Marie-Laure Cléry-Melin’s research while affiliated with Paris Descartes University and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (7)


TABLE 1 . Reward-related processes: Vulnerability markers and endophenotype characteristics of MDD
TABLE 1 . Continued
Reward systems and cognitions in Major Depressive Disorder
  • Literature Review
  • Full-text available

November 2018

·

709 Reads

·

61 Citations

CNS spectrums

Marie-Laure Cléry-Melin

·

·

A lack of motivation and anhedonia represent frequent and pervasive symptoms in depression, although with poor specificity. Historically described as a response bias, reward-related impairments in depression may account for the important aspects of the cognitive impairments associated with diagnosis of major depressive disorder. Reward processing is a broad psychological construct that can be parsed into 3 distinct components known as “reinforcement learning” (learning), “reward responsiveness” (liking), and “motivation to obtain a reward” (wanting). Depressed patients respond hyposensitively to reward and maladaptively to punishment: this pattern is related to a dysfunction in the frontostriatal systems modulated by the monoamine systems; seems to be observed in medicated and unmedicated patients with depression and in healthy individuals with high levels of anhedonia; and could be observed in patients with a history of depression, even when in full remission. Considered to be cognitive impairments, reward-related-impairments may also constitute part of an underlying neurobiological vulnerability to major depressive disorder (MDD). For example, the reward-related impairment is state dependent and, more or less, correlated with symptom severity in some studies but has also been proposed as being trait like, with endophenotype characteristics, possibly contributing to the persistence of the disease or treatment resistance. The 3 core aspects of reward processing have specific neurobiological correlates that involve the ventral and dorsal striatum, lateral habenula, ventral tegmental area, orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and ventromedial and dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. These structures underline the important role of the dopaminergic mesolimbic pathway, but glutamate and serotonin could also have an important role, at least in some aspects of reward-related impairments.

Download

A simple attention test in the acute phase of a major depressive episode is predictive of later functional remission: C léry -M elin et al .

October 2016

·

61 Reads

·

21 Citations

Background: Functional recovery after a major depressive episode (MDE) requires both clinical remission and preservation of cognitive skills. As attentional deficit may persist after remission, leading to functional impairment, its role as a prognosis marker needs to be considered. Methods: Five hundred eight depressed outpatients (DSM-IV) were assessed at baseline for clinical symptoms (QIDS-SR), social functioning (Sheehan Disability Scale, SDS) and attention through the d2 test of attention and the trail making test, simple tests, respectively, requiring to quote or to interconnect relevant items. All patients were treated by agomelatine, and examined 6 to 8 weeks after baseline to assess clinical remission (QIDS-SR ≤ 5) and/or functional remission (SDS ≤ 6). Results: At follow up, 154 patients (31%) were in clinical and functional remission. Shorter cumulative duration of prior depression, shorter present MDE, and two parameters of the d2 test of attention were predictive of such positive outcome, the number of omission mistakes (F1) being the only one still significantly predictive (P < .05) with a multivariate approach. F1 was unchanged after remission, patients with less than 11 mistakes had a 2.27 times increased chance to reach full remission, and a dose-response relationship was observed, with a regular increase of positive outcome for less mistakes. Conclusions: The number of omission mistakes (F1) of the d2 test of attention was a stable marker, being predictive of, and with a dose-effect for, clinical plus functional remission. It may constitute a specific marker of attentional deficit, involved in the resilience process that enables individuals to develop more adequate strategies to cope with everyday functional activities.


Neural Mechanisms Underlying Motivation of Mental Versus Physical Effort

February 2012

·

380 Reads

·

308 Citations

·

·

Marie-Laure Cléry-Melin

·

[...]

·

Mental and physical efforts, such as paying attention and lifting weights, have been shown to involve different brain systems. These cognitive and motor systems, respectively, include cortical networks (prefronto-parietal and precentral regions) as well as subregions of the dorsal basal ganglia (caudate and putamen). Both systems appeared sensitive to incentive motivation: their activity increases when we work for higher rewards. Another brain system, including the ventral prefrontal cortex and the ventral basal ganglia, has been implicated in encoding expected rewards. How this motivational system drives the cognitive and motor systems remains poorly understood. More specifically, it is unclear whether cognitive and motor systems can be driven by a common motivational center or if they are driven by distinct, dedicated motivational modules. To address this issue, we used functional MRI to scan healthy participants while performing a task in which incentive motivation, cognitive, and motor demands were varied independently. We reasoned that a common motivational node should (1) represent the reward expected from effort exertion, (2) correlate with the performance attained, and (3) switch effective connectivity between cognitive and motor regions depending on task demand. The ventral striatum fulfilled all three criteria and therefore qualified as a common motivational node capable of driving both cognitive and motor regions of the dorsal striatum. Thus, we suggest that the interaction between a common motivational system and the different task-specific systems underpinning behavioral performance might occur within the basal ganglia.


