Kelsey A. Heslin’s research while affiliated with Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (16)


Figure 4
Figure 5
Automated device for simultaneous photometry and electrophysiology in freely moving animals
  • Preprint
  • File available

April 2024

·

113 Reads

·

1 Citation

·

Youngcho Kim

·

Kelsey Heslin

·

[...]

·

Photometry and electrophysiology are powerful tools for investigating brain-behavior relationships. Combining these techniques would allow us to ask previously un-addressable questions, such as how neuromodulators impact neuronal firing rates. Current options are highly limited—requiring a substantial loss in data-quality or severely restricting naturalistic-movement. These drawbacks arise from engineering-limits on devices that allow optically-tethered subjects to move freely. Here, we introduce a device that overcomes these challenges. Its automated orientation-tracking system allows subjects to move freely for multiple-hours with minimal supervision and without sacrificing data-quality. The device is modular and adaptable, being compatible with most recording systems and equipped for added functionality (e.g., optogenetics). To demonstrate its utility, we simultaneously tracked extracellular striatal dopamine and single-neuron firing as mice performed a reward-learning task. Mice showed excellent mobility, and we observed robust trial-by-trial correlations between striatal firing and dopamine signaling. This device provides a powerful tool that outperforms current commercial solutions.

Download

A Limited Cerebellar Contribution to Suprasecond Timing Across Differing Task Demands

October 2022

·

9 Reads

·

5 Citations

Behavioral Neuroscience

The involvement of the cerebellum in suprasecond interval timing (i.e., timing in the seconds to minutes range) is controversial. A limited amount of evidence from humans, nonhuman primates, and rodents has shown that the lateral cerebellum, including the lateral cerebellar nucleus (LCN), may be necessary for successful suprasecond timing performance. However, many existing studies have pitfalls, such as limited timing outcome measures and confounded task demands. In addition, many existing studies relied on well-trained subjects. This approach may be a drawback, as the cerebellum is hypothesized to carry out ongoing error correction to limit timing variability. By using only experienced subjects, past timing studies may have missed a critical window of cerebellar involvement. In the experiments described here, we pharmacologically inactivated the rat LCN across three different peak interval timing tasks. We structured our tasks to address past confounds, collect timing variability measures, and characterize performance during target duration acquisition. Across these various tasks, we did not find strong support for cerebellar involvement in suprasecond interval timing. Our findings support the existing distinction of the cerebellum as a subsecond interval timing brain region. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).


The dorsal hippocampus’ role in context-based timing in rodents

August 2022

·

39 Reads

·

3 Citations

Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

To act proactively, we must predict when future events will occur. Individuals generate temporal predictions using cues that indicate an event will happen after a certain duration elapses. Neural models of timing focus on how the brain represents these cue-duration associations. However, these models often overlook the fact that situational factors frequently modulate temporal expectations. For example, in realistic environments, the intervals associated with different cues will often covary due to a common underlying cause. According to the 'common cause hypothesis,' observers anticipate this covariance such that, when one cue's interval changes, temporal expectations for other cues shift in the same direction. Furthermore, as conditions will often differ across environments, the same cue can mean different things in different contexts. Therefore, updates to temporal expectations should be context-specific. Behavioral work supports these predictions, yet their underlying neural mechanisms are unclear. Here, we asked whether the dorsal hippocampus mediates context-based timing, given its broad role in context-conditioning. Specifically, we trained rats with either hippocampal or sham lesions that two cues predicted reward after either a short or long duration elapsed (e.g., tone-8s / light-16s). Then, we moved rats to a new context and extended the long cue’s interval (e.g., light-32s). This caused rats to respond later to the short cue, despite never being trained to do so. Importantly, when returned to the initial training context, sham rats shifted back toward both cues’ original intervals. In contrast, lesion rats continued to respond at the long cue’s newer interval. Surprisingly, they still showed contextual modulation for the short cue, responding earlier like shams. These data suggest the hippocampus only mediates context-based timing if a cue is explicitly paired and/or rewarded across distinct contexts. Furthermore, as lesions did not impact timing measures at baseline or acquisiton for the long cue’s new interval, our data suggests that the hippocampus only modulates timing when context is relevant.


