, overweight, and obese individuals made verbal distance estimates. We also collected measures of beliefs about body size and measures of physical body size. Individuals who weighed more than others estimated distances to be farther. Furthermore, physical body weight influenced perceived distance but beliefs about body size did not. The results illustrate that whereas perception is influenced by physical characteristics, it is not influenced by beliefs. The results also have implications for perception as a contributing factor for lifestyle choices: people who weigh more than others may choose to perform less physically demanding actions not as a result of how they perceive their bodies, but as a result of how they perceive the environment.
January 2016
·
73 Reads
·
21 Citations
Attention Perception & Psychophysics
According to the action-specific account of perception, a perceiver's ability to act influences how the environment is perceived. For example, in a computer-based task, participants perceive fish as moving faster when they use a smaller net, and are thus less effective at catching the fish (Witt & Sugovic, 2013a). Here, we examined the degree to which attention may influence perceptual judgments by requiring participants to engage in a secondary task that directed their attention either toward (Exp. 1) or away from (Exp. 2) the to-be-caught fish. Though perceived fish speed was influenced by participants' catching performance-replicating previous results-attentional allocation did not impact this relationship between catching performance and perceived fish speed. The present results suggest that action directly influences spatial perception, rather than exerting indirect effects via attentional processes.
January 2016
·
59 Reads
·
19 Citations
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
The visual system is influenced by action. Objects that are easier to reach or catch look closer and slower, respectively. Here, we describe evidence for one action-specific effect, and show that none of the six pitfalls can account for the results. Vision is not an isolate module, as shown by this top-down effect of action on perception.
November 2015
·
1,199 Reads
·
159 Citations
Perception
A common conceptualization of signal detection theory (SDT) holds that if the effect of an experimental manipulation is truly perceptual, then it will necessarily be reflected in a change in d′ rather than a change in the measure of response bias. Thus, if an experimental manipulation affects the measure of bias, but not d′, then it is safe to conclude that the manipulation in question did not affect perception but instead affected the placement of the internal decision criterion. However, the opposite may be true: an effect on perception may affect measured bias while having no effect on d′. To illustrate this point, we expound how signal detection measures are calculated and show how all biases—including perceptual biases—can exert their effects on the criterion measure rather than on d′. While d′ can provide evidence for a perceptual effect, an effect solely on the criterion measure can also arise from a perceptual effect. We further support this conclusion using simulations to demonstrate that the Müller-Lyer illusion, which is a classic visual illusion that creates a powerful perceptual effect on the apparent length of a line, influences the criterion measure without influencing d′. For discrimination experiments, SDT is effective at discriminating between sensitivity and bias but cannot by itself determine the underlying source of the bias, be it perceptual or response based.
December 2013
·
7 Reads
September 2013
·
575 Reads
·
23 Citations
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review
Perceptual judgments of objects, such as judgments of their size, distance, and speed, are influenced by the perceiver's ability to act on these objects. For example, objects that are easier to block appear to be moving slower than objects that are more difficult to block. These effects are known as action-specific effects. Recent research has found similar patterns when a person observes someone else act: When the other person's task is more difficult, objects look farther away and faster to the observer, whereas when the other person's task is easier, the objects look closer and slower to the observer. These previous findings that another person's ability penetrates into perceptual judgments challenge the idea that action-specific effects are specific to the perceiver's own abilities. However, in the present study, we show that the apparent effects of another person's ability on the observer's judgments are actually due to the observer's own abilities as if he or she was in the other person's situation. This implicates a type of self-projection motor simulation mechanism. The results also preserve the critical idea that action-specific effects are perceiver specific and, consequently, that they could be adaptive for planning future actions.
July 2013
·
57 Reads
·
2 Citations
Journal of Vision
Action abilities are constrained by physical body size and characteristics, which, according to the action-specific account of perception, should influence perceived space. We examined whether physical body size or beliefs about body size affect distance perception by taking advantage of naturally-occurring dissociations typical in people who are obese but believe themselves to weigh less. Normal weight, overweight, and obese individuals made verbal distance estimates. We also collected measures of beliefs about body size and measures of physical body size. Individuals who weighed more than others estimated distances to be farther. Furthermore, physical body weight influenced perceived distance but beliefs about body size did not. The results illustrate that whereas perception is influenced by physical characteristics, it is not influenced by beliefs. The results also have implications for perception as a contributing factor for lifestyle choices: people who weigh more than others may choose to perform less physically demanding actions not as a result of how they perceive their bodies, but as a result of how they perceive the environment.
May 2013
·
215 Reads
·
35 Citations
Perception
According to the action-specific account of perception, the perceiver's ability to act influences perception of the target. For example, targets that are easier to acquire are reported to look closer, bigger, and slower. However, an alternative explanation for these effects is that they are due to response bias, rather than to changes in perception. To test the role of response bias, we employed two separate manipulations. We manipulated people's abilities to block a ball and measured the corresponding effects on estimated ball speed. We also created an explicit task demand by giving participants instructions that emphasized responding either slow or fast. Participants were grouped, based on whether they were compliant or non-compliant with the instructions. Regardless of their compliance, we found an action-specific effect of blocking ability on estimated speed. Given that non-compliant participants still showed the effect, the results provide strong evidence against a response-bias explanation of this action-specific effect. Paired with earlier research, we conclude that blocking ability influences perceived speed. Perception expresses the relationship between the environment and the perceiver, and this view is consistent with emerging neural and behavioral evidence for an interconnected perceptual-motor system.