Figure 2. Group-level results. Histograms show the effects of the main independent factors (incentive and arousal levels) on the main dependent variables (grip force, effort rating). Grip force is expressed as a percentage of the highest measure. Effort ratings were divided by the actual force produced on a trial-by-trial basis. Error bars are 6 inter-subjects standard errors of the mean. * Significant difference (paired t-test, P,0.05), between negative and neutral picture and between 0.01J and 1J in the task. NS = non significant. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023178.g002
Table 2 . Demographic and behavioral data.
Successive screenshots displayed in one trial are shown from left to right, with durations in ms. Neutral or arousing pictures (with positive or negative valence) were shown prior to physical effort exertion. Effort was cued by simultaneously showing the amount of money at stake, materialized as coin images (1 cent, 10 cents or 1 euro), and a graduated scale in which a cursor represented the force exerted on the handgrip. Subjects knew that the top of the scale corresponded to the monetary incentive, such that the more they squeezed the handgrip, the more money they would win. After force production, subjects rated the extent of their effort by positioning a cursor on an analog scale. The final screen informed subjects about the cumulative total of monetary earnings.
Histograms show the effects of the main independent factors (incentive and arousal levels) on the main dependent variables (grip force, effort rating). Grip force is expressed as a percentage of the highest measure. Effort ratings were divided by the actual force produced on a trial-by-trial basis. Error bars are ± inter-subjects standard errors of the mean. * Significant difference (paired t-test, P<0.05), between negative and neutral picture and between 0.01€ and 1€ in the task. NS = non significant.
Each point is a healthy subject (empty circle) or a depressed patient (filled circle). Graphs show difference scores for incentive (1€ - 0.01€) versus arousal (emotional - neutral) effects on force production and effort rating. Grip force and skin conductance is expressed as a percentage of the highest measure. Effort ratings were divided by the actual force produced on a trial-by-trial basis.
Why Don't You Try Harder? An Investigation of Effort Production in Major Depression

August 2011

·

229 Reads

·

160 Citations

Depression is mainly characterized as an emotional disorder, associated with reduced approach behavior. It remains unclear whether the difficulty in energising behavior relates to abnormal emotional states or to a flattened response to potential rewards, as suggested by several neuroimaging studies. Here, we aimed to demonstrate a specific incentive motivation deficit in major depression, independent of patients' emotional state. We employed a behavioral paradigm designed to measure physical effort in response to both emotional modulation and incentive motivation. Patients did exert more effort following emotionally arousing pictures (whether positive or negative) but not for higher monetary incentives, contrary to healthy controls. These results show that emotional and motivational sources of effort production are dissociable in pathological conditions. In addition, patients' ratings of perceived effort increased for high incentives, whereas controls' ratings were decreased. Thus, depressed patients objectively behave as if they do not want to gain larger rewards, but subjectively feel that they try harder. We suggest that incentive motivation impairment is a core deficit of major depression, which may render everyday tasks abnormally effortful for patients.


Separate Valuation Subsystems for Delay and Effort Decision Costs

October 2010

·

226 Reads

·

456 Citations

The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience

Decision making consists of choosing among available options on the basis of a valuation of their potential costs and benefits. Most theoretical models of decision making in behavioral economics, psychology, and computer science propose that the desirability of outcomes expected from alternative options can be quantified by utility functions. These utility functions allow a decision maker to assign subjective values to each option under consideration by weighting the likely benefits and costs resulting from an action and to select the one with the highest subjective value. Here, we used model-based neuroimaging to test whether the human brain uses separate valuation systems for rewards (erotic stimuli) associated with different types of costs, namely, delay and effort. We show that humans devalue rewards associated with physical effort in a strikingly similar fashion to those they devalue that are associated with delays, and that a single computational model derived from economics theory can account for the behavior observed in both delay discounting and effort discounting. However, our neuroimaging data reveal that the human brain uses distinct valuation subsystems for different types of costs, reflecting in opposite fashion delayed reward and future energetic expenses. The ventral striatum and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex represent the increasing subjective value of delayed rewards, whereas a distinct network, composed of the anterior cingulate cortex and the anterior insula, represent the decreasing value of the effortful option, coding the expected expense of energy. Together, these data demonstrate that the valuation processes underlying different types of costs can be fractionated at the cerebral level.


Figure 1. 
Figure 2. 
Figure 4. 
Get Aroused and Be Stronger: Emotional Facilitation of Physical Effort in the Human Brain

August 2009

·

185 Reads

·

109 Citations

The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience

Effort magnitude is commonly thought to reflect motivation, but little is known about the influence of emotional factors. Here, we manipulated the emotional state of subjects, via the presentation of pictures, before they exerted physical effort to win money. After highly arousing pictures, subjects produced more force and reported lower effort sensation, regardless of monetary incentives. Functional neuroimaging revealed that emotional arousal, as indexed by postscan ratings, specifically correlated with bilateral activity in the ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. We suggest that this region, by driving the motor cortex, constitutes a brain pathway that allows emotional arousal to facilitate physical effort.


