Luke McEllin’s research while affiliated with University of Vienna and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (26)


Observed reaching speed signals stimulus value and informs foraging
  • Article
  • Full-text available

April 2025

·

11 Reads

Cognition

Luke McEllin

·

·

Günther Knoblich

·

Natalie Sebanz
Download

Flexible Social Learning of Technical Skills: The Case of Action Coordination

March 2024

·

8 Reads

A novel, interdisciplinary exploration of the relative contributions of rigidity and flexibility in the adoption, maintenance, and evolution of technical traditions. Techniques can either be used in rigid, stereotypical ways or in flexibly adaptive ways, or in some combination of the two. The Evolution of Techniques, edited by Mathieu Charbonneau, addresses the impacts of both flexibility and rigidity on how techniques are used, transformed, and reconstructed, at varying social and temporal scales. The multidisciplinary contributors demonstrate the important role of the varied learning contexts and social configurations involved in the transmission, use, and evolution of techniques. They explore the diversity of cognitive, behavioral, sociocultural, and ecological mechanisms that promote and constrain technical flexibility and rigidity, proposing a deeper picture of the enablers of, and obstacles to, technical transmission and change. In line with the extended evolutionary synthesis, the book proposes a more inclusive and materially grounded conception of technical evolution in terms of promiscuous, dynamic, and multidirectional causal processes. Offering new evidence and novel theoretical perspectives, the contributors deploy a diversity of methods, including ethnographies, field and laboratory experiments, cladistics and phylogenetic tree building, historiography, and philosophical analysis. Examples of the wide range of topics covered include field experiments with potters from five cultures, stability and change in Paleolithic toolmaking, why children lack flexibility when making tools, and cultural techniques in nonhuman animals. The volume's three thematic sections are: • Timescales of technical rigidity and flexibility • Rigid copying to flexible reconstruction • Exogenous factors of technical rigidity and flexibility The volume closes with a discussion by philosopher Kim Sterelny. Contributors Rita Astuti, Adam Howell Boyette, Blandine Bril, Josep Call, Mathieu Charbonneau, Arianna Curioni, Nicola Cutting, Bert De Munck, György Gergely, Anne-Lise Goujon, Ildikó Király, Catherine Lara, Sébastien Manem, Luke McEllin, Helena Miton, Giulio Ongaro, Sarah Pope-Caldwell, Valentine Roux, Manon Schweinfurth, Dan Sperber, Kim Sterelny, Dietrich Stout, James W. A. Strachan, Sadie Tenpas


Synchrony Influences Estimates of Cooperation in a Public-Goods Game

January 2024

·

40 Reads

·

5 Citations

Psychological Science

Benefiting from a cooperative interaction requires people to estimate how cooperatively other members of a group will act so that they can calibrate their own behavior accordingly. We investigated whether the synchrony of a group’s actions influences observers’ estimates of cooperation. Participants (recruited through Prolific) watched animations of actors deciding how much to donate in a public-goods game and using a mouse to drag donations to a public pot. Participants then estimated how much was in the pot in total (as an index of how cooperative they thought the group members were). Experiment 1 ( N = 136 adults) manipulated the synchrony between players’ decision-making time, and Experiment 2 ( N = 136 adults) manipulated the synchrony between players’ decision-implementing movements. For both experiments, estimates of how much was in the pot were higher for synchronous than asynchronous groups, demonstrating that the temporal dynamics of an interaction contain signals of a group’s level of cooperativity.


Depersonalization Affects Self-Prioritization of Bodily, but Not Abstract Self-Related Information

November 2023

·

307 Reads

·

2 Citations

Depersonalization is a common and distressing experience characterized by a feeling of estrangement from one’s self, body, and the world. In order to examine the relationship between depersonalization and selfhood we conducted an experimental study comparing processing of three types of self-related information between nonclinical groups of people experiencing high and low levels of depersonalization. Using a sequential matching task, we compared three types of biases for processing of self-related information: prioritization of one’s name, of self-associated abstract stimuli (geometrical shapes), and of self-associated bodily stimuli (avatar faces). We found that both groups demonstrated the standard pattern of results for self-prioritization of one’s name and geometrical shapes, but they differed with regard to avatar faces. While people with low depersonalization showed the standard prioritization of avatar faces, people with high depersonalization showed overall better response accuracy with avatar faces, and faster response times for stranger-associated avatar faces. These results were complemented by the additional finding that people with high depersonalization reported being more likely to use avatars of a different gender to their own outside of the experimental context. Finally, in this large sample (N = 180) we investigated the relationships between different measures of self-related processing and self-identification, finding no correlation between explicit reports of self-identification with self-associated avatar faces and geometrical shapes, self-prioritization of these stimuli, and prioritization of one’s name.



