Matthew A. Lambon Ralph’s research while affiliated with MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (531)


Late maturation of semantic control promotes conceptual development
  • Preprint

March 2025

·

1 Read

·

Matthew A Lambon Ralph

·

Control processes underpinned by the prefrontal cortex are critical for generating task-appropriate behaviour across cognitive domains, yet this region develops extremely late. Traditionally, this developmental pattern is considered negative but necessary. However, an alternative (yet perhaps complementary) view suggests that a developmental period without control could support learning, particularly in the semantic domain. Here, we exploit a recent computational model to test formally whether late development of the context-sensitive use of conceptual knowledge, or ‘semantic control’, would promote concept acquisition. Simulations show that late maturation of semantic control and anatomical connectivity conspire to promote conceptual learning. Delayed control speeds conceptual learning without compromising conceptual representations, particularly when control connects to intermediate layers. To assess whether semantic control also develops late in human children, we conducted a meta-analysis of the classic triadic matching task where participants decide which of two options best matches a third. Matching can be based on taxonomic or thematic relations. When these conflict, participants must exert semantic control to determine which relation is task appropriate. Context-sensitivity develops later than conceptual knowledge with large increases between 3 and 6 years. Thus, the protracted PFC development leads to a delay in acquiring semantic control processes, benefiting conceptual learning.


Figure 5: Slice leakage analysis. (A) Seed and possible artefact locations for a single
Figure 6: Contrast despite signal dropout and geometric distortions. (A) Mean EPI across all
Optimising 7T-fMRI for imaging regions of magnetic susceptibility
  • Preprint
  • File available

March 2025

·

4 Reads

·

Marta Morgado Correia

·

Minghao Zhang

·

[...]

·

The temporal signal-to-noise ratio (tSNR) of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) is particularly poor in ventral anterior temporal and orbitofrontal regions because of magnetic field inhomogeneity, a problem that is exacerbated at higher field strengths. In this 7T-fMRI study we compared three methods of improving sensitivity in these areas: parallel transmit, which uses multiple transmit elements, controlled independently, to homogenise the flip angle experienced by the tissue; multi-echo, which entails collection of multiple volumes at different echo times following a single radiofrequency pulse; and multiband, in which multiple slices are acquired simultaneously. We found that parallel transmit and multi-echo increased the magnitude of the BOLD signal change, but only multi-echo increased BOLD magnitude in areas prone to susceptibility artefacts. Multiband and denoising of multi-echo data with independent components analysis (ICA) both improved precision of GLM fit. Exploratory results suggested that multi-echo and ICA denoising can both benefit multivariate analyses. In conclusion, a multi-echo, multiband sequence improved fMRI quality in areas prone to susceptibility artefacts while maintaining sensitivity across the whole brain. We recommend this approach for studies investigating the functional roles of ventral temporal and orbitofrontal regions with 7T fMRI. For the purpose of open access, the authors have applied a Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) licence to any Author Accepted Manuscript arising from this work.

Download

Late maturation of semantic control promotes conceptual development

March 2025

·

3 Reads

Control processes underpinned by the prefrontal cortex are critical for generating task-appropriate behaviour across cognitive domains, yet this region develops extremely late. Traditionally, this developmental pattern is considered negative but necessary. However, an alternative (yet perhaps complementary) view suggests that a developmental period without control could support learning, particularly in the semantic domain. Here, we exploit a recent computational model to test formally whether late development of the context-sensitive use of conceptual knowledge, or ‘semantic control’, would promote concept acquisition. Simulations show that late maturation of semantic control and anatomical connectivity conspire to promote conceptual learning. Delayed control speeds conceptual learning without compromising conceptual representations, particularly when control connects to intermediate layers. To assess whether semantic control also develops late in human children, we conducted a meta-analysis of the classic triadic matching task where participants decide which of two options best matches a third. Matching can be based on taxonomic or thematic relations. When these conflict, participants must exert semantic control to determine which relation is task appropriate. Context-sensitivity develops later than conceptual knowledge with large increases between 3 and 6 years. Thus, the protracted PFC development leads to a delay in acquiring semantic control processes, benefiting conceptual learning.