Figure 2. No notable differences in baseline timing performance between sham rats and rats with dorsal hippocampus lesions. (A) Minimal task diagram. Dotted rectangle emphasizes data come from the initial training phase. (B) Mean probe trial response rates under baseline conditions. Left and right panels show data from shortand long-cue trials, respectively. For presentation, data are normalized to the sum of responding and smoothed over a 3-bin window. (C) Measures of timing under baseline conditions. Peak-time and CV (spread / mean) are derived from fitting mean response distributions with Gaussian-like functions. Start and stop times are derived from analysis of single-trials.
Figure 3. Effects of dorsal hippocampus lesions on acquisition of a new interval following a context change. (A) Minimal task diagram. (B) Average probe trial response rates at different points of acquisiton. Data are pooled across 3 -block windows occurring at the start, middle, and at the end of the acquisiton phase (left, middle, right, respectively). (C) Performance measures across individual acquisition blocks, including the peak-time, R 2 of the Gaussian fit, start times, and stop times. For reference, the first value on each axis shows data from the last training session. Dotted lines demarcate phases of acquisition, defined as the 'early' and 'late' stages. Asterisks indicate significant group-differences during that stage. Other effects (e.g., block) are noted in-text.
Figure 4. Dorsal hippocampus lesions impact context-based timing only for the explicitly changed-cue. (A) Minimal task diagram. Dotted rectangle emphasizes the primary data of interest come from the test phase. (B) Test data for the sham group. Average probe trial response rates for the short cue (left) and long cue (middle) during training, testing in the original context, and testing in the change context. The right panel shows proportional shifts in peak times during testing in either context, relative to baseline training. (C) Same as B, yet showing data from the lesion group. In all cases, asterisks centered over the bars indicate significant peak time shifts from baseline training, and asterisks spanning pairs of bars indicate significant differences between testing in either context.
The dorsal hippocampus' role in context-based timing in rodents

January 2022

·

74 Reads

To act proactively, we must predict when future events will occur. Individuals generate temporal predictions using cues that indicate an event will happen after a certain duration elapses. Neural models of timing focus on how the brain represents these cue-duration associations. However, these models often overlook the fact that situational factors frequently modulate temporal expectations. For example, in realistic environments, the intervals associated with different cues will often covary due to a common underlying cause. According to the 'common cause hypothesis,' observers anticipate this covariance such that, when one cue's interval changes, temporal expectations for other cues shift in the same direction. Furthermore, as conditions will often differ across environments, the same cue can mean different things in different contexts. Therefore, updates to temporal expectations should be context-specific. Behavioral work supports these predictions, yet their underlying neural mechanisms are unclear. Here, we asked whether the dorsal hippocampus mediates context-based timing, given its broad role in context-conditioning. Specifically, we trained rats with either hippocampal or sham lesions that two cues predicted reward after either a short or long duration elapsed (e.g., tone-8s / light-16s). Then, we moved rats to a new context and extended the long-cue's interval (e.g., light-32s). This caused rats to respond later to the short cue, despite never being trained to do so. Importantly, when returned to the initial training context, sham rats shifted back toward both cues' original intervals. In contrast, lesion rats continued to respond at the long cue's newer interval. Surprisingly, they still showed contextual modulation for the short cue, responding earlier like shams. These data suggest the hippocampus only mediates context-based timing if a cue is explicitly paired and/or rewarded across distinct contexts. Furthermore, as lesions did not impact timing measures at baseline or acquisiton for the long cue's new interval, our data suggests that the hippocampus only modulates timing when context is relevant.


