February 2021
·
14 Reads
·
1 Citation
Psychogeriatrics
This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.
February 2021
·
14 Reads
·
1 Citation
Psychogeriatrics
October 2017
·
669 Reads
·
15 Citations
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience
Background: In this study, we investigated the relationship among a history of depression, depressive states, and dementia in a community-based old-old cohort. Methods: From 2012 to 2013, we recruited 200 subjects residing in Tome, Japan. Ultimately, 181 subjects were enrolled in our study and completed the whole study protocol. We used the World Mental Health-Composite International Diagnostic Interview 3.0 to evaluate whether subjects had a history of depression or other affective disorders. Simultaneously, 3.0 Tesla brain magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) was performed for each subject. Results: Of 181 subjects, 66 were normal (clinical dementia rating [CDR] = 0), 88 had MCI (CDR = 0.5), and 27 had dementia (CDR = 1 or above). Nine of the 181 subjects (4.9%) had a history of depressive episodes. CDR was significantly higher in subjects with a history of depression (0.9 vs. 0.4, p = 0.046) than in those without it. Seventy-two of the 181 subjects (39.7%) exhibited depressive symptoms. Subjects with depression exhibited lower Mini–Mental State Examination scores (21.6 vs. 23.3, p = 0.008), higher CDR scores (0.6 vs. 0.3, p = 0.004), and more atrophy of the medial temporal lobe (4.4 vs. 3.7, p = 0.036). Conclusion: A history of depression should be considered a risk factor for all-cause dementia. In the old-old population, depression is associated with a higher prevalence of dementia, lower cognitive performance, and a smaller hippocampus.
January 2016
·
20 Reads
·
15 Citations
Psychogeriatrics
Aim: After the Great East Japan Earthquake of 2011, we investigated the safety of residents in the affected communities. Most of the people requiring help were elderly and had previously been assessed as Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) 0.5 (i.e. as having mild cognitive impairment (MCI)). We examined how well they understood the television news and whether they could make appropriate decisions. Methods: This community-based study of dementia and difficulties following a disaster started in Tome, northern Japan. The subjects were 188 randomly selected older residents who underwent CDR, blood tests, magnetic resonance imaging, and cognitive tests, including an original visual risk cognition task. They were shown NHK news broadcasts from the day of the earthquake to determine whether they could understand the content. Results: Neither the CDR 0 (healthy) nor the CDR 0.5 (MCI) subjects fully understood the television news. Some subjects did not recognize the danger of aftershocks and engaged in risky behaviour. CDR 0.5 subjects who exhibited such behaviour scored lower on the visual risk cognition task. Conclusions: It is noteworthy that television news is difficult to understand, even for healthy older adults. We found that MCI subjects had particular difficulties due to the disaster and suggest that risk cognition could be evaluated using visually presented materials.
January 2016
·
30 Reads
·
2 Citations
Psychogeriatrics
Background: The Necker cube is usually used for evaluating the visuoconstructional ability of patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and dementia. However, the Necker cube is often considered a drawing with a visual illusionary perspective. The purpose of this study was to investigate whether Necker cube copying could detect participants with MCI due to dementia. Methods: We retrospectively analyzed the database of the 1998 prevalence study that was part of the Tajiri Project (n = 599). Pencil drawings of the Necker cube on A4-sized white paper by non-demented people (Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) 0 and 0.5, n = 256) were classified into two patterns: non-three-dimension (3-D) and 3-D. Two neuropsychologists assessed Necker cube copying according to the criteria of the classification. After the classification, the database of the 2003 incidence study was used according to the subjects' conversion to dementia. Results: In the prevalence study, among those who made a non-3-D drawing of the Necker cube, there were significantly fewer people in the CDR 0 group than in the CDR 0.5 and CDR 1+ groups; similarly, there were significantly fewer people in the CDR 0.5 group than in the CDR 1 + group (χ(2) = 32.6, P < 0.001; post-hoc tests using χ(2) tests, CDR 0 > CDR 0.5 > CDR 1+, P < 0.001). In the incidence study, among those who made a non-3-D drawing of the Necker cube, there were significantly fewer people in the non-converter group than in the converter group (χ(2) = 19.9, P < 0.001). However, there was no significant difference between the non-converter group (n = 21) and the converter group (n = 21) when age, sex, educational levels, and Mini-Mental State Examination scores were controlled (χ(2) = 0.0, P = 1.000). Conclusions: Our results suggested that Necker cube copying may evaluate visual illusion as well as visuoconstructional ability. The Necker cube may not be an appropriate test to detect participants with MCI due to dementia.
January 2016
·
29 Reads
·
9 Citations
Journal of Clinical Neuroscience
November 2015
·
100 Reads
·
2 Citations
Background: Language impairment and behavioral symptoms are both common phenomena in dementia patients. In this study, we investigated the behavioral symptoms in dementia patients with different language backgrounds. Through this, we aimed to propose a possible connection between language and delusion. Methods: We recruited 21 patients with Alzheimer's disease (AD), according to the DSM-IV and NINCDS-ADRDA criteria, from the memory clinic of the Cardinal Tien Hospital in Taipei, Taiwan. They were classified into two groups: 11 multilinguals who could speak Japanese, Taiwanese and Mandarin Chinese, and 10 bilinguals who only spoke Taiwanese and Mandarin Chinese. There were no differences between age, education, disease duration, disease severity, environment and medical care between these two groups. Comprehensive neuropsychological examinations, including Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR), Mini-Mental Status Examination (MMSE), Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument (CASI), Verbal fluency, Chinese version of the Boston naming test (BNT) and the Behavioral Pathology in Alzheimer's Disease Rating Scale (BEHAVE-AD), were administered. Results: The multilingual group showed worse results on the Boston naming test. Other neuropsychological tests, including the MMSE, CASI and Verbal fluency, were not significantly different. More delusions were noted in the multilingual group. Three pairs of subjects were identified for further examination of their differences. These three cases presented the typical scenario of how language misunderstanding may cause delusions in multilingual dementia patients. Consequently, more emotion and distorted ideas may be induced in the multilinguals compared with the MMSE-matched controls. Conclusion: Inappropriate mixing of language or conflict between cognition and emotion may cause more delusions in these multilingual patients. This reminds us that delusion is not a pure biological outcome of brain degeneration. Although the cognitive performance was not significantly different between our groups, language may still affect their delusion.
November 2015
·
120 Reads
·
4 Citations
BMC Neurology
Background We previously demonstrated a positive correlation with nursing home (NH) replacement and donepezil (DNP) administration on lifetime expectancy after the onset of Alzheimer’s disease (AD). However, the correlation with quality-adjusted life-year (QALY) remains to be elucidated, along with the additional impact of concomitant cerebrovascular disease (CVD). Based upon our recently reported health state utility values, we retrospectively analyzed the correlation with NH replacement and/or DNP administration on QALY and life expectancy in ‘pure’ AD (without CVD) and AD with CVD patients. Methods All outpatients at the Tajiri Clinic from 1999–2012 with available medical records and death certificates were included. The entry criteria were a dementia diagnosis (DSM-IV) and diagnoses of pure AD or AD with CVD (NINCDS-ADRDA), medical treatment for more than 3 months, and follow up to less than 1 year before death. The main outcomes were lifetime expectancy (months between the onset of dementia and death) and QALY. Results We identified 390 subjects, of whom 275 had the diagnosis of dementia that met the entry criteria, including 67 pure AD, 33 AD with CVD, and 110 VaD patients. For the AD patients, 52 had taken DNP and 48 had not received the drug due to treatment prior to the introduction of DNP in 1999 in Japan. For the pure AD group, there were positive correlation between NH and DNP and QALY, as well as lifetime expectancy. As for the AD with CVD group, only a correlation between DNP and lifetime expectancy was noted, with no correlation with QALY. Conclusions We found positive correlations between DNP administration and NH replacement and lifetime expectancy and QALY after the onset of AD. However, concomitant CVD negated such a positive correlation with QALY. The findings suggest that QALY in AD is affected by CVD; thus, indicating the importance of CVD prevention.
July 2015
·
6 Reads
July 2015
·
25 Reads
June 2015
·
76 Reads
·
3 Citations
Psychogeriatrics
Dubois et al. proposed the criteria for prodromal Alzheimer's disease (AD) to detect dementia in its very early stage. Because detection requires magnetic resonance imaging and (18) F-fluorodeoxyglucose-positron emission tomography (PET), the prevalence and prognosis have not been fully investigated. Our database included 346 healthy participants (Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR) 0), 119 with questionable dementia (CDR 0.5), and 32 dementia participants (CDR 1+) and was applied to investigate the prevalence of prodromal AD. Forty-four CDR 0.5 participants (37%) were randomly selected to undergo (18) F-fluorodeoxyglucose-PET. The same percentage was applied to select 128 CDR 0 and 12 CDR 1 + participants (total: n = 184) to calculate the prevalence. A neuroradiologist classified the PET images in a blinded manner based on the criteria of Silverman et al. Participants were considered to have prodromal AD if they exhibited 'parietal/temporal +/- frontal hypometabolism' (PET) with hippocampal atrophy (magnetic resonance imaging). Eighteen CDR 0.5 participants (40.9%) met the criteria for prodromal AD, which was a prevalence rate of 9.8% among older adults aged ≥65 years. Thirteen prodromal AD participants (72%) converted to AD during the 5-year follow-up period. The concept and criteria for prodromal AD are useful for predicting which subjects in a community will convert to AD. © 2015 The Authors. Psychogeriatrics © 2015 Japanese Psychogeriatric Society.
... Third, the sleep-wake cycle is correlated with the clearance of brain amyloid-β protein [20], while longer duration in the recumbent position possibly increases the length of time spent with high intracranial pressure and may subsequently alter the cerebrospinal/interstitial fluid dynamics and reduce β-amyloid clearance during sleep [21], which has been related to the pathophysiology of cognitive dysfunction [22]. Fourth, depression has been shown to be associated with poor sleep, including sleep periods [23], and depression is related to lower cognitive performance in the elderly population [24]. In addition, most older adults with impaired cognitive function have indicated changes in sleep patterns, and cognitive decline is a lengthy process. ...
October 2017
Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience
... In our study [10], we compared semantic dementia (SD) with AD in deficits of semantic memory. We hypothesized that impaired semantic access with preserved semantic memory would be reflected in successful performance on those tasks without a time restriction but impaired performance with a time restriction. ...
March 2014
... For the CC item, Oonuma et al [54] reported that 31.1% of individuals with normal cognition failed to complete the test correctly. The difficult level revealed for the single CC item in this study suggested that 37% (306/827) of the participants failed to complete the task. ...
January 2016
Psychogeriatrics
... However, social support and compensation coverage have been demonstrated to have had a positive impact on health [20], which helps reduce the risk of subsequent suicide attempts. By contrast, cognitive decline is more likely to occur 2 years later after natural disasters [51,52], resulting from the new onset of depression and disruption of social contacts (e.g. loss of interactions with neighbours) [51]. ...
January 2016
Psychogeriatrics
... Although only 10% of the longitudinal case-control studies assessed attentional networks, those that did found that attention performance discriminated between AD cases and controls in 100% of cases (Twamley et al., 2006). Furthermore, also vascular dementia is associated with changes in attentional processes (Akanuma et al., 2016). Therefore, investigating the connection between cardiovascular health and cognitive function appears relevant. ...
January 2016
Journal of Clinical Neuroscience
... Also, BEHAVE-AD FW can distinguish between cognitive and psychological delusions. For example, the delusion that house is not my real home, is a cognitive delusion and the delusion of institutionalization or abandonment is a psychological delusion [14]. ...
July 2012
... As described previously [5,6], diagnoses of the dementing diseases were determined during a meeting of two neurologists, a psychiatrist, and a geriatrician. Briefly, AD and VaD were diagnosed according to the NINCDS-ADRDA and the NINDS-AIREN criteria, respectively [7,8]. ...
November 2015
BMC Neurology
... Also, the number of behavioral symptoms is vast, and this feature may be yet another sign of brain degeneration. Specifically, bilinguals/multilinguals show poor performance on the Boston naming test and have more delusional ideas (81). Dementia has been shown to affect language skills and conversational performance. ...
November 2015
... Individuals with Alzheimer's disorder often experience attention deficits, in addition to memory-related complaints [47]. Stroke, a condition often involving the parietal and frontal lobes, can result in cognitive deficits, including auditory attention deficits. ...
June 2015
... Individuals with MCI appear to have intact general cognitive function and activities of daily living, but their memory is impaired for normal age. The prevalence of MCI is estimated at 3%-19% in adults older than 65 years and 15% in adults older than 75 years [14][15][16][17][18]. Less than half of people with MCI are stable or able to reverse back to normal memory function again within 5 years [19][20][21]. ...
June 2015
Psychogeriatrics