Figure 3. A) tbTUS-induced changes in the normalised accuracy in the semantic task. B) tbTUS-induced changes in the normalised accuracy in the control (pattern matching) task. Light blue lines and triangles indicate the ATL stimulation. Black lines and circles represent the control (ventricle) stimulation. Error bars represent standard error. ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05
Figure 4. The results of MRS analysis. A) The location of ATL VOI (40 x 20 x 20 mm 3 ). B-F) Comparison of neurochemical levels between ATL and ventricle (control) stimulation in the ATL VOI. G) The location of OCC (control) VOI (30 x 30 x 30 mm 3 ). H-L) Comparison of neurochemical levels between ATL and ventricle (control) stimulation in the OCC (control) VOI. Colour bar indicates the number of overlapping participants. Light blue bars represent the ATL stimulation. White bars indicate the control (ventricle) stimulation. Grey circles represent individual data. Error bars represent standard error. ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05
Figure 5. A) Results of VBM analysis. Colour bar represents t score. B) Top: fMRI results of the contrast of interest (semantic > control) after ventricle stimulation. Bottom: Region of interest (ROI) analysis in the semantic network including the ATL, IFG, and pMTG. Light blue bars represent the ATL stimulation. White bars indicate the control (ventricle) stimulation. Black circles represent individual data. Error bars represent standard error. ** p < 0.01, * p < 0.05
Transcranial focused ultrasound stimulation of the anterior temporal lobe enhances semantic memory by modulating brain morphology, neurochemistry and neural dynamics

March 2025

·

59 Reads

Understanding neural functioning and plasticity of the brain is a fundamental goal of neuroscience. The ventromedial anterior temporal lobe (ATL) has been suggested as the centre-point of a core transmodal hub for semantic memory, playing a crucial role in the representation of coherent conceptual knowledge. However, non-invasive direct modulation of the ventromedial ATL has remained challenging. Transcranial ultrasound stimulation (TUS) is an emerging neuromodulatory technique that delivers acoustic energy with high spatial precision, making it uniquely suited for targeting deep brain structures non-invasively. In this study, we investigated whether theta-burst TUS (tbTUS) to the ventromedial ATL could enhance semantic memory performance in the adult brain. Using a multimodal neuroimaging approach, magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS), functional MRI (fMRI), and voxel-based morphometry (VBM), we assessed tbTUS-induced changes in neurochemical concentrations, functional network connectivity, structural plasticity, and semantic memory performance. Compared to control stimulation (ventricle), tbTUS at the ventromedial ATL significantly improved semantic task performance in healthy individuals. MRS analysis revealed that tbTUS decreased GABA and increased Glx levels, reflecting shifts in excitation-inhibition balance. Additionally, tbTUS increased neurometabolites in the ATL, including NAA, creatine and choline, suggesting enhanced neuronal function and metabolism. fMRI analysis demonstrated that tbTUS reduced task-induced regional activity in the ATL and functionally connected semantic regions, while also enhancing intrinsic and effective connectivity across the semantic network. Structural analysis revealed increased grey matter volume in the ATL following tbTUS compared to control stimulation. These findings provide the first convergent evidence that tbTUS can modulate neurochemistry, functional dynamics, and brain morphology to enhance semantic memory function. Our results highlight TUS as a powerful neuromodulatory tool with potential applications in cognitive enhancement and neurorehabilitation, offering a promising intervention for dementia and neurodegenerative disorders.


Multidimensional cognitive deficits in the typical and atypical variants of Alzheimer's disease