Different valuation systems for delay versus effort discounting in the human brain

July 2009

·

128 Reads

·

1 Citation

NeuroImage

Decision making consists of choosing among available options on the basis of a valuation of their potential costs and benefits. Most theoretical models of decision making in behavioral economics, psychology, and computer science propose that the desirability of outcomes expected from alternative options can be quantified by utility functions. These utility functions allow a decision maker to assign subjective values to each option under consideration by weighting the likely benefits and costs resulting from an action and to select the one with the highest subjective value. Here, we used model-based neuroimaging to test whether the human brain uses separate valuation systems for rewards (erotic stimuli) associated with different types of costs, namely, delay and effort. We show that humans devalue rewards associated with physical effort in a strikingly similar fashion to those they devalue that are associated with delays, and that a single computational model derived from economics theory can account for the behavior observed in both delay discounting and effort discounting. However, our neuroimaging data reveal that the human brain uses distinct valuation subsystems for different types of costs, reflecting in opposite fashion delayed reward and future energetic expenses. The ventral striatum and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex represent the increasing subjective value of delayed rewards, whereas a distinct network, composed of the anterior cingulate cortex and the anterior insula, represent the decreasing value of the effortful option, coding the expected expense of energy. Together, these data demonstrate that the valuation processes underlying different types of costs can be fractionated at the cerebral level.

Citations (7)


... The former account considers depression to be due to impairments in reinforcement processing and the inability to exploit affective information to guide behavior (Eshel & Roiser, 2010). A number of studies suggested that depressed patients show dysfunctional responses to feedback (especially when it is negative) and indifference to rewards compared to the healthy controls (Cléry-Melin et al., 2019;Eshel & Roiser, 2010;Horne et al., 2021). This could explain why depressed individuals demonstrate diminished motivation in response to the given reward-the reward just does not feel so rewarding to them. ...

Reference:

“Sticky” Thinking Disrupts Decision Making for Individuals With a Tendency Toward Worry and Depression
Reward systems and cognitions in Major Depressive Disorder

CNS spectrums

... 42 In another study, however, agomelatine showed significant improvements in the d2 (d2 test of attention) and TMT-A/B test for MDD patients. 43 In yet another study with MDD outpatients, agomelatine displayed significant improvements in both the TMT-A and TMT-B tests. 44 In our results, agomelatine also showed significant improvements. ...

A simple attention test in the acute phase of a major depressive episode is predictive of later functional remission: C léry -M elin et al .
  • Citing Article
  • October 2016

... Research has converged upon a common set of brain regions that appear to track value during intertemporal choice tasks. These include the ventral striatum, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and medial prefrontal cortex (Kable and Glimcher, 2007;Gregorios-Pippas et al., 2009;Peters and Buchel, 2009;Pine et al., 2009;Prevost et al., 2009;Sripada et al., 2011). In addition, several studies have reported associations between brain function and intertemporal choice. ...

Different valuation systems for delay versus effort discounting in the human brain
  • Citing Article
  • July 2009

NeuroImage

... (A caveat is that the amygdala was significantly activated exclusively for mental-e8ort choice.) The view that cost evaluations rely on a unified neural population is also supported by both human (Schmidt, Lebreton, et al. 2012;Westbrook, Lamichhane, and Braver 2019) and animal (Borderies, Bornert, et al. 2020;Bornert and Bouret 2021) research. ...

Neural Mechanisms Underlying Motivation of Mental Versus Physical Effort

... MDD has been associated with decreased willingness to exert cognitive effort relative to comparison groups in some studies (Ang, Gelda, & Pizzagalli, 2023;Vinckier et al., 2022;Westbrook et al., 2022) though not in others (Barch et al., 2023;Tran, Hagen, Hollenstein, & Bowie, 2021). Willingness to exert physical effort has been found to be decreased in MDD relative to comparison groups in some studies (Berwian et al., 2020;Cléry-Melin et al., 2011;Treadway, Bossaller, Shelton, & Zald, 2012;Vinckier et al., 2022;Wang et al., 2022;Yang et al., 2014;Zou et al., 2020), though not in others (Cathomas et al., 2021;Sherdell, Waugh, & Gotlib, 2012;Tran et al., 2021;Wang et al., 2022;Yang et al., 2021). ...

Why Don't You Try Harder? An Investigation of Effort Production in Major Depression

... Likewise, if a reward is obtained only after completing an effortful task, the subjective value of the reward will be discounted (e.g., Nishiyama, 2014;Sugiwaka & Okouchi, 2004). This discounting phenomenon is robust to differences in commodities, species, and factors affecting the receipt of an outcome (e.g., McClure et al., 2004;Prévost et al., 2010;Vanderveldt et al., 2016) and is important in the study of decision making. ...

Separate Valuation Subsystems for Delay and Effort Decision Costs
  • Citing Article
  • October 2010

The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience

... The motor and cognitive performance tasks (i.e., grip and Stroop tasks, Fig. 2) were similar to those used in previous studies (Pessiglione et al., 2007;Schmidt et al., 2008Schmidt et al., , 2009Schmidt et al., , 2012Meyniel et al., 2013;Vinckier et al., 2022). Participants were told that their goal was to accumulate as much money as possible across trials. ...

Get Aroused and Be Stronger: Emotional Facilitation of Physical Effort in the Human Brain

The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience