Mary C. Larson’s research while affiliated with University of Minnesota Duluth and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (14)


Behavioral and Physiological Responsivity, Sleep, and Patterns of Daily Cortisol Production in Infants with and without Colic
  • Chapter

December 2002

·

4 Reads

·

1 Citation

·

·

Mary C. Larson

·

[...]

·


Figure 1 Proportion of time infants expressed negative vocalizations by periods of the test procedure. Bars reflect standard errors of the mean. Note that Baseline refers to cardiac baseline occurring after electrode placement, not to behavioral baseline before any manipulations. 
Table 1 Characteristics of Study Sample Characteristics Control ( n 20) Colic ( n 20)
Figure 2 Group differences in intensity of crying, consolability, and distress to limitations. Values have been converted to standard scores for the purposes of explication. Bars reflect standard errors of the mean. 
Figure 3 Proportion of negative vocalizations scored as fuss, minimal cry, moderate cry, and intense cry. 
Figure 4 Heart rate (A), vagal tone (B), and salivary cortisol (C) shown by group and by sampling period. Bars reflect standard errors of the mean. 

+1

Behavioral and Physiological Responsivity, Sleep, and Patterns of Daily Cortisol Production in Infants with and without Colic
  • Article
  • Full-text available

July 2000

·

275 Reads

·

166 Citations

Child Development

To describe the behavioral and physiological responses associated with colic, the responses of 20 two-month-old infants with and 20 without colic were studied during a physical examination. Parents kept a diary of infant behaviors (including crying and fussing) for 3 days following the visit. Using Wessel, Cobb, Jackson, Harris, & Detwiler criteria, colic was defined as fussing/crying for 3 hr or more on each of the 3 days. Behavioral data coded by ‘blind’ observers showed that during the physical exam, colic infants cried twice as much, cried more intensely, and were more inconsolable than were control infants. Despite these behavioral differences, heart rate, vagal tone, and cortisol measures indicated no appreciable difference in physiological responsivity for the two groups. At home, parents collected saliva cortisol samples at wakeup, midmorning, midafternoon, and evening for 2 days. In a finding similar to that shown by the laboratory data, the colic and control infants did not have different levels of daily average cortisol. These laboratory and home data provide no evidence of greater responsivity in the physiological substrate of difficult temperament for colic infants and are consistent with evidence of similarity in temperament once colic is resolved. At home, compared with control infants, colic infants did display a blunted rhythm in cortisol production. By diary, they also slept about 2 hr less per day than did control infants. Nighttime sleep was still significantly different when fussing/crying was statistically controlled. These data suggest that colic might be associated with a disruption or delay in the establishment of the circadian rhythm in activity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical axis and associated sleep – wake activity.

Download

Dampening of the cortisol response to handling at 3 months in human infants and its relation to sleep, circadian cortisol activity, and behavioral distress

January 1999

·

48 Reads

·

97 Citations

Developmental Psychobiology

The decrease in responsiveness of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) system is marked over the first months of life. Seventy-eight healthy infants (44 girls), 7 to 15 weeks old, were given a laboratory mock physical examination. Salivary cortisol samples were collected pre- and postexamination and at home. Behavioral state during the examination and home sleep/wake activity were measured. Subjects younger than 11 weeks showed an increase in pre- to postexamination cortisol, while older subjects did not. Further, there was no decrease in behavioral distress to the examination with age. Infants who showed an early- morning peak (EMP) in home cortisol levels were significantly older and were likely to be those who slept through the night. However, the presence of an EMP was not associated with a lack of cortisol response to the examination. The decrease in cortisol responsiveness witnessed around the age of 3 months is presumably due to other processes associated with age, and not with the expression of the day-night rhythm in basal cortisol.


Dampening of the cortisol response to handling at 3 months in human infants and its relation to sleep, circadian cortisol activity, and behavioral distress

December 1998

·

53 Reads

·

95 Citations

Developmental Psychobiology

The decrease in responsiveness of the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical (HPA) system is marked over the first months of life. Seventy-eight healthy infants (44 girls), 7 to 15 weeks old, were given a laboratory mock physical examination. Salivary cortisol samples were collected pre- and postexamination and at home. Behavioral state during the examination and home sleep/wake activity were measured. Subjects younger than 11 weeks showed an increase in pre- to postexamination cortisol, while older subjects did not. Further, there was no decrease in behavioral distress to the examination with age. Infants who showed an early-morning peak (EMP) in home cortisol levels were significantly older and were likely to be those who slept through the night. However, the presence of an EMP was not associated with a lack of cortisol response to the examination. The decrease in cortisol responsiveness witnessed around the age of 3 months is presumably due to other processes associated with age, and not with the expression of the day–night rhythm in basal cortisol. © 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Dev Psychobiol 33: 327–337, 1998