March 2025

·

10 Reads

In this two-part investigation, we examined whether Alzheimer's disease (AD) phenotypes are distinct clinical entities or represent positions within a graded multidimensional space. First, using a large retrospective dataset of past research participants (n = 413) from memory clinics, we examined the comparative distributions of cognitive performance in people diagnosed with typical amnestic AD (tAD), logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia (lvPPA), and posterior cortical atrophy (PCA). Secondly, a prospective deep phenotyping study of lvPPA (n = 18) compared to typical AD (n = 9) addressed the following questions: (1) Does the multidimensional cognitive pattern of impairment only emerge in advanced lvPPA, and how does it compare to tAD? (2) Do memory deficits in lvPPA appear in a simple clinic-level cognitive assessment or require in-depth neuropsychological investigation? (3) To what extent is performance on verbal episodic memory attributable to language impairment? (4) Do the patterns of decline in lvPPA and tAD stay categorical or multidimensional over time? We explored the associations between scores derived from a principal component analysis of cognitive measures, and grey matter volumes in key memory- and language-related brain regions, at baseline and longitudinally. The clinic-level assessment revealed similar results in both the prospective and retrospective data: (i) patients showed graded distinctions (e.g., predominant visual versus language impairment in people with PCA versus lvPPA) and overlap (e.g., shared weakness in domains such as memory); and (ii) people with lvPPA and tAD were equally impaired on both verbal and non-verbal memory tests in lvPPA and tAD. Longitudinal assessment showed phenotypic dispersion: (i) people with tAD showed varied patterns of phenotypic differentiation; and (ii) people with lvPPA and lvPPA+ exhibited a multidimensional pattern of decline with decreasing principal component scores and worsening multi-domain cognitive performance. The results of Bayesian linear regressions showed evidence for the association of grey matter volumes in language and memory networks, including the bilateral hippocampi, precuneus, posterior cingulate, and temporo-parietal regions with principal component analysis derived scores. The graded distinctions amongst typical amnestic and atypical (language and visual) phenotypes of AD support the proposal for a transdiagnostic, multidimensional phenotype geometry that spans all AD subtypes. We argue that it is essential to include all AD phenotypes in clinical trials and treatments, with a particular focus on transdiagnostic symptoms, considering their relevance to disease burden and interventions.


The role of GABA in semantic memory and its neuroplasticity

March 2025

·

10 Reads

A fundamental aspect of neuroscience is understanding neural functioning and plasticity of the brain. The anterior temporal lobe (ATL) is a hub for semantic memory, which generates coherent semantic representations about the world. GABAergic inhibition plays a crucial role in shaping human cognition and plasticity, but it is unclear how this inhibition relates to human semantic memory and its plasticity. Here, we employed a combination of continuous theta burst stimulation (cTBS), MR spectroscopy and fMRI to investigate the role of GABA in semantic memory and its neuroplasticity. Our results demonstrated that the inhibitory cTBS increased regional GABA levels in the ATL and decreased ATL blood-oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) activity during semantic processing. Importantly, changes in GABA levels were strongly associated with changes in regional activity induced by cTBS. These results suggest that GABAergic activity may be the mechanism by which cTBS induces after effects on cortical excitability. Furthermore, individuals with better semantic performance exhibited selective activity in the ATL, attributable to higher concentrations of inhibitory GABA, which can sharpen distributed semantic representations, leading to more precise semantic processing. Our results revealed a non-linear, inverted-U-shape relationship between GABA levels in the ATL and semantic performance, thus offering an explanation for the individual differences in the cTBS effect on task performance. These results provide neurochemical and anatomical specificity in shaping task-related cortical activity and behaviour. Understanding the link between neurochemistry and semantic memory has important implications for understanding individual differences in semantic behaviour and developing therapeutic interventions for patients with semantic impairments.


Figure 4: Correlation matrix for AMI-CG and self-report measures. Correlation matrix for AMI-CG total and subscale scores with self-reported AMI, GDS, and SHAPS scores, along with ACE total score (cognitive function) for all patients.The pink box highlights the crucial CG-SR discrepancy for AMI Total score as well as the three subscales. Values in the cells and background colour indicate Spearman's ρ correlation coefficients. Correlations that did not survive correction for multiple comparisons are left blank. Bonferroni correction was applied for multiple comparison correction with α = 0.00045, computed based on 0.05 / total number of correlations computed.
Figure 8: Correlation between Apathy Reporting Discrepancy (ARD) and cognitive measures. (A) Correlation matrix displaying the relationships between ARD, cognitive measures, depression, anhedonia, and demographic variables across all patients (N=314). Values in the cells and background colour indicate Spearman's ρ correlation coefficients. Correlations that did not survive correction for multiple comparisons are left blank. Bonferroni correction was applied for multiple comparison correction with α = 0.001, computed based on 0.05 / total number of correlations computed. (B) Correlation between ARD Total and ACE Total (i.e., the top left corner in (A)) across patient groups. Coloured lines indicate each cohort. Spearman's ρ and p values are shown.
Self- versus caregiver-reported apathy across neurological disorders

