Larry L. Jacoby’s research while affiliated with Washington University in St. Louis and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (172)


Blurring past and present: Using false memory to better understand false hearing in young and older adults
  • Article

July 2020

·

61 Reads

·

14 Citations

Memory & Cognition

·

·

Larry L. Jacoby

A number of recent studies have shown that older adults are more susceptible to context-based misperceptions in hearing (Rogers, Jacoby, & Sommers, Psychology and Aging, 27, 33–45, 2012; Sommers, Morton, & Rogers, Remembering: Attributions, Processes, and Control in Human Memory [Essays in Honor of Larry Jacoby], pp. 269–284, 2015) than are young adults. One explanation for these age-related increases in what we term false hearing is that older adults are less able than young individuals to inhibit a prepotent response favored by context. A similar explanation has been proposed for demonstrations of age-related increases in false memory (Jacoby, Bishara, Hessels, & Toth, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134, 131–148, 2005). The present study was designed to compare susceptibility to false hearing and false memory in a group of young and older adults. In Experiment 1, we replicated the findings of past studies demonstrating increased frequency of false hearing in older, relative to young, adults. In Experiment 2, we demonstrated older adults’ increased susceptibility to false memory in the same sample. Importantly, we found that participants who were more prone to false hearing also tended to be more prone to false memory, supporting the idea that the two phenomena share a common mechanism. The results are discussed within the framework of a capture model, which differentiates between context-based responding resulting from failures of cognitive control and context-based guessing.


Cognitive control constrains memory attributions

January 2019

·

36 Reads

·

1 Citation

Behavioral and Brain Sciences

Cognitive control constrains retrieval processing and so restricts what comes to mind as input to the attribution system. We review evidence that older adults, patients with Alzheimer's disease, and people with traumatic brain injury exert less cognitive control during retrieval, and so are susceptible to memory misattributions in the form of dramatic levels of false remembering.


The Importance of Time to Think Back: The Role of Reminding in Retroactive Effects of Memory
  • Article
  • Publisher preview available

August 2018

·

136 Reads

·

14 Citations

Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition

Change has been described as detrimental for later memory for the original event in research on retroactive interference. Popular accounts of retroactive interference treat learning as the formation of simple associations and explain interference as due to response competition, perhaps along with unlearning or inhibition of the original response. By such accounts, providing additional study time for a changed response in a classic A–B, A–D learning paradigm should increase retroactive interference. In contrast, our experiments show that changing a response produces retroactive facilitation rather than retroactive interference but that outcome requires that the change be detected in the form of a reminding. When reminding does not occur, retroactive interference is observed. Increasing time to study the changed response increases the likelihood of being reminded. Accounts in terms of simple associations cannot explain the importance of reminding. We do so by assuming that being reminded results in a recursive representation that includes both the original and changed response along with the order in which they occurred. We discuss the importance of our results for application as well as for theory.

View access options

Multinomial models reveal deficits of two distinct controlled retrieval processes in aging and very mild Alzheimer disease

May 2018

·

135 Reads

·

7 Citations

Memory & Cognition

Dual-process models of episodic retrieval reveal consistent deficits of controlled recollection in aging and Alzheimer disease (AD). In contrast, automatic familiarity is relatively spared. We extend standard dual-process models by showing the importance of a third capture process. Capture produces a failure to attempt recollection, which might reflect a distinct error from an inability to recollect when attempted (Jacoby et al. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(2), 131–148, 2005a). We used multinomial process tree (MPT) modeling to estimate controlled recollection and capture processes, as well as automatic retrieval processes, in a large group of middle-aged to older adults who were cognitively normal (N = 519) or diagnosed with the earliest detectable stage of AD (N = 107). Participants incidentally encoded word pairs (e.g., knee bone). At retrieval, participants completed cued word fragments (e.g., knee b_n_) with primes that were congruent (e.g., bone), incongruent (e.g., bend), or neutral (i.e., &&&) to the target (e.g., bone). MPT models estimated retrieval processes both at the group and the individual levels. A capture parameter was necessary to fit MPT models to the observed data, suggesting that dual-process models of this task can be contaminated by a capture process. In both group- and individual-level analyses, aging and very mild AD were associated with increased susceptibility to capture, decreased recollection, and no differences in automatic influences. These results suggest that it is important to consider two distinct modes of attentional control when modeling retrieval processes. Both forms of control (recollection and avoiding capture) are particularly sensitive to cognitive decline in aging and early-stage AD.