Neonatal Stress Reactivity: Predictions to Later Emotional Temperament

March 1995

·

48 Reads

·

170 Citations

Child Development

To investigate the relations among popular measures of neonatal stress and their link to subsequent temperament, 50 full-term newborns from a normal care nursery were examined responding to a heelstick blood draw. Baseline and heelstick measures of behavioral state, heart period, vagal tone, and salivary cortisol were obtained. Recovery measures of behavioral and cardiac activity were also analyzed. Mothers completed Rothbart's Infant Behavior Questionnaire when their infants reached 6 months of age. Baseline vagal tone predicted cortisol in response to the heelstick, suggesting that baseline vagal tone reflects the infants' ability to react to stressors. Greater reactivity to the heelstick (more crying, shorter heart periods, lower vagal tone, and higher cortisol) was associated with lower scores on "Distress-to-Limitations" temperament at 6 months. This finding was consistent with the expectation that the capacity to react strongly to an aversive stimulus would reflect better neurobehavioral organization in the newborn. Recovery measures of cardiac activity approximated and were correlated with baseline measures indicating the strong self-righting properties of the healthy newborn. Finally, vagal tone and salivary cortisol measures were not significantly related, suggesting the importance of assessing both systems in studies of the ontogeny of stress-temperament relations.


First time experiences in infancy: When they appear to be pleasant, Do they activate the adrenocortical stress response?

July 1992

·

35 Reads

·

35 Citations

Developmental Psychobiology

The effect of a positive first-time experience on adrenocortical activity was examined in 48 6- to 13-month-old infants who took part in two sessions of a YMCA-like mother-infant swim class. Experience was manipulated by comparing Novice swimmers with infants who previously had taken a swim class, and by examining responses to first versus second class session. In addition, the effects of temperamental fear of novelty were examined by maternal report prior to the first session. Preswim and Postswim cortisol levels were compared to measures obtained at the same time of day in two comparison groups, one tested at home and one tested in a playroom at the University. The results yielded no evidence of an adrenocortical stress response (elevated cortisol) to the swim sessions for either the Novice or Experienced swimmers. Infant temperamental fearfulness predicted behavioral but not adrenocortical responses during swimming. Postswim cortisol levels were significantly lower than posttest levels in the two baseline comparison groups, and lower postswim cortisol levels were associated with less negative and more positive emotional behavior and engagement of the swimming experience. These results are consistent with the hypothesis that emotions serve as a major pathway influencing adrenocortical activity, and provide no support for the uncertainty hypothesis of adrenocortical activation.


The Stressfulness of Separation among Nine-Month-Old Infants: Effects of Social Context Variables and Infant Temperament

May 1992

·

74 Reads

·

275 Citations

Child Development

This manuscript reports on the results of 2 experiments dealing with behavioral and adrenocortical responses to separation among 9-month-old human infants. In both experiments the social context of separation was manipulated. The results of Experiment 1 yielded evidence of a statistically significant adrenocortical response to 30 min of separation under conditions in which the substitute caregiver responded sensitivity to infant distress, but was busy and relatively noninteractive when babies were not distressed during the separation period. Altering the behavior of the substitute caregiver such that she was warm, responsive, and interactive throughout the separation produced a significant reduction in adrenocortical activity and in negative affect. In fact, these measures were not significantly different than those obtained when the mother and infant remained together in the playroom (No Separation). In Experiment 2, the effects of group versus singleton care were examined using the less stressful mode of substitute caregiving as described above. No significant condition differences in behavioral distress or cortisol were found. Furthermore, neither condition elicited a significant increase in cortisol over basal levels. Finally, these data provide evidence that maternal reports of infant Distress to Limits temperament, using Rothbart's Infant Behavior Questionnaire, predict adrenocortical responses to separation, while reports of Fear of Novelty do not.