Flexible Cultural Learning Through Action Coordination

July 2023

·

28 Reads

·

13 Citations

Perspectives on Psychological Science

The cultural transmission of technical know-how has proven vital to the success of our species. The broad diversity of learning contexts and social configurations, as well as the various kinds of coordinated interactions they involve, speaks to our capacity to flexibly adapt to and succeed in transmitting vital knowledge in various learning contexts. Although often recognized by ethnographers, the flexibility of cultural learning has so far received little attention in terms of cognitive mechanisms. We argue that a key feature of the flexibility of cultural learning is that both the models and learners recruit cognitive mechanisms of action coordination to modulate their behavior contingently on the behavior of their partner, generating a process of mutual adaptation supporting the successful transmission of technical skills in diverse and fluctuating learning environments. We propose that the study of cultural learning would benefit from the experimental methods, results, and insights of joint-action research and, complementarily, that the field of joint-action research could expand its scope by integrating a learning and cultural dimension. Bringing these two fields of research together promises to enrich our understanding of cultural learning, its contextual flexibility, and joint action coordination.


Partner Choice in Action

November 2022

·

22 Reads

Prudently choosing who to interact with and who to avoid is an important ability to ensure that we benefit from a cooperative interaction. While the role of others' preferences, attributes, and values in partner choice have been established (Rossetti, Hilbe & Hauser, 2022), much less is known about whether the manner in which a potential partner plans and implements a decision provides helpful cues for partner choice. We used a partner choice paradigm in which participants chose who to partner with in economic games (either prisoners’ dilemma or stag-hunt). Before making their choice between two partners, participants were presented with information about the potential partners’ decision-related actions in another round of that same game. They received either information about the potential partners’ planning during decision making (Experiment 1, N = 144) or action execution during decision implementation (Experiment 2, N = 144). Across both games, participants preferred to interact with those whose action planning or action execution indicated they were making and implementing prosocial decisions (i.e., cooperate or stag) with certainty. This demonstrates that action cues present in either the planning or implementation of economic decisions influence partner choice, by revealing the strength of the social preferences of potential partners. We discuss implications of this finding for human decision-making and perception-action coupling in action understanding.


Sensorimotor communication fosters trust and generosity: The role of effort and signal utility

July 2022

·

32 Reads

·

9 Citations

Cognition

Previous research has shown that a co-actor's willingness to bear the monetary costs of prior cooperative activities influences our cooperative behaviour towards them. But what about when such information is lacking? In addition to monetary costs, people routinely engage in joint actions in which they incur effort costs in order to help each other achieve their goals, for example by adapting their goal-directed actions in order to send informative signals. We aimed to investigate whether people act more cooperatively towards those who are willing to bear the effort costs of an interaction by adapting their movements to send informative signals. We find that the effort that a co-actor invests in order to produce informative movement adaptations increases a participant's trust towards that co-actor, and that both the effort and utility of these informative movement adaptations increase generosity towards that co-actor. This demonstrates that we may base decisions about cooperation with a person on their willingness to bear the effort costs of interaction. These findings are discussed with respect to the role that effort investment plays in sustained cooperation.