Figure 2. Inactivating the prelimbic cortex (PL) or mediodorsal thalamus (MD) flattens mean response curves (n = 9). (A) Mean response rate across time during probe trials (normalized by sum) for the 8s and 16s cues (top and bottom, respectively), split by infusion type. Data were grouped into 1s bins and have been smoothed over a 3-bin window (for visualization only). (B) Percent change in the fit-quality of a gaussian distribution to response rates across time and overall mean response rates (i.e., ignoring time) during probe trials for each area, relative to saline sessions (top and bottom, respectively). Asterisks indicate p < 0.05, relative to saline sessions for a given area.
Figure 3. Inactivating the MD or PL disorganizes singe-trial patterns of timed responding (n = 9). (A) Heat maps of responses during individual trials for each infusion type (1s bins). Shuffled saline data is also presented (see text). All data have been sorted by the stop-time from the burst analysis. (B) Percent change in fit-quality of a step function fit to individual trials for each inactivation type, relative to saline. (C) Accuracy of a support vector machine trained to discriminate shuffled data from unshuffled data, split by infusion type. Centered asterisks indicate above-chance classification for a given infusion type (dotted-line = .5). Asterisks above horizontal lines indicate significance across areas.
Figure 4. Inactivation design and histological analysis for MD-PL disconnection procedure. (A) Illustration of infusion design for the disconnection procedure, including saline, crossed, and ipsilateral infusions. For simplicity, only one spatial permutation for the crossed and ipsilateral infusions is diagramed. However, all permutations were performed across sessions (see methods). (B) Diagram of infusion cannula tip locations for all rats in the experiment for the MD (top) and PL (bottom).
Figure 5. Disconnecting the MD from the PL flattens mean response curves (n = 10). (A) Mean response rate across time during probe trials (normalized by sum) for the 8s and 16s cues (top and bottom, respectively), split by infusion type. Data were grouped into 1s bins and have been smoothed over a 3-bin window (for visualization only). (B) Percent change in the fit-quality of a gaussian distribution to response rates across time and overall mean response rates (i.e., ignoring time) during probe trials for each inactivation type, relative to saline sessions (top and bottom, respectively).
Figure 6. Disconnecting the MD from the PL disorganizes singe-trial patterns of timed responding (n = 10). (A) Heat maps of responses during individual trials for each infusion type (1s bins). Shuffled saline data is also presented (see text). All data have been sorted by the stop-time from the burst analysis. (B) Percent change in fit-quality of a step function fit to individual trials for each inactivation type, relative to saline. (C) Accuracy of a support vector machine trained to discriminate shuffled data from unshuffled data, split by infusion type.
Communication between the mediodorsal thalamus and prelimbic cortex regulates timing performance in rats

June 2021

·

132 Reads

·

2 Citations

Predicting when future events will occur and adjusting behavior accordingly is critical to adaptive behavior. Despite this, little is known about the brain networks that encode time and how this ultimately impacts decision-making. One established finding is that the prefrontal cortex (PFC) and its non-human analogues (e.g., the rodent prelimbic cortex; PL) mediate timing. This provides a starting point for exploring the networks that support temporal processing by identifying areas that interact with the PFC during timing tasks. For example, substantial work has explored the role of frontostriatal circuits in timing. However, other areas are undoubtedly involved. The mediodorsal nucleus of the thalamus (MD) is an excellent candidate region. It shares dense, reciprocal connections with PFC-areas in both humans and non-human species and is implicated in cognition. However, causal data implicating MD-PFC interactions in cognition broadly is still sparse, and their role in timing specifically is currently unknown. To address this, we trained male rats on a time-based, decision-making task referred to as the 'peak-interval' procedure. During the task, presentation of a cue instructed the rats to respond after a specific interval of time elapsed (e.g., tone-8 seconds). We incorporated two cues; each requiring a response after a distinct time-interval (e.g., tone-8 seconds / light-16 seconds). We tested the effects of either reversibly inactivating the MD or PL individually or functionally disconnecting them on performance. All manipulations caused a comparable timing deficit. Specifically, responses showed little organization in time, as if primarily guided by motivational systems. These data expand our understanding of the networks that support timing and suggest that MD-PL interactions specifically are a core component. More broadly, our results suggest that timing tasks provide a reliable assay for characterizing the role of MD-PL interactions in cognition using rodents, which has been difficult to establish in the past.


