Megan Oaten’s research while affiliated with Griffith University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (54)


Age-related disgust responses to signs of disease
  • Article

February 2024

·

33 Reads

·

·

Amanda L Duffy

·

[...]

·

Megan Oaten

Previous studies found similarities in adults' disgust responses to benign (e.g. obesity) and actual disease signs (e.g. influenza). However, limited research has compared visual (i.e. benign and actual) to cognitive (i.e. disease label) disease cues in different age groups. The current study investigated disgust responses across middle childhood (7-9 years), late childhood (10-12 years), adolescence (13-17 years), and adulthood (18+ years). Participants viewed individuals representing a benign visual disease (obese), sick-looking (staphylococcus), sick-label (cold/flu), and healthy condition. Disgust-related outcomes were: (1) avoidance, or contact level with apparel the individual was said to have worn, (2) disgust facial reactions, and (3) a combination of (1) and (2). Avoidance was greater for the sick-looking and sick-label than the healthy and obese conditions. For facial reaction and combination outcomes, middle childhood participants responded with greater disgust to the sick-looking than the healthy condition, while late childhood participants expressed stronger disgust towards the sick-looking and obese conditions than the healthy condition. Adolescents and adults exhibited stronger disgust towards sick-label and sick-looking than obese and healthy conditions. Results suggest visual cues are central to children's disgust responses whereas adolescents and adult responses considered cognitive cues.


The affiliative power of others' pain online

November 2022

·

46 Reads

·

1 Citation

Journal of Applied Social Psychology

Pain is rarely suffered alone. In contemporary online contexts, publicly shared pain can command the collective attention of hundreds, even millions of people. We sought to explore the possibility that collectively attending to others' pain promotes affiliation among those with whom it is attended online and identify the mechanisms that mediate these effects. Across two experimental studies, utilizing independent group designs, physically dispersed undergraduate students attended to real‐world videos depicting either physical, social, or no‐pain online. In Study 1 (N = 74, 66.22% female, Mage = 25.31 years, SDage = 6.81 years), we found evidence for the phenomenon of pain collectively attended to online, with online videos depicting physical and social pain eliciting stronger perceptions of collective attention than the non‐painful online video. In Study 2 (Time 1: N = 185, 75.14% female, Mage = 22.62 years, SDage = 7.44 years; Time 2: N = 91, 72.53% female, Mage = 23.32, SDage = 8.19), we subsequently found collectively attending to others' physical and social pain online indirectly promoted cohesion, interpersonal closeness, and desire to affiliate among participants through perceived emotional synchrony. This pattern of indirect effects was found immediately after collective attention to painful online content (Time 1) and at 1‐week follow‐up (Time 2). Although preliminary, our findings increase practical understanding of how shared pain can be harnessed to bond physically dispersed individuals together online, the implications of which we discuss in the context of the COVID‐19 pandemic.


Development and psychometric evaluation of the Aversion to Bowel Cancer Screening Scale
  • Article
  • Full-text available

July 2022

·

18 Reads

·

1 Citation

European Journal of Cancer Care

Objective: Avoidance of colorectal cancer (CRC) screening is well documented with emotional barriers deterring screening intention and uptake. However, the assessment of such aversion is limited by the available instruments focusing on siloed emotions or screening procedures, limiting relevance to the complete process of decision-making in the CRC context. Methods: To address this gap, psychometric properties of the newly developed Aversion to Bowel Cancer Screening Scale (ABCSS) were assessed using data from 640 CRC screening eligible asymptomatic community members. Item review and piloting reduced 179 items to the initial 29-item scale. Using a holdout sample technique, exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis, reliability and validity checks were conducted. Results: A three-factor model (Fecal Occult Blood Test [FOBT] Aversion, Colonoscopy Aversion and Health Conscientiousness) with 21 items was identified. Analyses of the 21-item ABCSS indicated excellent reliabilities for the scale and subscales (α = .91 to .95). Correlations with relevant existing measures, intention and behaviour indicated good construct validity. Conclusion: The ABCSS is a valid measure of aversion to CRC screening for asymptomatic community members facing the decision to undertake CRC screening. This instrument may provide a more comprehensive understanding of the decision-making process for CRC screening.