The Stressfulness of Separation among Nine‐Month‐Old Infants: Effects of Social Context Variables and Infant Temperament

April 1992

·

34 Reads

·

183 Citations

Child Development

This manuscript reports on the results of 2 experiments dealing with behavioral and adrenocortical responses to separation among 9-month-old human infants. In both experiments the social context of separation was manipulated. The results of Experiment 1 yielded evidence of a statistically significant adrenocortical response to 30 min of separation under conditions in which the substitute caregiver responded sensitively to infant distress, but was busy and relatively noninteractive when babies were not distressed during the separation period. Altering the behavior of the substitute caregiver such that she was warm, responsive, and interactive throughout the separation produced a significant reduction in adrenocortical activity and in negative affect. In fact, these measures were not significantly different than those obtained when the mother and infant remained together in the playroom (No Separation). In Experiment 2, the effects of group versus singleton care were examined using the less stressful mode of substitute caregiving as described above. No significant condition differences in behavioral distress or cortisol were found. Furthermore, neither condition elicited a significant increase in cortisol over basal levels. Finally, these data provide evidence that maternal reports of infant Distress to Limits temperament, using Rothbart's Infant Behavior Questionnaire, predict adrenocortical responses to separation, while reports of Fear of Novelty do not.


Cortisol and behavioral responses to repeated stressors in human newborn

November 1991

·

30 Reads

·

112 Citations

Developmental Psychobiology

This manuscript reports on the effects of stressors repeated at a 24-hr interval on three samples of human newborns. In Study 1, newborns meeting criteria for obstetric and perinatal optimality experienced either 2 mock Discharge Exams, viewed as a type of handling stressor, or 2 Heelstick Blood Draws, viewed as a type of nocioceptive stressor. As in a previous study, newborns meeting optimality criteria showed habituation of the adrenocortical response to the repeated Discharge Exam. The adrenocortical response to the repeated Heelstick tended to increase or sensitize. In Studies 2 and 3, samples of newborns with more nonoptimal obstetric complication scale scores were examined. These newborns failed to show habituation of the adrenocortical response to the Discharge Exam, but responded like "Optimal" newborns to the repeated Heelstick. The applicability to these data of the Groves and Thompson (1970) Dual-Process Model of Habituation is discussed. The results also provided evidence of stability in behavioral distress independent of stability in adrenocortical activity. The implications of these data for studies using cortisol as an index of the physiological basis of early temperament are considered.


Citations (13)


... Mostly, our participants' saliva was sampled during the day, while colic most commonly debuts in the early hours of the night. White et al. [41] have previously described a blunted cortisol rhythm in infants with colic, but their overall cortisol levels in infants with colic did not differ from their controls. Although physical pain has been a suggested stressor in infants [11], colic and abdominal pain is a common stressful concern for parents [42]. ...

Reference:

Maternal Stress, Early Life Factors and Infant Salivary Cortisol Levels
Behavioral and Physiological Responsivity, Sleep, and Patterns of Daily Cortisol Production in Infants with and without Colic
  • Citing Chapter
  • December 2002

... In the present study, infants' cortisol levels decreased in a laboratory setting after an extended warm-up phase with the parent. This finding supports the idea that a short or no warm-up phase might have contributed to the absent or low increase in infants' cortisol levels after having experienced a stress induction similar to the strange situation in previous studies (e.g., Beijers et al., 2013; Gunnar et al., 1989; Nachmias et al., 1996; Spangler and Schieche, 1998). The t1 salivary cortisol levels found in the present study fell within the range of baseline levels obtained in these studies. ...

Attachment, Temperament, and Adrenocortical Activity in Infancy: A Study of Psychoendocrine Regulation
  • Citing Article
  • May 1989

Developmental Psychology

... Im Unterschied zu sicher gebundenen Kindern zeigen diese aber in den Trennungsphasen keinen oder nur sehr geringen Trennungsschmerz, vermeiden in der Wiedervereinigungsepisode aktiv den Blickkontakt zur Mutter und fokussieren sich auf das Spielzeug (Solomon & George, 1999 Besonderheiten bei der Messung von Cortisolwerten finden sich aber bei ambivalent gebundenen Kindern. Während Gunnar et al. (1989) keine Unterschiede zwischen den organisierten Bindungsmustern finden, sprechen Luijk et al. (2010) (Solomon & George, 1999). Offenbar ist hier aber auch eine Gen-Umwelt-Interaktion von Relevanz. ...

Attachment, Temperament, and Adrenocortical Activity in Infancy: A Study of Psychoendocrine Regulation

Developmental Psychology

... Furthermore, in adults, the two main adrenocortical steroid hormones (aldosterone and cortisol) follow a diurnal rhythm (higher in the morning, declining throughout the day, even to zero in the evening) (Loomis et al., 2015). However, the diurnal pattern of adrenocortical steroids is not present obviously for infants (Simons et al., 2015;Ivars et al., 2017;˙I nce et al., 2018;Hollanders et al., 2020) and in a transition phase to adult-type (Price et al., 1983;Larson et al., 1998;Hollanders et al., 2020). Dhayat et al. (2015) conducted a longitudinal study to evaluate infant urinary steroid metabolites, and found that glucocorticoid and mineralocorticoid were excreted at a constant level (small range of fluctuations) in the first few months of life. ...

Dampening of the cortisol response to handling at 3 months in human infants and its relation to sleep, circadian cortisol activity, and behavioral distress

Developmental Psychobiology

... There was no significant difference between the repeated measurements in the control group. It has been demonstrated that the daily average cortisol levels in colicky infants were similar to the control group, but the rhythm of cortisol was irregular in colicky infants (52). ...

Behavioral and Physiological Responsivity, Sleep, and Patterns of Daily Cortisol Production in Infants with and without Colic

Child Development

... Examples of experimental situations include the "Strange Situation Procedure" (Biro et al., 2021;Peltola et al., 2020;Cortes Hidalgo et al., 2019;Quevedo et al., 2017;Lyons-Ruth et al., 2016;Moutsiana et al., 2014Moutsiana et al., , 2015Tharner et al., 2011), "Still-face procedure" (Catalina Camacho et al., 2019), separation paradigm (Swingler et al., 2007(Swingler et al., , 2010Gunnar and Nelson, 1994) and divided attention task (Mize and Jones, 2012;Dawson et al., 1999). While all these procedures allow infants' behaviours to be investigated Gunnar et al., 1992 Gunnar andNelson, 1994 Manchester Assessment of Caregiver-Infant Interaction (MACI; Wan et al., 2012) Elsabbagh et al., 2015Kochanska, 1997, 1998Hane and Fox, 2006Custom Dawson et al., 1999Hanford et al., 2018 Affect recovery Affect following a mildly stressful situation Dawson et al., 1999 Mize andJones, 2012 Communication Infant's level of engagement and communication (e.g., vocal and nonvocal behaviour directed towards the partner) Global Rating Scale (GRS; Murray et al., 1996) Sethna et al., 2017 Inhibition Level of restraint of exploration: proximity to caregiver during play and when stranger approaches; latency to approach a stranger and a novel toy. Custom Jones et al., 2009 "Interactive behaviour" Measured using positive facial affect, directed attention, and positive vocalizations. ...

The Stressfulness of Separation among Nine‐Month‐Old Infants: Effects of Social Context Variables and Infant Temperament
  • Citing Article
  • April 1992

Child Development

... This is concerning, as the number of days infants are separated from their COVID-19-positive mothers was found to be negatively correlated with infant development in several domains . Even brief maternal separation is known to increase infant cortisol (Larson et al., 1991) and there is evidence for increased microglial density in response to maternal separation (Howes, 2017). ...

The Effects of Morning Naps, Car Trips, and Maternal Separation on Adrenocortical Activity in Human Infants
  • Citing Article
  • April 1991

Child Development

... Researchers use this index to understand the stress levels in acute or long time periods [25]. According to the literature, salivary cortisol concertation in both infants [26] and children [27] gave reliable results and showed that children's mood depends on it and when they are engaged with one activity the cortisol's concentration values alter. ...

First time experiences in infancy: When they appear to be pleasant, Do they activate the adrenocortical stress response?
  • Citing Article
  • July 1992

Developmental Psychobiology

... Lowered plasma cortisol levels measured in saliva can also be indicative of stress in children as young as nine months old (Gunnar et al., 1992). Perroud et al. (2014) asserted that such an effect can be explained by the transmission of parental ptsd to offspring (i.e., the transmission of epigenetic processes, such as the methylation status of the gr gene). ...

The Stressfulness of Separation among Nine-Month-Old Infants: Effects of Social Context Variables and Infant Temperament
  • Citing Article
  • May 1992

Child Development

... During the hospital stay, very preterm infants were exposed to 24 (12-38) skin-breaking procedures, whereas the exposure in moderate to late preterm and term neonates was significantly lower (6 [4][5][6][7][8][9][10][11][12][13] and 7 [3][4][5][6][7][8][9][10][11], as appropriate, p < 0.001). Similarly, very preterm infants experienced the highest number of total procedures related to hospitalization in comparison to the other groups (64 [43-100] vs. 24 [18][19][20][21][22][23][24][25][26][27][28][29][30][31][32] vs. 19 [15][16][17][18][19][20][21][22][23][24][25][26], p < 0.001). ...

Cortisol and behavioral responses to repeated stressors in human newborn
  • Citing Article
  • November 1991

Developmental Psychobiology