Depersonalization affects self-prioritization of bodily, but not abstract self-related information

April 2022

·

283 Reads

·

1 Citation

Depersonalization is a common and distressing experience characterized by a feeling of estrangement from one’s self, body and the world. In order to examine the relationship between depersonalization and selfhood we conducted an experimental study comparing processing of three types of self-related information between non-clinical groups of people experiencing high and low levels of depersonalization. Using a sequential matching task, we compared three types of biases for processing of self-related information: prioritization of one’s name, of self-associated abstract stimuli (geometrical shapes), and of self-associated bodily stimuli (avatar faces). We found that both groups demonstrated the standard pattern of results for self-prioritization of one’s name and geometrical shapes, but they differed with regards to avatar faces. While people with low depersonalization showed the standard prioritization of avatar faces, people with high depersonalization showed overall better response accuracy with avatar faces, and faster response times for stranger-associated avatar faces. These results were complemented by the additional finding that people with high depersonalization reported being more likely to use avatars of a different gender to their own outside of the experimental context. Finally, in this large sample (N=180) we investigated the relationships between different measures of self-related processing and self-identification, finding no correlation between explicit reports of self-identification with self-associated avatar faces and geometrical shapes, self-prioritization of these stimuli, and prioritization of one’s name.


A Humanoid Robot’s Effortful Adaptation Boosts Partners’ Commitment to an Interactive Teaching Task

March 2022

·

31 Reads

·

6 Citations

ACM Transactions on Human-Robot Interaction

·

Henry Powell

·

·

[...]

·

We tested the hypothesis that, if a robot apparently invests effort in teaching a new skill to a human participant, the human participant will reciprocate by investing more effort in teaching the robot a new skill, too. To this end, we devised a scenario in which the iCub and a human participant alternated in teaching each other new skills. In the Adaptive condition of the robot teaching phase , the iCub slowed down its movements when repeating a demonstration for the human learner, whereas in the Unadaptive condition it sped the movements up when repeating the demonstration. In a subsequent participant teaching phase , human participants were asked to give the iCub a demonstration, and then to repeat it if the iCub had not understood. We predicted that in the Adaptive condition , participants would reciprocate the iCub’s adaptivity by investing more effort to slow down their movements and to increase segmentation when repeating their demonstration. The results showed that this was true when participants experienced the Adaptive condition after the Unadaptive condition and not when the order was inverted, indicating that participants were particularly sensitive to the changes in the iCub’s level of commitment over the course of the experiment.


Citations (21)


... These range from the synchronization of movements between co-actors, action predictions about what the partners are about to do, to higher-level mental state inference or 'mentalizing' others' proximal goals, distal intentions, and beliefs [41]. Recently, it has been demonstrated that synchrony during decision-making and movement phases enhances perceptions of cooperation within group activities [42]. ...

Reference:

Predictive strategies for the control of complex motor skills: recent insights into individual and joint actions
Synchrony Influences Estimates of Cooperation in a Public-Goods Game
  • Citing Article
  • January 2024

Psychological Science

... Importantly, how an individual executes an instrumental action is a rich source of information about both the individual's mental state and the environment, with specific parameters of an individual's goal directed actions 'reading-out' specific mental states associated with that action Becchio et al., 2012. For example the width of an individual's grasp allows observers to predict the object they are reaching for (Cavallo, Koul, Ansuini, Capozzi, & Becchio, 2016;Sacheli, Tidoni, Pavone, Aglioti, & Candidi, 2013), the level of exaggeration of an action tells observers whether or not it's intended to be communicative Vesper & Richardson, 2014), and the smoothness and directness in a movement reveals to observers an individual's level of confidence in a decision (McEllin, Fiedler, & Sebanz, 2023;Slepian, Young, Rutchick, & Ambady, 2013). This is particularly useful for social learning as it allows observers to pick up on numerous mental states that may be informative about the individual's actions, the environment, and about the interaction as a whole (Charbonneau, Curioni, McEllin, & Strachan, 2024;. ...

Action planning and execution cues influence economic partner choice
  • Citing Article
  • October 2023

Cognition

... For example, they are more likely to experience the rubber hand illusion (see also Braithwaite et al., 2017) and mirror-pain synaesthesia (Bowling et al., 2019). Previous quantitative and qualitative studies also report that DP is related to differences in self/other distinction at the level of body perception (Adler et al., 2016;Bowling et al., 2019;Ciaunica, McEllin, et al., 2022;Farmer et al., 2021;Woźniak et al., 2023). Crucially, previous works illustrate that multisensory integration of bodily signals plays a key role in structuring our basic sense of self, i.e., the subjective experience of an "I" or "self," bound to my body and distinct from the world and others (Blanke & Metzinger, 2009;Gallagher, 2000;Gallese & Sinigaglia, 2010Park & Blanke, 2019;Qin et al., 2020;Tsakiris, 2017). ...