February 2025

·

40 Reads

Apathy is a prevalent and persistent neuropsychiatric syndrome across many neurological disorders, significantly impacting on both patients and caregivers. We systematically quantified discrepancies between self- and caregiver-reported apathy in 335 patients with a variety of diagnoses, frontotemporal dementia (behavioural variant and semantic dementia subtypes), Parkinson's disease, Parkinson's disease dementia, dementia with Lewy bodies, Alzheimer's disease dementia, mild cognitive impairment, small vessel cerebrovascular disease, subjective cognitive decline and autoimmune encephalitis. Using the Apathy-Motivation Index (AMI) and its analogous caregiver version (AMI-CG), we found that caregiver-reported apathy consistently exceeded self-reported levels across all conditions. Moreover, self-reported apathy accounted for only 14.1% of the variance in caregiver ratings. This apathy reporting discrepancy was most pronounced in conditions associated with impaired insight, such as behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia, and was significantly correlated with cognitive impairment. Deficits in memory and fluency explained an additional 11.2% of the variance in caregiver-reported apathy, highlighting their crucial role in goal-directed behaviour. Specifically, executive function deficits (e.g., indexed by fluency) and memory impairments may contribute to behavioural inertia or recall of it. These findings highlight the need to integrate patient and caregiver perspectives in apathy assessments, especially for conditions with prominent cognitive impairment. To improve diagnostic accuracy and deepen our understanding of apathy across neurological disorders, we emphasise the need of standardised apathy assessment tools tailored to individuals with cognitive deficits. Understanding the cognitive mechanisms underpinning discordant apathy reporting in dementia might help to inform targeted clinical interventions and reduce caregiver burden.


Behavioural changes in frontotemporal dementia and their cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates

February 2025

·

41 Reads

Brain

Behavioural changes are a central feature of frontotemporal dementia (FTD); they occur in both behavioural-variant (bvFTD) and semantic dementia (SD)/semantic-variant primary progressive aphasia subtypes. In this study, we addressed two current clinical knowledge gaps; (i) are there qualitative or clear distinctions between behavioural profiles in bvFTD and SD, and (ii) what are the precise roles of the prefrontal cortex and anterior temporal lobes in supporting social behaviour? Resolving these conundrums is crucial for improving diagnostic accuracy and for the development of targeted interventions to treat challenging behaviours in FTD. Informant questionnaires to assess behavioural changes included the Cambridge Behavioural Inventory-Revised and two targeted measures of apathy and impulsivity. Participants completed a detailed neuropsychological battery to permit investigation of the relationship between cognitive status (including social-semantic knowledge, general semantic knowledge and executive function) with behaviour change in FTD. To explore changes in regional grey matter volume, a subset of patients had structural MRI. Diagnosis-based group comparisons were supplemented by a transdiagnostic approach which encompassed the spectrum of bvFTD, SD and “mixed” or intermediate cases. Such an approach is sensitive to the systematic graded variation in FTD and allows the neurobiological underpinnings of behaviour change to be explored across an FTD spectrum. We found a wide range of behavioural changes across FTD. Although quantitatively more severe on average in bvFTD, as expected, the item-level analyses found no evidence for qualitative differences in behavioural profiles or “behavioural double dissociations” between bvFTD and SD. Comparisons of self and informant ratings revealed strong discrepancies in the perspective of the caregiver versus patient. Logistic regression revealed that neuropsychological measures had better discriminative accuracy for bvFTD versus SD than caregiver-reported behavioural measures. A principal component analysis of all informant questionnaire domains extracted three components, interpreted as reflecting: (1) apathy, (2) challenging behaviours and (3) activities of daily living. More severe apathy in both FTD subtypes was associated with (a) increased levels of impaired executive function and (b) anterior cingulate cortex atrophy. Questionnaire ratings of impaired behaviour did not correlate with either anterior temporal lobe atrophy or degraded social-semantic knowledge. Together, these findings highlight the presence of a wide range of behavioural changes in both bvFTD and SD, which vary by degree rather than quality. We recommend a transdiagnostic approach for future studies of the neuropsychological and neuroanatomical underpinnings of behavioural deficits in FTD.


Apraxia as a Predictor of Post-Stroke Recovery: Insights from the Birmingham Cognitive Screening Program

January 2025

·

10 Reads

Our study highlights cognitive measures underlying post-stroke recovery using the 'Birmingham Cognitive Screening' (BCOS) program, the only dataset with comprehensive assessments of cognition, praxis, and stroke outcomes. We analysed 256 stroke patients tested for cognitive deficits, which uniquely included apraxia and the Barthel Index for daily living activities at subacute and chronic stages. Using step-wise multivariate linear regression, we identified significant predictive factors: praxis, language, orientation, and baseline independence linked to improved daily activities, and validated our model with 4-fold cross-validation. This research underscores the vital role of specific cognitive measures including apraxia as key cognitive predictors for stroke recovery.