Context Specificity of Automatic Influences of Memory

February 2018

·

66 Reads

·

17 Citations

Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition

It has often been shown that intentional recollection is influenced by context manipulations, such as context reinstatement (e.g., Smith, 2013; Smith & Vela, 2001), but whether or not automatic retrieval (e.g., Jacoby, 1991) is likewise context dependent remains an open question. Here, we present two experiments that examined effects of context manipulations on indirect measures of memory. The first experiment tested anagram completion, and the second experiment used word fragment completion to test effects of context reinstatement; both experiments found reinstatement effects. To address potential problems of explicit contamination, we also asked participants if they were aware of the priming manipulations. Separating participants according to their test awareness showed effects of context manipulations for both aware and unaware participants. A greater effect size was found for aware participants only in Experiment 1, in which participants had enough time on each test trial for recollection to be used. We conclude that context reinstatement does affect automatic retrieval.


Prior experience shapes metacognitive judgments at the category level: the role of testing and category difficulty

July 2015

·

185 Reads

·

12 Citations

Metacognition and Learning

Most metacognition research has focused on aggregate judgments of overall performance or item-level judgments about performance on particular questions. However, metacognitive judgments at the category level, which have not been as extensively explored, also play a role in students’ study strategies, for example, when students determine what topics to study for an exam. We investigated whether category learning judgments (CLJs) were sensitive to differences in the difficulty of general knowledge categories. After either studying or being tested on facts from several categories (e.g., Shakespeare, Astronomy), participants estimated the likelihood that they could correctly answer new questions from those categories on a later test (i.e., they made CLJs). Results of two studies showed that CLJs were sensitive to differences in category difficulty. Further, participants gave lower or more conservative CLJs when they took an initial test as compared to studying questions from the categories. Results are discussed in terms of the value and relevance of CLJs both in educational settings and in theories of metacognition.


Memory Consequences of Looking Back to Notice Change: Retroactive and Proactive Facilitation

May 2015

·

1,025 Reads

·

62 Citations

Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition

Three experiments contrasted recollection of change with differentiation as means of avoiding retroactive interference and proactive interference. We manipulated the extent to which participants looked back to notice change between pairs of cues and targets (A-B, A-D) and measured the effects on later cued recall of either the first or second response. Two lists of word pairs were presented. Some right-hand members of pairs were changed within List 2, whereas others were changed between lists. Participants in a Within-List Back condition were instructed to detect changes that occurred only during List 2, in an effort to reduce noticing changes in pairs between lists while simultaneously differentiating the 2 lists. In contrast, participants in an N-Back condition were instructed to detect both within-list and between-list changes. Recall of first list responses that changed between lists produced retroactive facilitation for the N-Back condition but not for the Within-List Back condition. Similarly, recall of second list responses that changed between lists produced proactive facilitation for the N-Back condition but not for the Within-List Back condition. The greater extent of looking back increased detection of change and later recollection of change, which produced facilitation. When change was not recollected, detected change produced proactive interference. The recursive reminding produced when change is noticed contrasts with the simple associations of classic interference theory, and memory performance when change is recollected contrasts with the predictions of interference theory. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2015 APA, all rights reserved).


FIGuRe 1. examples of face pictures used in all experiments. left column: original face. middle column: high-similarity lure. right column: low-similarity lure 
table 1 . mean (Sd) performance, experiment 1 
table 2 . mean (Sd) Within-participant confidence-accuracy Gamma correlations, experiments 1-3 
FIGuRe 2. Hit rate and false alarm rate comparing yes-no recognition with 2-alternative forced-choice (2afc) recognition, as a function of lure similarity and target presence, experiment 3. data are full report (all responses). error bars represent the pooled Se for the betweensubject comparison of yes-no and 2afc
FIGuRe 3. receiver operating characteristics averaged across participants, comparing yes-no recognition with 2-alternative forced-choice (2afc) recognition, as a function of lure similarity to target (low vs. high) and target presence (present vs. absent), experiment 3. data are full report (all responses). italicized numbers are the mean area under the curve 

+5

Simultaneous Versus Sequential Presentation in Testing Recognition Memory for Faces

February 2015

·

539 Reads

·

9 Citations

The American Journal of Psychology

·

·

·

[...]

·

Larry L. Jacoby

Three experiments examined the issue of whether faces could be better recognized in a simultaneous test format (2- alternative forced choice [2afc]) or a sequential test format (yes-no). all experiments showed that when target faces were present in the test, the simultaneous procedure led to superior performance (area under the roc curve), whether lures were high or low in similarity to the targets. However, when a target- absent condition was used in which no lures resembled the targets but the lures were similar to each other, the simultaneous procedure yielded higher false alarm rates (experiments 2 and 3) and worse overall performance (experiment 3). this pattern persisted even when we excluded responses that participants opted to withhold rather than volunteer. We conclude that for the basic recognition procedures used in these experiments, simultaneous presentation of alternatives (2afc) generally leads to better discriminability than does sequential presentation (yes-no) when a target is among the alternatives. However, our results also show that the opposite can occur when there is no target among the alternatives. an important future step is to see whether these patterns extend to more realistic eyewitness lineup procedures. the pictures used in the experiment are available online at http://www.press.uillinois.edu/journals/ajp/media/testing_recognition/.


The Power of Examples: Illustrative Examples Enhance Conceptual Learning of Declarative Concepts

June 2014

·

830 Reads

·

84 Citations

Educational Psychology Review

Declarative concepts (i.e., key terms with short definitions of the abstract concepts denoted by those terms) are a common kind of information that students are expected to learn in many domains. A common pedagogical approach for supporting learning of declarative concepts involves presenting students with concrete examples that illustrate how the abstract concepts can be instantiated in real-world situations. However, minimal prior research has examined whether illustrative examples actually enhance declarative concept learning, and the available outcomes provide weak evidence at best. In the three experiments reported here, students studied definitions of declarative concepts followed either by illustrative examples of those concepts or by additional study of the definitions. On a subsequent classification test in which learners were presented with examples and were asked to identify which concept the example illustrated, performance was greater for students who had studied illustrative examples during learning than for students who only studied definitions (ds from 0.74 to 1.67). However, the effects of illustrative examples on declarative concept learning depended in part on the conditions under which those examples were presented. Although performance was similar when examples were presented after versus before concept definitions (Experiments 1a–1b), classification accuracy depended on the extent to which examples of different concepts were interleaved and whether definitions were presented along with the examples (Experiment 2).


Memory for flip-flopping: Detection and recollection of political contradictions

May 2014

·

178 Reads

·

39 Citations

Memory & Cognition

During political campaigns, candidates often change their positions on controversial issues. Does changing positions create confusion and impair memory for a politician's current position? In 3 experiments, two political candidates held positions on controversial issues in two debates. Across the debates, their positions were repeated, changed, or held only in the second debate (control). Relative to the control condition, recall of the most recent position on issues was enhanced when change was detected and recollected, whereas recall was impaired when change was not recollected. Furthermore, examining the errors revealed that subjects were more likely to intrude a Debate 1 response than to recall a blend of the two positions, and that recollecting change decreased Debate 1 intrusions. We argue that detecting change produces a recursive representation that embeds the original position in memory along with the more recent position. Recollecting change then enhances memory for the politician's positions and their order of occurrence by accessing the recursive trace.


Citations (95)


... This resulted in a null decadevalue-frame-size congruity effect for unit-decade incompatible pairs. A possible reason for not considering the placevalues of the numbers under these circumstances may be an "overload" of information which caused the participants to neglect the place-value information (e.g., Jacoby et al., 1999;Ratcliff, 1980;Schwarz, 1992Schwarz, , 1994Smith, 2000). This finding of a null decade-value-frame-size congruity effect is consistent with García-Orza et al. (2017), which demonstrated that the inclusion of more complex stimuli in the physical comparison task (i.e., presenting four-digit numbers comprised of four nonzero numerals, compared with four-digit numbers comprised of one nonzero numeral and three zeros) resulted in a weaker size congruity effect. ...

Reference:

Place-value and physical size converge in automatic processing of multi-digit numbers
Automatic Influences as Accessibility Bias in Memory and Stroop Tasks: Toward a Formal Model
  • Citing Chapter
  • April 1999

... However, recent research on moral inclinations using the psychometric technique of process dissociation (Jacoby, 1991;Kelley and Jacoby, 2000;Yonelinas, 2002;Payne and Bishara, 2009) has demonstrated that utilitarian and deontological response inclinations are not necessarily negatively correlated (Conway and Gawronski, 2013;Conway et al., 2018;Fleischmann et al., 2019). ...

Recollection and Familiarity: Process-Dissociation
  • Citing Chapter
  • May 2000

... Consistent with the source-monitoring framework, the results suggested earlier accessibility of item information than source information. In subsequent years, more rigorous tests on Johnson et al.'s (1994) data (Kinjo, 1998;McElree et al., 1999) raised concerns regarding the conclusiveness of the original findings. Following that, Kinjo (1998, Experiment 1) conducted a stronger test in a modified procedure with more response-lag conditions and still observed that item memory was accessed before source memory. ...

Isolating the Contributions of Familiarity and Source Information to Item Recognition: A Time Course Analysis

Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition

... Age differences in false remembering have also been investigated by studies using sentential materials, as opposed to word lists (e.g., Failes et al., 2020;Gunter et al., 1992Gunter et al., , 1995Hartman & Hasher, 1991;Matzen & Benjamin, 2013), even though these studies are far less consistent in their results, compared to previous DRM studies. ...

Blurring past and present: Using false memory to better understand false hearing in young and older adults
  • Citing Article
  • July 2020

Memory & Cognition

... Cognitive control refers to flexible allocations of mental resources and coordination of information processes to reduce environmental uncertainty (Fan, 2014;Posner & Snyder, 1975). As a core component of information processing, cognitive control is essential for our daily cognitive activities, including perception and attention (Mackie, Van Dam, & Fan, 2013;Whitehead, Ooi, Egner, & Woldorff, 2019), memory (Boag, Strickland, Heathcote, Neal, & Loft, 2019;Kelley & Jacoby, 2020;Koslov, Mukerji, Hedgpeth, & Lewis-Peacock, 2019) and learning (Abrahamse, Braem, Notebaert, & Verguts, 2016;Sakreida et al., 2018), language and speech (Bourguignon, 2014;Grundy, Anderson, & Bialystok, 2017;Tao, Wang, Zhu, & Cai, 2021), and decision-making (Robertson, Hiebert, Seergobin, Owen, & MacDonald, 2015;Waskom, Frank, & Wagner, 2017). Recently, a protocol has been developed for estimating the capacity of cognitive control (CCC) that represents the maximal amount of information processed per unit of time (in bits per second, bps) with arbitrarily low error rate. ...

Cognitive control constrains memory attributions
  • Citing Article
  • January 2019

Behavioral and Brain Sciences

... The present study examined the role of interpolated retrieval in the retroactive enhancement of existing memories and subsequent cross-episode integrative encoding using A-B, A-C tasks (for review, see Anderson and Neely 1996). This approach is ideal because the stimuli include shared and unique features that allow for assessment of accessibility and interdependence in responses across phases Jacoby et al. 2015;Negley et al. 2018;Garlitch and Wahlheim 2020). In these tasks, participants study two lists of cue-response pairs with varying rela-tionships among elements between lists. ...

The Importance of Time to Think Back: The Role of Reminding in Retroactive Effects of Memory

Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition

... Issues like copying others' assignments, illegal activities such as selling fake products or viewing pornographic materials, cyberbullying, the use of the internet to cheat others and the use of the internet to do illegal gambling and scamming has become very rampant (Lambert et al., 2003). There is a tendency that the moral conduct of these youngsters are corrupted by content they view online (Verhoef & Coetser, 2021). ...

Stereotypes as Dominant Responses: On the "Social Facilitation" of Prejudice in Anticipated Public Contexts

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

... Unfortunately, none of the studies invoking an early-phase elevated attention hypothesis have included older adults. Millar, Balota, Bishara, and Jacoby (2018) have more recently reported a study using multinomial modeling and indeed provided evidence of a capture process within a process dissociation paradigm above and beyond the standard estimates of recollection and familiarity. Importantly, older adults and individuals with earlystage Alzheimer's disease were more susceptible to the capture process compared to younger adults. ...

Multinomial models reveal deficits of two distinct controlled retrieval processes in aging and very mild Alzheimer disease

Memory & Cognition

... Concretely, right frontal-polar regions are specifically activated in participants who remember a word from a previously learned list by the presentation of a related word stem cue 67 . In this line, Buckner 65,68 has proposed that the right frontal-polar cortex may be involved in sustained processes, in a type of attentional set or task mode involving multiple individual retrieval events 65,69,70 . This was the case with our present protocol as the TNT phase included a single block of 240 consecutive trials. ...

Functional–Anatomic Correlates of Sustained and Transient Processing Components Engaged during Controlled Retrieval
  • Citing Article
  • September 2003

The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience

... Reinstating the word encoding context during the memory test, relative to testing with nonreinstated context, considerably increases participants' performance [19]. This is also supported by the results of a neuroimaging evidence, showing that neural reinstatement of encoding patterns only improves memory when the retrieval and encoding context match, whereas neural reinstatement is detrimental when these contexts differ [20]. ...

Context Specificity of Automatic Influences of Memory

Journal of Experimental Psychology Learning Memory and Cognition