No preference for prosocial helping behavior in rats with concurrent social interaction opportunities

April 2021

·

46 Reads

·

16 Citations

Learning & Behavior

Helping behavior tasks are proposed to assess prosocial or “empathic” behavior in rodents. This paradigm characterizes the behavior of subject animals presented with the opportunity to release a conspecific from a distressing situation. Previous studies found a preference in rats for releasing restrained or distressed conspecifics over other controls (e.g., empty restrainers or inanimate objects). An empathy account was offered to explain the observed behaviors, claiming subjects were motivated to reduce the distress of others based on a rodent homologue of empathy. An opposing account attributes all previous results to subjects seeking social contact. To dissociate these two accounts for helping behavior, we presented subject rats with three simultaneous choice alternatives: releasing a restrained conspecific, engaging a nonrestrained conspecific, or not socializing. Subjects showed an initial preference for socializing with the nonrestrained conspecific, and no preference for helping. This result contradicts the empathy account, but is consistent with the social-contact account of helping behavior.


Why do learners ignore expected feedback in making metacognitive decisions about retrieval practice?

March 2021

·

97 Reads

·

2 Citations

Memory & Cognition

We report two experiments investigating why learners, in making metacognitive judgments, often seem to ignore or otherwise fail to appreciate that feedback following retrieval practice provides a restudy opportunity. Learners practiced word pairs for a final cued-recall test by studying each pair initially, making a judgment of learning (JOL), and then deciding whether to practice the pair again after a short or long spacing interval, or not at all. For different groups in Experiment 1, additional practice involved restudying, retrieval practice without feedback, or retrieval practice with feedback (the full pair). We used procedures (long feedback duration and covert retrieval practice) designed to rule out the possibility that feedback is ignored because it is usually brief or because participants’ choices are influenced by a desire to look good by performing well on overt practice tests. In the relearning condition, learners preferred a long spacing interval for items at all JOL levels. Despite the feedback duration and the covert retrieval practice, learners in both retrieval-practice conditions preferred a short spacing interval for hard, low-JOL items and a long spacing interval for easy, high-JOL items, even though this may not be an effective strategy when feedback is provided. In Experiment 2, instructions framed feedback either as a presentation of the correct answer or as a restudy opportunity preceded by retrieval practice. Framing feedback as a restudy opportunity markedly changed the choices learners made. Apparently, the restudy function of feedback does not occur to learners unless they are specifically alerted to it.


Figure 1. (a) Diagram of the task arena (top view). All three choice chambers options were
No preference for prosocial ‘helping’ behavior in rats with concurrent social interaction opportunities

November 2020

·

76 Reads

·

1 Citation

‘Helping behavior’ tasks are proposed to assess prosocial or ‘empathic’ behavior in rodents. This paradigm characterizes the behavior of subject animals presented with the opportunity to release a conspecific from a distressing situation. Previous studies found a preference in rats for releasing restrained or distressed conspecifics over other controls (e.g., empty restrainers or inanimate objects). An empathy account was offered to explain the observed behaviors, claiming subjects were motivated to reduce the distress of others based on a rodent homologue of empathy. An opposing account attributes all previous results to subjects seeking social-contact. To dissociate these two accounts for helping behavior, we presented subject rats with three simultaneous choice alternatives: releasing a restrained conspecific, engaging a non-restrained conspecific, or not socializing. Subjects showed an initial preference for socializing with the non-restrained conspecific, and no preference for helping. This result contradicts the empathy account, but is consistent with the social-contact account of helping behavior.