Download

The Factorial Structure of Stigma and Its Targets

January 2022

·

55 Reads

·

3 Citations

Social Psychology

We aimed to determine (1) the attributes of multiple stigmatized populations, (2) whether Kurzban and Leary’s (2001) functional typology of stigma emerges and identifies the dimensions upon which each stigma type differs, and (3) the emotional responses toward emergent stigma types. Participants ( N = 2,674) were assigned to 1/52 stigma target conditions and their attitudes surveyed. Data were analyzed by multilevel factor analysis with stigma targets at Level 2. There were five within-factors at Level 1 (social competence, interpersonal access, social inclusion, immorality, perceived permanence) and three between-factors at Level 2 (interpersonal stigmatization, morality-based stigmatization, chronic stigmatization). Interpersonal stigmatization was predicted by disgust and pity, and morality-based stigmatization was predicted by disgust and [-]happiness, with no predictors for chronic stigmatization. These results support a functional typology of stigma.


Mean (and s.e.) ratings for the wanting and liking test, on Day 1 (a,b) and Day 8 (c,d) before and after the meal (breakfast) for each group and the key difference between liking and wanting ratings, before and after breakfast, on Day 1 and 8, for the controls (e) and for the WS-diet group (f). In (e,f), it can be seen that the difference between changes in wanting and liking across breakfast are significantly smaller in the WS-diet group following the intervention (Day 8), while remaining unchanged in controls.
Scatter plots (with linear fitted slope) of change in performance on the measure of hippocampal-dependent learning and memory (HDLM; Hopkins verbal learning test), between Day 1 and 8, and the change in wanting relative to liking across breakfast, between Day 1 and 8, for the control (a) and WS-diet (b) groups.
HVLT performance across the experiment.
Hippocampal-dependent appetitive control is impaired by experimental exposure to a Western-style diet

February 2020

·

408 Reads

·

63 Citations

Animals fed a Western-style diet (WS-diet) demonstrate rapid impairments in hippocampal function and poorer appetitive control. We examined if this also occurs in humans. One-hundred and ten healthy lean adults were randomized to either a one-week WS-diet intervention or a habitual-diet control group. Measures of hippocampal-dependent learning and memory (HDLM) and of appetitive control were obtained pre- and post-intervention. HDLM was retested at three-week follow-up. Relative to controls, HDLM performance declined in the WS-diet group (d = 0.43), but was not different at follow-up. Appetitive control also declined in the WS-diet group (d = 0.47) and this was strongly correlated with HDLM decline (d = 1.01). These findings demonstrate that a WS-diet can rapidly impair appetitive control in humans-an effect that could promote overeating in consumers of a WS-diet. The study also suggests a functional role for the hippocampus in appetitive control and provides new evidence for the adverse neurocognitive effects of a WS-diet.


Is obesity treated like a contagious disease?

December 2019

·

103 Reads

·

11 Citations

Journal of Applied Social Psychology

The behavioral avoidance of people with obesity is well documented, but its psychological basis is poorly understood. Based upon a disease avoidance account of stigmatization, we tested whether a person with obesity triggers equivalent self‐reported emotional and avoidant‐based responses as a contagious disease (i.e., influenza). Two hundred and sixty‐four participants rated images depicting real disease signs (i.e., person with influenza), false alarms (i.e., person with obesity), person with facial bruising (i.e., negative control), and a healthy control for induced emotion and willingness for contact along increasing levels of physical proximity. Consistent with our prediction, as the prospect for contact became more intimate, self‐reported avoidance was equivalent in the influenza and obese target conditions, with both significantly exceeding reactions to the negative and healthy controls. In addition, participants reported greatest levels of disgust toward the obese and influenza target conditions. These results are consistent with an evolved predisposition to avoid individuals with disease signs. Implicit avoidance occurs even when participants know explicitly that such signs—here, obese body form—result from a noncontagious condition. Our findings provide important evidence for a disease avoidance explanation of the stigmatization of people with obesity.


Perceptions of IBD within patient and community samples: a systematic review

September 2019

·

75 Reads

·

27 Citations

Psychology and Health

Objective: Inflammatory bowel disease (IBD) is a chronic, gastrointestinal condition that involves a range of debilitating bowel symptoms. Adjustment to living with IBD can be negatively impacted by maladaptive cognitive and behavioural factors (e.g. negative illness representations and repressing emotions). Patient samples also report negative reactions from the general public and such perceptions can further negatively impact people living with IBD. Therefore, we aimed to systematically review literature investigating the illness perceptions, perceived stigmatisation, and negative emotional reactions toward IBD within patient and community samples. We also aimed to review how these factors impact those living with IBD (i.e. adjustment, psychological health). Design: A range of databases (e.g. Psych INFO, PubMed) were searched over two years. One reviewer individually screened titles and abstracts using the specified inclusion criteria, and this process was repeated by a second reviewer. Subsequently, the full text articles were screened and data were extracted for the 82 articles that satisfied the inclusion criteria. Following data extraction, a narrative synthesis was conducted. Results: The review of 82 studies suggested that negative illness perceptions are linked to poorer psychosocial outcomes, that patient samples frequently anticipate stigmatisation, fear relating to bowel accidents was the most common emotion reported, and that the general public direct little enacted stigma towards IBD. Conclusion: For people living with IBD: (i) poorer psychological adjustment was reported by those who held more negative perceptions and reactions toward their illness; and (ii) their concerns regarding public perceptions of IBD warrants further empirical attention. Results emphasise the importance of targeting perceptions, and facilitating education and adaptive responding during treatment.


A Proximal Perspective on Disgust

June 2019

·

139 Reads

·

35 Citations

Emotion Review

The functional basis of disgust in disease avoidance is widely accepted; however, there is disagreement over what disgust is. This is a significant problem, as basic questions about disgust require knowing if single/multiple forms/processes exist. We address this issue with a new model with one form of disgust generated by multiple processes: (a) pure disgust experienced during gastrointestinal illness; (b) somatosensory disgust elicited by specific cues that activate the pure disgust state; (c) anticipatory disgust elicited by associations between distance cues for somatosensory disgust and requiring threat evaluation; (d) simulated disgusts elicited by imagining somatosensory and anticipatory disgust and frequently involving other emotions. Different contamination processes interlink (a–d). The implications of our model for fundamental questions about disgust (e.g., emotion status; continuation into animals) are examined.


Wanting and liking for sugar sweetened beverages and snacks differ following depletion and repletion with energy and fluids

March 2019

·

26 Reads

·

11 Citations

Appetite

Drinking sugar-sweetened beverages (SSBs) seems to uniquely contribute to excess weight gain, and several mechanisms have been proposed to account for this. Here we examine a further proposal, namely that explicit wanting and liking for SSBs may be less sensitive to changes in physiological state, when contrasted to equi-palatable solid sweet snacks. Study 1 explored this by having participants rate wanting for (on seeing) and liking of (on tasting) several SSBs and snacks, before and after receiving a solid lunch with ad libitum water. Participant reports of hunger and thirst, obtained at multiple time-points, equally reduced across lunch. Wanting for the snacks decreased significantly more across lunch than liking, but for the SSBs, wanting and liking decreased in parallel. Study 2 engineered a far more dramatic alteration in thirst, by using fluid deprivation, a liquid lunch, and encouraging drinking to satiation. This time, reduction in thirst exceeded reduction in hunger. However, all this served to achieve was an equivalent change across lunch for snacks and SSBs, with wanting reducing more than liking now for both. These findings suggest that changes in wanting, relative to liking, for SSBs, are less sensitive to alterations in physiological state than equi-palatable solid snacks, enhancing the chance of consumption.



Citations (49)


... A psychometric validation of a measure assessing cultural constructs related to breast and cervical cancer screening in Latina populations yielded one factor related to disposition toward cancer prevention (i.e., negative beliefs about health professionals, sociocultural deterrents to screening), and a second factor related to attitudes and beliefs about cancer [i.e., catastrophic disease expectations (63)]. In a psychometric evaluation of a scale measuring aversion to CRC screening, one subscale encompassed related emotions (64). Our findings are also consistent with extant conceptualizations of disease-specific health literacy [e.g., (65)] and cultural and conceptual knowledge [e.g., (26,63)]. ...

Reference:

Development and psychometric validation of the Colorectal Cancer Literacy Scale—Uruguay Version
Development and psychometric evaluation of the Aversion to Bowel Cancer Screening Scale

European Journal of Cancer Care

... Extreme caution is thus needed to avoid inadvertently sanctioning 'other condemning' via moral types of stigma motivated by anger and contempt (Dastani & Pankov, 2017;Oaten et al., 2022). As we have highlighted, the limited existence of evidence for constructive 'disapproval' effects appear particularly contingent on including meaningfully empathic and reintegrative components (Harris, 2006;Snoek et al., 2021), but these appear particularly counter to the broader force of addiction stigma. ...

The Factorial Structure of Stigma and Its Targets

Social Psychology

... Following a Western diet during the transition off treatment was associated with greater loss of appetite. The previous literature has established a correlation between Western diet and appetite dysregulation [37][38][39]. Excessive intake of fat and sugar (consistent with a Western diet) can disrupt hippocampal function, leading to altered eating behaviors [37][38][39]. As overconsumption continues, hippocampal dysfunction worsens, further impacting appetite regulation [40,41]. ...

Hippocampal-dependent appetitive control is impaired by experimental exposure to a Western-style diet

... Fatter people experiencing mistreatment report mixed coping mechanisms, with the majority indicating poor coping, including mechanisms that include self-harm (e.g., excessive drinking, drug use, cutting themselves; Gerend et al., 2021;Lemmon & Jensen, 2017;Puhl & Brownell, 2006). In fact, many of the effective coping mechanisms (italicized next; Zhang et al., 2020) suggested for those being stigmatized could fail to work when the victim believes the mistreatment is earned: Perhaps they cannot manage information about their weight (e.g., their weight may be a product of genetics or illness) because they believe that these are simply excuses (Lee et al., 2014); perhaps they cannot reconstruct what it means to be fatter because the moral failing of fatness is too embedded within our culture (Crawford, 1980); perhaps they cannot engage in effective emotion work because their feelings of self-blame inhibit self-compassion (Hilbert et al., 2015); and perhaps they cannot put in place effective boundary management by finding solace in an "in-group" because others feel that their weight may be communicable (Tapp et al., 2020). For the victims of weight-based mistreatment, offering compassion and kindness in the face of mistreatment is clearly needed as they receive censure from both themselves and Western culture at large (Hilbert et al., 2015). ...

Is obesity treated like a contagious disease?
  • Citing Article
  • December 2019

Journal of Applied Social Psychology

... The symptoms of UC can be severe and have a significant impact on the quality of life of affected individuals [3,4]. Moreover, studies link UC to subsequent psychological challenges, further adding to the burden of the disease [5][6][7]. ...

Perceptions of IBD within patient and community samples: a systematic review
  • Citing Article
  • September 2019

Psychology and Health

... In order to evoke disgust more directly, less commonly employed methods involve the use of sensory stimuli. According to Stevenson et al. (2019), gustatory, olfactory, and tactile cues elicit a so-called somatosensory disgust, by signaling the potential presence of pathogens and then communicating with the brain via a specific neural link to evoke the negative affect. The easiest for administration are visual sets, usually involving a photo with a disgusting stimulus and a corresponding control photo without the stimulus, such as the Curtis Image Set (Curtis et al., 2004) or the Culpepper Disgust Image Set (C-DIS; Culpepper et al., 2018). ...

A Proximal Perspective on Disgust
  • Citing Article
  • June 2019

Emotion Review

... However, the degree to which wanting and liking decline after a meal is not the same. Several studies have found that for the same food, wanting declines to a far greater extent than liking (e.g., Attuquayefio et al., 2016;Chan et al., 2021;Pender et al., 2019). It has been suggested that this greater reduction in wanting relative to liking serves to minimise the likelihood of further eating when sated, and that this may be a memory-related phenomenon (Davidson et al., 2019;Stevenson et al., 2020). ...

Wanting and liking for sugar sweetened beverages and snacks differ following depletion and repletion with energy and fluids
  • Citing Article
  • March 2019

Appetite

... In addition to this sex difference, there are several factors that modulate sexual disgust. Overall, sexual arousal and attractiveness of a potential mate increase sexual disgust thresholds (Crosby et al., 2021;Oaten et al., 2019;Stevenson et al., 2011). This finding is especially important considering the dilemma of engaging in sex, while it carries a high risk of pathogen transmission (Borg & de Jong, 2012). ...

The Role of Disgust in Male Sexual Decision-Making

... The first cluster identified in the 'activation' map had a volume of 7840 mm 3 , spanning from (− 24, − 32, − 18) to (0, 0, 14) centered at (− 10, − 14.9, 2) with 6 peaks with a max value of 0.0369 ALE, 3.5330288E-9 P, 5.79 Z at (− 6, − 18, 10). Activation from these coordinates have been attributed to the Medial Dorsal Nucleus 39 and Ventral Lateral Nucleus 40 of the Left Thalamus 41 , as well as the Red Nucleus of the Left Brainstem/Midbrain 42,43 , as well as the Left Lentiform Nucleus 44 . Ten studies contributed foci to this cluster [25][26][27][28][29][30][31][32][33][34] . ...

Moral Violations and the Experience of Disgust and Anger

... A correlation between interoceptive sensitivity and prospective memory has also been established (Umeda, Tochizawa, Shibata, & Terasawa, 2016). Perhaps most notably, Stevenson, Francis, Oaten, and Schilt (2018) demonstrated that higher interoceptive sensitivity was associated with higher performance on measures of list learning and prose memory. Our results suggesting a positive relationship between interoceptive sensitivity and verbal memory are consistent with these prior studies. ...

Hippocampal dependent neuropsychological tests and their relationship to measures of cardiac and self-report interoception
  • Citing Article
  • March 2018

Brain and Cognition