... The proposed scaling mechanism has already been criticized on various grounds (e.g., Firestone, 2013;Firestone & Scholl, 2016), and the proponents of this mechanism responded to the raised criticism by providing counterarguments and by defending their original claims (Clore & Proffitt, 2016;Proffitt, 2013;Witt, 2015;Witt et al., 2016). One argument in favor of body scaling in this debate seems to be theoretical rather than empirical in nature (empirical issues are raised later). ...
January 2016
Behavioral and Brain Sciences
... Electronic copy available at: https://ssrn.com/abstract=5148020 P r e p r i n t n o t p e e r r e v i e w e d of analyses using SDT (Rosenthal et al., 2009;Knotts & Shams, 2016;Witt et al., 2015Witt et al., , 2016, specifically that criterion measures (response bias) are often misinterpreted as internal decision criteria, and some studies have pointed out that perceptual sensitivity also affects criterion measures (Witt et al., 2015(Witt et al., , 2016. Thus, the criterion measures in this study may reflect perceptual processes and not necessarily respond to decision criteria. ...
September 2016
Journal of Vision
... First and foremost, our ability to perform an action depends on our body characteristics: our body size determines what we can reach as well as what we can see (Linkenauger and Proffitt, 2008;Sugovic et al., 2016). The effect of our body mass on perceived distance was assessed in an experiment of Sugovic et al. (2016) in which normal weight, overweight, and obese individuals were asked to estimate a same distance and report their beliefs concerning their body size. ...
March 2016
Acta Psychologica
... In such cases, faster presentation speed may require greater attention to adequately process the textual content, complicating the interpretation of whether movement speed alone affects physiological responses. Recent research addressing these methodological concerns found no consistent differences in consumers' physiological responses to objects moving at varying speeds (Jia et al., 2020;Witt et al., 2016). These findings imply that altering movement speed might not always provoke a direct physiological response, but rather influence consumer perceptions by generating specific inferences (e.g. ...
January 2016
Attention Perception & Psychophysics
... Given the consequences of visual representation for physical activity, researchers have attempted to identify the factors that give rise to perceptual experiences. Evidence suggests that people for whom exercise is more taxing, including those who are overweight, older, or experiencing chronic fatigue, perceive their environments in more extreme ways (Sugovic & Witt, 2011Witt et al., 2009; for counterevidence and a lively debate regarding these findings, see Firestone, 2013;Proffitt, 2013;Schnall, 2017 and responses to this target article). People who felt tired judged a finish line as farther away than did people who felt full of energy . ...
January 2011
Visual Cognition
... Furthermore, Proffitt's work suggests that a recalibration of perceived steepness should occur over time as the level of body fat increases or decreases. When body weight has been included, increased body weight was associated with increased reported steepness of stairs and hills (Sugovic & Witt, 2013). ...
July 2013
Journal of Vision
... Finally, the model included three condition-wise starting bias (z) parameters, which represent another potential mechanism by which biases at the onset of evidence accumulation can impact the decision process. Note that, although we estimated these starting biases to improve model fit, our focus in all primary analyses reported in this paper is on the evidence-related perceptual bias for merit and need (represented by Bias condition in merit and need blocks, respectively), not these motor-related starting biases (Witt et al., 2015). A detailed description of the model fitting procedure is provided in Appendix 3. ...
November 2015
Perception
... An equally relevant question that has received much less attention in perceptual neuroscience is how sounds induce changes in visual detection criterion. Although SDT criterion variations are typically interpreted as decision-level response biases (i.e., a stimulus-independent bias to report a given response), some perceptual illusions that affect perceptual accuracy often manifest in a shifted criterion parameter 23,24 . Thus, whether the sound-induced increase in false alarms in visual detection tasks exclusively represents a decisional-level bias or it might also reflect a perceptual-level bias is still unknown. ...
May 2012
Journal of Vision
... According to this, perception is a function of the perceiver's ability to perform an action, so space is experienced as smaller given that the resources required to cross it are high (Witt, 2011). People dynamically change their perception of the environment depending on their physical resources or the energy costs related to performance needed in a given space: distances were judged to be larger by subjects who were carrying a heavy backpack as compared to those who were not (Proffitt et al., 2003), by older adults when compared to younger counterparts (Sugovic and Witt, 2011), or by participants supposed to throw a heavy ball compared to those who threw a light ball (Proffitt et al., 2003). Also, objects appeared farther away after walking on a treadmill as the effect of an increase in walking effort (Proffitt et al., 2003). ...
June 2011
Journal of Vision
... Indeed, OPTIMAL theory posits that attentional (i.e., external focus) and motivational (i.e., visual illusion to enhance expectancies) factors are mediated through a common goal-action coupling mechanism (Wulf & Lewthwaite, 2016). For example, enhanced expectancies can protect against a task-irrelevant focus (i.e., self-referential processing) and strengthen a focus on the task goal (i.e., an external focus) by increasing a learner's assessment of their action capabilities (Linkenauger et al., 2009;Witt et al., 2014;Wulf & Lewthwaite, 2016). Additionally, an external focus further protects against self-referential processing by facilitating the efficient switching between the default mode network to taskrelevant networks required for effective motor performance (McKay et al., 2015;Wulf & Lewthwaite, 2016). ...
September 2013
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review