Disentangling phonology from phonological short-term memory in Alzheimer's disease phenotypes

January 2025

·

30 Reads

Impaired phonological short-term memory is a core feature of the logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia (lvPPA), but it is not clear whether a core phonological processing deficit is also present. We asked three questions: (i) beyond short-term memory impairment, do people with lvPPA have an impairment within phonology itself?; (ii) is their performance in working memory and confrontation naming reflective of this phonological impairment and/or other key contributing deficits (e.g., semantic)?; and (iii) is their repetition performance related to structural and functional differences in key language-dominant regions? We compared non-word and word repetition and short-term memory performance in patients with typical, amnestic Alzheimer's disease (tAD, n = 9), lvPPA per consensus criteria (n = 10), and others who previously satisfied definitions of lvPPA but had progressed with multi-domain cognitive impairments (lvPPA+, n = 8). Bayesian analyses revealed no group differences in phonological tasks of word and non-word repetition. There was, however, a very strong group effect on multi-syllabic word/phrase repetition. We found very strong evidence for an effect of self-reported hearing loss on both word and non-word repetition, but not multi-syllabic word/phrase repetition. A comparison of phonological (as indexed by non-word repetition) versus working memory (as measured by multisyllabic word repetition and digit span) and confrontation naming tasks produced either no evidence or evidence for no correlation. Confrontation naming correlated positively with multisyllabic word/phrase repetition and semantic assessments across the whole group. Beyond the expected grey matter reductions in patients relative to controls in the left posterior superior temporal gyrus, anterior temporal lobe, and inferior temporal gyrus, we found no evidence for the associations between non-word repetition and functional connectivity between all regions of interest in the whole group. When excluding controls, the only anecdotal evidence that was corroborated by frequentist multiple regression was for an association between non-word repetition and functional connectivity between dorsal premotor and posterior superior temporal gyrus after controlling for the grey matter volumes in these regions. We have shown that deficits in "pure" phonological tasks, namely single word and non-word repetition, are (i) not dependent on working memory and (ii) greater in patients with a self-reported hearing loss across all groups with lvPPA or tAD. Our results suggest that instead of having a core phonological impairment, lvPPA patients have a working memory/buffering impairment that adversely affects their performance on length-dependent working memory tasks.


Citations (45)


... 22,42-44 Recent studies have investigated semantic cognition in lvPPA. 9,45,46 In a previous study, 47 we demonstrated that, although individuals with lvPPA do have semantic impairments, these are of a distinctively different type and degree to that observed in svPPA/SD. In contrast to the representational degradation in svPPA/SD and more akin to the pattern observed in semantic aphasia and Wernicke's aphasia patients, [48][49][50][51][52][53] lvPPA patients exhibited a deficit in semantic control (the ability to access and manipulate semantic representations flexibly for a specific context or task), showing presentation of AD (tAD), 69 ten patients met strict criteria for lvPPA, 1,70 and eight patients were classified as "lvPPA+" as they previously satisfied definitions of lvPPA but, at the time of this study, they exhibited multi-domain cognitive impairments. ...

Reference:

Disentangling phonology from phonological short-term memory in Alzheimer's disease phenotypes
Impaired semantic control in the logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia
  • Citing Article
  • December 2024

Brain Communications

... The left superior/medial frontal gyrus is implicated in self-initiated responses (Robinson et al., 2012) and estimation of cognitive effort (Hauser et al., 2017), while the left lateral posterior temporal cortex (BA37) is associated with semantic control (Hodgson et al., 2023) and executive control processes (Zhang et al., 2021). To explore these regions' roles in creative activities in more detail and anatomical precision, future research based on new fMRI comparisons across tasks within the same participants will be needed, as recent studies have done for the middle/inferior frontal gyri (Assem et al., 2024;Chiou et al., 2025) and inferior parietal cortex (Humphreys et al., 2024). ...

The Dimensionality of Neural Coding for Cognitive Control Is Gradually Transformed within the Lateral Prefrontal Cortex

The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience

... Previous reports have consistently shown that caregivers report higher levels of apathy in patients than patients report themselves. 15,21,24,27,[30][31][32]42,[79][80][81] Without an objective measure of goaldirected behaviour, it remains unclear whether patients underreport their symptoms of caregivers overreport them. Individuals with apathy may perceive themselves as "content" and be less troubled by behavioural changes, whereas caregiver burden may heighten sensitivity to signs of apathy. ...