Depersonalization Affects Self-Prioritization of Bodily, but Not Abstract Self-Related Information

... Moreover, cultural flexibility influences cognitive processes, affecting how individuals perceive and interpret their surroundings (Charbonneau et al., 2024). Those who exhibit cultural flexibility demonstrate greater cognitive adaptability, allowing them to shift their thinking patterns based on new information and experiences. ...

Flexible Cultural Learning Through Action Coordination
  • Citing Article
  • July 2023

Perspectives on Psychological Science

... In fact, preliminary data from our ongoing studies suggest that people with high-levels of depersonalisation experiences will show a modulation of the magnitude of self-prioritization of self-associated bodily (avatar faces) versus abstract stimuli (geometrical shapes) in the sequential matching task (Wozniak, McEllin, Hohwy, & Ciaunica, 2022). For example, several studies demonstrate that selfrelated stimuli (e.g., one's face or name) are processed faster and more accurately than others' names and faces (Alexopoulos, Muller, Ric, & Marendaz, 2012;Bortolon & Raffard, 2018;Wozńiak and Hohwy, 2020). ...

Depersonalization affects self-prioritization of bodily, but not abstract self-related information

... Results showed that participants adopted, to some extent, the intentional stance towards iCub. This finding led to further exploration of the factors that may influence the adoption of the intentional stance, such as expectations and trust Vinanzi et al., 2021), behavioral variability and adaptiveness Vignolo et al., 2022), length of the interaction (Abubshait & Wykowska, 2020), or human likeness in behavior (Bryant et al., 2020;Marchesi et al., 2021Marchesi et al., , 2022. Human likeness of behavior is quite a critical hint for humans to attribute human traits to artifacts . ...

A Humanoid Robot’s Effortful Adaptation Boosts Partners’ Commitment to an Interactive Teaching Task
  • Citing Article
  • March 2022

ACM Transactions on Human-Robot Interaction

... Further, although most events of cultural transmission rely in some way on some psychological mechanisms, these mechanisms are most likely very different for the transmission of different cultural items (Charbonneau et al. 2022). Indeed, there is little similarity in the cognitive mechanisms involved between building a house, maintaining a fire, tracking prey, abiding by social norms, recalling a story, solving a linear equation, and speaking some language. ...

Flexible cultural learning through action coordination

... Depersonalization has a significant impact on the individual's emotional and social quality of life. In fact, it is associated with parallel manifestations of anxiety, depression, or excessive burnout (Ciaunica et al., 2022;Santiago-Torner, 2024;Silistraru et al., 2024). For this reason, the second objective of this study was to assess whether depersonalization is a sufficiently important factor to hinder the possible relationship between a benevolent ethical climate and affective commitment mediated by intrinsic motivation. ...

Zoomed out: digital media use and depersonalization experiences during the COVID-19 lockdown

... Generalizing this path minimization, by weighting target types based on stable cooperative or competitive tendencies in each dyad, we successfully characterized the large part of emergent strategies. Extending our model by recent interaction history and invite placements (sensorimotor communication) [70,[75][76][77][78] across collection cycles further enhanced prediction accuracy. Due to spatial continuity between cycles, a non-engaged participant can signal their intention to cooperate by inviting the partner to select a specific joint target. ...

Sensorimotor communication fosters trust and generosity: The role of effort and signal utility
  • Citing Article
  • July 2022

Cognition

... First of all, we know that the sense of commitment is elicited by coordination (Michael et al. 2016a), by the perception of effort (Székely and Michael 2018) even in cases of human-robot interaction ) and by repetition (Bonalumi et al. 2019(Bonalumi et al. , 2022. We know how these three factors interact, and that they essentially boil down to mechanisms of effort perception (McEllin et al. 2023), and that therefore commitments are detectable by probing effort. We also know about the failures of the sense of commitment, namely that it is impaired by Borderline Personality Disorder (Ooi et al. 2018). ...

EXPRESS: The Fruits of our Labour: Interpersonal coordination generates commitment by signaling a willingness to adapt
  • Citing Article
  • January 2022

Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006)