Marissa A. Ramsier’s research while affiliated with Humboldt State University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (16)


Audiograms of howling monkeys: are extreme loud calls the result of runaway selection?
  • Preprint
  • File available

February 2019

·

317 Reads

·

3 Citations

Marissa A Ramsier

·

Andrew J Cunningham

·

May R Patiño

·

[...]

·

The eponymous vocalizations of howling monkeys (genus Alouatta ) are associated with territorial defense and male-male competition, yet the extreme loudness of howls, which are among the loudest vocalizations of any terrestrial mammal, have yet to be fully explained. Loudness facilitates long-distance sound propagation but the effectiveness of any vocal signal depends in part on the auditory capabilities of the intended receiver, and the auditory sensitivities of howling monkeys are unknown. To better understand the evolution of loud calls, we used the auditory brainstem response (ABR) method to estimate the auditory sensitivities of Alouatta palliata . The mean estimated audiogram of four wild-caught adults displayed a w-shaped pattern with two regions of enhanced sensitivity centered at 0.7-1.0 and 11.3 kHz. The lower-frequency region of auditory sensitivity is pitched moderately higher than the fundamental frequencies of howling, whereas the higher-frequency region corresponds well with harmonics in an infant distress call, the wrah-ha . Fitness advantages from detecting infants amid low-frequency background noise, including howling, could explain the incongruity between our ABR thresholds and the fundamental frequencies of howling. Attending to infant calls is expected to enhance reproductive success within an infanticidal genus, and we suggest that the extraordinary loudness of male howling is an indirect (runaway) result of positive feedback between the selective pressures of hearing infant distress calls and deterring infanticide.

Download

Primate Audition: Reception, Perception, and Ecology

September 2017

·

91 Reads

·

10 Citations

The auditory system of nonhuman primates shows evidence of many similarities to humans, such as specializations for the processing of vocalizations overall, processing species-specific vocalizations in particular, and in some cases, the recognition of specific individuals based on call structure. Additionally, nonhuman primates are similar to humans in their excellent localization acuity. Nonhuman primates show differences from humans, though, and not only in the subtleties of the aforementioned abilities. With respect to overall auditory sensitivity, primates have traditionally been portrayed as unspecialized, although there is variation between species. Species in the semiorder Strepsirrhini are, on average, more adept at detecting higher frequencies, whereas the Haplorhini are, on average, more adept at detecting lower frequencies. In addition, a well-supported allometric model explains that smaller headed species with smaller interaural distances need to utilize high-frequency cues for sound localization. Overall auditory sensitivity, particularly to high frequencies, also has been related to increased sociality in some primates. The lack of identification of additional broad trends and relationships between audition and ecology may be partially attributed to the limited dataset, which lacks representation from several major taxonomic subgroups. Additionally, order-wide trends may be minimal given the many possible reasons why enhanced or reduced sensitivity to certain frequency regions may be beneficial for different species. These are just a few of the many facets of primate audition that need to be explored in more depth through additional data gathered via continually evaluated and refined methodologies.


Introduction to Primate Hearing and Communication

September 2017

·

96 Reads

·

3 Citations

The diverse and well-studied order Primates serves as an excellent model for understanding the evolution of acoustic communication among mammals. Over the past 60 million years, primates have evolved into more than 300 extant species that range from nocturnal to diurnal, arboreal to terrestrial, and solitary to groups of thousands, and they range in body mass from the 30-g pygmy mouse lemur (Microcebus myoxinus) to the 175-kg eastern lowland gorilla (Gorilla beringei graueri). Nonhuman primates vary in their auditory sensitivity and perceptual capabilities and emit a wide range of often complex vocalizations. Some aspects of primate audition and vocalizations have been related to each other and/or phylogeny, anatomy, and ecology, but many aspects have yet to be fully understood. The integration of anatomical and behavioral data on acoustic communication, and the correlates thereof, have significant potential for reconstructing behavior in the fossil record, including that of humans. This volume presents a comprehensive review of nonhuman primate audition and vocal communication to bridge these closely related topics that are often addressed separately. The first section of the book is a discussion of primate sound production, reception, and perception, as well as habitat acoustics in the environmental settings occupied by primates in the wild. The second section focuses on vocal communication in extant primates, including consideration of spectral analyses of primate calls and the evolutionary relationships among hearing, vocal communication, and human language. The goal for this comprehensive approach is to provide new insights into these related topics.


FIG. 1. The ABR of subject B in response to 16 kHz stimuli. (a) Waveforms evoked in response to decreasing stimulus level. (b) Corresponding response amplitude (dots with horizontal error bars) and relative (uncalibrated) 50 nV threshold (star) determined by an ordinary least squares regression (dashed line) of response amplitude by stimulus level. 
Figure 2 of 2
Auditory sensitivity of the tufted capuchin (Sapajus apella), a test of allometric predictions

June 2017

·

180 Reads

·

3 Citations

The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America

New World monkeys are a diverse primate group and a model for understanding hearing in mammals. However, comparable audiograms do not exist for the larger monkeys, making it difficult to test the hypothesized relationship between interaural distance and high-frequency hearing limit (i.e., the allometric model). Here, the auditory brainstem response (ABR) method is used to assess auditory sensitivity in four tufted capuchins (Sapajus apella), a large monkey with a large interaural distance. A primate-typical four-peak pattern in the ABR waveforms was found with peak latencies from ca. 2 to 12 ms after stimulus onset. Response amplitude decreased linearly with decreasing stimulus level (mean r² = 0.93, standard deviation 0.14). Individual variation in each threshold was moderate (mean ± 7 dB). The 10-dB bandwidth of enhanced sensitivity was 2–16 kHz—a range comparable to smaller monkeys and congruent with the bandwidth of their vocal repertoire. In accord with the general principles of the allometric model, the 60-dB high-frequency limit of S. apella (26 kHz) is lower than those of smaller-headed monkeys; however, it is substantially lower than 44.7 kHz, the value predicted by the allometric model. These findings and other exceptions to the allometric model warrant cautious application and further investigation of other potential selective factors.


Sensory Ecology: Bioacoustics

April 2017

·

39 Reads

Primate bioacoustics includes the study of sounds produced or received by primates, including the acoustic structure of such sounds, underlying mechanisms of production and reception/perception, associated meanings/behaviors, and the influence of habitat structure. The majority of data on primate bioacoustics are focused on vocalizations, both to aid in interpreting the behavior of nonhuman primates, and for comparative purposes in the quest to understand the evolution of human speech and language. Comparably little attention has been paid to the other aspects of primate bioacoustics; however, it is clear that habitat structure may have played an important role in the evolution of primate vocalizations, that primates as a whole are adept at detecting the location of sound sources, and that there may be complex coevolutionary relationships between vocal acoustics, sociality, size, and audition.


Primate Hearing and Communication

January 2017

·

104 Reads

·

14 Citations

Primate Hearing and Communication provides unique insights into the evolution of hearing and communication in primates, including humans. · Introduction to Primate Hearing and Communication Marissa A. Ramsier and Rolf M. Quam · The Primate Peripheral Auditory System and the Evolution of Primate Hearing SirpaNummela · Primate Audition: Reception, Perception, and Ecology Marissa A. Ramsier and Josef P. Rauschecker · Primate Habitat Acoustics Charles H. Brown and Peter M. Waser · Evolutionary Origins of Primate Vocal Communication: Diversity, Flexibility, and Complexity of Vocalizations in Basal Primates Elke Zimmermann · Vocal Communication in Family-Living and Pair-Bonded Primates Charles T. Snowdon · The Primate Roots of Human Language Klaus Zuberbühler · Evolution of Hearing and Language in Fossil Hominins Rolf M. Quam, Ignacio Martínez, Manuel Rosa, and Juan Luis Arsuaga Rolf M. Quam is Associate Professor in the Department of Anthropology, Binghamton University (SUNY), Binghamton, NY Marissa A. Ramsier is Assistant Professor in the Department of Anthropology, Humboldt State University, Arcata, CA Richard R. Fay is Distinguished Research Professor of Psychology at Loyola University of Chicago Arthur N. Popper is Professor Emeritus and Research Professor in the Department of Biology at the University of Maryland, College Park


Receiver bias and the acoustic ecology of aye-ayes (Daubentonia madagascariensis)

November 2012

·

292 Reads

·

22 Citations

Communicative & Integrative Biology

The aye-aye is a rare lemur from Madagascar that uses its highly specialized middle digit for percussive foraging. This acoustic behavior, also termed tap-scanning, produces dominant frequencies between 6 and 15 kHz. An enhanced auditory sensitivity to these frequencies raises the possibility that the acoustic and auditory specializations of aye-ayes have imposed constraints on the evolution of their vocal signals, especially their primary long-distance vocalization, the screech. Here we explore this concept, termed receiver bias, and suggest that the dominant frequency of the screech call (~2.7 kHz) represents an evolutionary compromise between the opposing adaptive advantages of long-distance sound propagation and enhanced detection by conspecific receivers.




Evolution of Auditory Sensitivity Among Strepsirhine Primates

August 2012

·

9 Reads

·

4 Citations

Hearing is a crucial element of primate behavior and ecology. Beginning in 1969, traditional behavioral testing methods produced comparable audiograms for five strepsirhine taxa. Variation in this relatively small data set can be explained in part by head size, but relationships with social behavior and ecology have been elusive. Recently, with the use of auditory brainstem response (ABR) methods, standardized audiograms have been published for eleven strepsirhine species. Within this data set, social complexity explains a significant amount of the variation in auditory sensitivity, possibly because sociality favored an enhanced ability to detect conspecific vocalizations such as high frequency alarm calls. These findings shed light on the comparative biology of primate hearing and enable a reconstruction of the ancestral strepsirhine audiogram.


Citations (11)


... 10,15 However, the auditory sensitivities of primates can vary widely, sometimes as a function of phylogeny and head-size constraints, 10,16 or the pressures of natural and sexual selection. [17][18][19] The genetic basis of auditory perception was poorly under- Since then, at least 70 genes have been identified in humans as underlying the auditory sense, 20 but unfortunately, comparative study of the genetic basis of audition in nonhuman primates and other mammals remains almost nonexistent. A rare exception is recent work by Bankoff et al., 21 who compared seven genes involved in auditory processing across echolocating mammals (bats, cetaceans) and primates to study the molecular evolution of specialized hearing in aye-ayes (Daubentonia madagascariensis). ...

Reference:

The sensory ecology of primate food perception, revisited
Audiograms of howling monkeys: are extreme loud calls the result of runaway selection?

... Recently, Ramsier et al. ( 2012 ) described the audiogram of four captive adult A. palliata . The mean audiogram was dual peaked (w shaped) with two regions of enhanced sensitivity separated by a mid-frequency "dip" of decreased sensitivity. ...

Hearing sensitivity and the evolution of acoustic communication in platyrrhine monkeys
  • Citing Conference Paper
  • March 2012

... The ability to discriminate between individuals based on their vocalizations has been demonstrated in several taxa (Narins et al., 2006;Quam et al., 2017;Suthers et al., 2016). When individuals differ in the presence or absence of certain signal attributes or when there is a greater betweenindividual variability than within-individual variability in one or more signal properties, it results in distinctiveness (Beecher, 1982(Beecher, , 1989Bee et al., 2001). ...

Primate Hearing and Communication
  • Citing Book
  • January 2017

... Strepsirrhines are generally conceptualized as having small, simple and undifferentiated facial muscles with little complexity in how these muscles connect with one another (Burrows et al., 2011), yet few studies are available to support this argument. While Lemuriformes vary in size, behavior and ecology, Lorisiformes are considerably more uniform in their small bodies, nocturnality and high arboreality (Ramsier & Rauschecker, 2017). Despite the variability among the strepsirrhine suborder, they generally favor multi-modal communication systems, such as olfactory and/or vocal communication (Kappeler, 2012). ...

Primate Audition: Reception, Perception, and Ecology
  • Citing Chapter
  • September 2017

... Experimental studies have produced audiograms measuring hearing sensitivity in primates for a great range of frequencies. Audiograms for Strepsirrhini show a V-shape, with maximum sensitivity ranging from 8 to 16 kHz (Quam et al., 2012;Ramsier, Vinyard, & Dominy, 2017), indicating that this group is adapted to the perception of high frequencies. In Haplorhini, audiograms show more interspecies variation. ...

Auditory sensitivity of the tufted capuchin (Sapajus apella), a test of allometric predictions

The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America

... How social complexity can be reliably determined is debated. Some researchers rely on group size (e.g., Kappeler 2012) or foraging unit size (e.g., Ramsier et al. 2012a;Ramsier 2013). In contrast, others (e.g., Thierry et al. 2000;Freeberg et al. 2012) suggest that more complex distinctions need to be considered, and they characterize social complexity by social unit size (stable in space and over time), unit density, nature of member roles, egalitarian/despotic structure, and/or interunit density. ...

Evolution of Auditory Sensitivity Among Strepsirhine Primates
  • Citing Chapter
  • August 2012

... This was confirmed by Emerson who used a tool to amplify the sounds produced by soldiers of R. flavipes species but was unable to detect any sound during the "convulsive movements" [19]. As mentioned, certain biological systems, such as ayeayes and woodpeckers [2], [3], [20]- [24] utilize active acoustic wave generation through tap testing in their foraging processes. Termites similarly use the same mechanism for vibroacoustic generation, albeit on a much smaller scale, raising several unanswered questions for engineers regarding how they can generate such high-intensity vibroacoustic waves in their communication and foraging processes. ...

Receiver bias and the acoustic ecology of aye-ayes (Daubentonia madagascariensis)

Communicative & Integrative Biology

... Mammals depend on audition for vital interactions with their environments, including food detection and attainment [43,44], hazard detection and evasion [45,46], stabilising balance [16], and participating in communal exchanges governing territoriality or mating [47,48]. The auditory system comprises two fluid-filled receptor organs that form the membranous labyrinth, which is suspended within the petrous temporal bone inside a network of bony voids known as the bony labyrinth [49]. ...

Social drive and the evolution of primate hearing

... Across species, an inverse correlation exists between body size and the fundamental frequency of vocalizations, with large African elephants (Loxodonta africana) known to produce calls with an f o as low as 16.8 Hz on one side of the spectrum [32] and small rainforest bats (Kerivoula pellucida) whose buzz calls can attain a maximum frequency of 250 kHz on the other side [33]. However, this assumption often does not hold when comparing individuals of the same species, sex and age, which can be attributed to the fact that surrounding bones do not constrain laryngeal structure growth. ...

Primate communication in the pure ultrasound

... The distinctiveness within the Neanderthal clade of Sima de los Huesos, Krapina, and late Neanderthal samples in our study corroborates that semicircular canal morphology represents a valuable proxy to analyze affinities in closely related taxa 62,64,65,87 and even species populations 61 . DMorph methods have revealed additional groupspecific differences beyond those previously identified based on linear measurement-67,88-90 and 3DGM landmark-based approaches 59,63,68 . ...

The mammalian bony labyrinth reconsidered, introducing a comprehensive geometric morphometric approach
  • Citing Article
  • March 2012

Journal of Anatomy