Behavioural changes in frontotemporal dementia and their cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates

... For example, deep orthographies such as English often have words that do not have one-to-one spelling and sound correspondences (e.g., yacht or colonel) or may have words that are similar in their written form but differing in their spoken form (e.g., hint -pint) 16 . These irregular words may thus require input from semantically mediated pathways (e.g., spelling-to-meaning-sound) to be correctly pronounced 14,[17][18][19] . Additionally, the PSH provides a framework for examining how reading behavior or neural responses may be individually or simultaneously influenced by specific psycholinguistic factors related to components of reading such as word frequency (the degree to which a word appears within a given language; e.g., work vs. wilt), consistency (the degree to which a word's written form matches its sound form; e.g., hint vs. pint) and imageability (the degree to which a word evokes of a mental image; e.g., moth vs. truth). ...

Simultaneous simulations of pure, surface and phonological acquired dyslexia within a full computational model of the primary systems hypothesis

Cortex

... 47,48 Direct comparisons between the left and right ATL atrophy in SD are not straightforward; however, even if asymmetrical, the pathology is always bilateral, making it hard to unpick the relative contributions of each side. 1,49,50 Indeed, L > R SD patients can also develop behavioural impairment. Furthermore, R > L patients typically present to clinic later than L > R patients and, consequently, often have more severe temporal lobe atrophy 1,42 and increased atrophy in pre-frontal regions important for social behaviour. ...

The impact of bilateral versus unilateral anterior temporal lobe damage on face recognition, person knowledge and semantic memory

Cerebral Cortex

... Finally, controversies persist regarding the differences in language profiles between PCA and other AD populations, such as lvPPA and even tAD. Although tAD patients have been reported to exhibit language profiles similar to those of PCA, more comprehensive studies are needed to clarify these relationships 33,34,25,18,21,31,21 . ...

Graded Multidimensional Clinical and Radiologic Variation in Patients With Alzheimer Disease and Posterior Cortical Atrophy
  • Citing Article
  • July 2024

Neurology

... Indeed, large cohort studies have reported similar rates of behaviour change in 10 SD and bvFTD. 17,19,26,27 Unlike bvFTD, the atrophy in SD is primarily centred on the ATLs. 15 In 11 their severest form, the co-occurring semantic and behavioural impairments are reminiscent of the 12 ...

The graded multidimensional geometry of phenotypic variation and progression in neurodegenerative syndromes
  • Citing Article
  • July 2024

Brain

... Using an active control (i.e., stimulating a brain region not involved with the tFUS primary target region [46],) or a volume not containing neuronal cells (i.e., stimulating the cerebral ventricles) [47]. ...

Ventricle stimulation as a potential gold-standard control stimulation site for transcranial focused ultrasound stimulation

... This study serves as a worked example demonstrating how parallel meta-analyses of tDCS studies from different subfields could guide the discovery of unifying explanations regarding the cognitive and neural bases of a cognitive activity (as has been done with ALE meta-analysis of fMRI data: Humphreys & Lambon Ralph, 2023;Humphreys et al., 2024;Chan et al., 2025). We believe that this approach not only applies to the understanding of creative thought, but also many other higher cognitive abilities e.g., working memory. ...

The left posterior angular gyrus is engaged by autobiographical recall not object-semantics, or event-semantics: Evidence from contrastive propositional speech production

... As shown in Fig. 3, a cluster-based permutation test across the left fronto-temporal search volume revealed a significant cluster within an anterior-mid portion of the left ventromedial temporal lobe (unexpected > expected), p = .002. This cluster spanned two neuroanatomical subregions: (a) a left ventral temporal region (including the anterior inferior temporal and anterior-mid fusiform cortex, 15 voxels), which has been implicated in lexical processing, i.e. mapping orthographic forms on to sets of semantic features (Hirshorn et al., 2016;Woolnough et al., 2021), and (b) a left medial temporal region (including the parahippocampal gyrus and hippocampus, 5 voxels), which has been implicated in domain-general conceptual processingmapping distributed semantic features on to unique concepts (Cox et al., 2024;Lambon-Ralph et al., 2017;Patterson et al. 2007). ...

Representational similarity learning reveals a graded multidimensional semantic space in the human anterior temporal cortex