Figure 1. Chronic stress increases activity of prefrontal PV + neurons in males and females. (A) Representative image showing PV (red) and cFos (green) double-labeling (white arrow) and DAPI (blue). Scale bar: 10 µm. (B) Chronic stress increases the number of PV/cFos double-labeled cells in the mPFC of females and males. *p = 0.05; ***p < 0.001.
Figure 2. Injection of an AAV-DIO-hM3DGq DREADD virus in the mPFC of PV:Cre mice. (A) Representative coronal image shows bilateral expression of mCherry localized to the infralimbic cortex (IL) and part of the prelimbic cortex (PrL). Scale bar: 50 µm. (B) Representative images showing selective expression of the hM3DGq virus (red) in parvalbumin (PV -green) interneurons. Scale bar: 20 µm. (C) Stereological analysis indicated that 92.23 ± 1.31% of mPFC PV + neurons expressed the control or hM3DGq virus, and (D) 96.85 ± 0.70% mCherry cells are PV + . (E) Verification of hM3DGq virus response to acute and chronic (once per day for 21 days) CNO injection using stereological analysis of the number of mCherry cells expressing cFos: both acute and chronic injection of CNO in the PFC of mice expressing the hM3DGq virus leads to high expression of cFos in mCherry cells when compared to cells expressing the control virus. Representative image of mCherry cells expressing cFos (white arrow) vs. mCherry cells not expressing cFos (white arrowhead). Scale bar: 20 µm. (F) Verification that acute and chronic chemogenetic activation of prefrontal PV + cells lead to hypofunction of the PFC: both regimen of CNO injection reduce significantly the amount of cFos + cells, showing overall hypofrontality. Representative images of cFos expression in the PFC of mice expressing the control or the hM3DGq virus. Scale bar: 20 µm. **p < 0.001; ***p < 0.0001.
Figure 3. Chemogenetic chronic activation of PV + neurons in the mPFC of PV:Cre male and female mice increases anxiety-like behaviors in a sex-specific manner. (Top) Experimental schematic of the timeline followed. Mice injected with the control (AAV-DIO-mCherry) or the AAV-DIO-hM3DGq virus received a daily injection of CNO over the course of 21 days. Four days after the last injection, mice were tested once daily in a behavioral test to assess various sub-domains of emotional behaviors and cognitive functions. (A-F) Behavioral endpoints measured in the OF test in control or hM3DGq mice. Female mice that underwent chronic activation of mPFC PV + neurons have decreased time spent in the center of the arena, longer distance travelled near the walls, and decreased number of unsupported rearing. In males, chronic activation of mPFC PV + neurons did not lead to changes in behaviors in the OF test. (G) Females that submitted to chronic activation of mPFC PV + neurons displayed increased latency to eat the food in the NSF test, while no change in behavior was observed in males. (H) No effect of chronic activation of mPFC PV + neurons was found in the ORT. ***p < 0.001; *p ≤ 0.05. *p = 0.057 N = 7-9 per group per sex. CNO: clozapine-N-oxide; OF: open field; ORT: object recognition test; NSF: novelty-suppressed feeding test.
Figure 4. Chemogenetic acute activation of PV + neurons in the mPFC of PV:Cre male and female mice does not impact behaviors in the OF test. (Top) Experimental schematic of the timeline followed. Mice injected with the control (AAV-DIO-mCherry) or the AAV-DIO-hM3DGq virus received one injection of CNO 30 minutes prior to the OF. (A-C) Acute activation of mPFC PV + neurons did not impact time spent in the center, locomotion near the walls or total locomotion in the novel arena in male and female mice. N = 8-9 per group per sex. OF: open field.
Prefrontal parvalbumin cells are sensitive to stress and mediate anxiety-related behaviors in female mice

December 2019

·

689 Reads

·

91 Citations

Reduced activity of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) is seen in mood disorders including depression and anxiety. The mechanisms of this hypofrontality remain unclear. Because of their specific physiological properties, parvalbumin-expressing (PV+) inhibitory interneurons contribute to the overall activity of the PFC. Our recent work using a chronic stress mouse model showed that stress-induced increases in prefrontal PV expression correlates with increased anxiety-like behaviors in female mice. Our goal is now to provide a causal relationship between changes in prefrontal PV+ cells and changes in emotional behaviors in mice. We first show that, in addition to increasing overall level of PV expression, chronic stress increases the activity of prefrontal PV+ cells. We then used a chemogenetic approach to mimic the effects of chronic stress and specifically increase the activity of prefrontal PV+ cells. We observed that chemogenetic activation of PV+ cells caused an overall reduction in prefrontal activity, and that chronic activation of PV+ cells lead to increased anxiety-related behaviors in female mice only. These results demonstrate that activity of prefrontal PV+ cells could represent a novel sex-specific modulator of anxiety-related behaviors, potentially through changes in overall prefrontal activity. The findings also support the idea that prefrontal PV+ cells are worth further investigation to better understand mood disorders that are more prevalent in female populations.


Cerebellar D1DR-expressing neurons modulate the frontal cortex during timing tasks

August 2019

·

51 Reads

·

8 Citations

Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

Converging lines of evidence suggest that the cerebellum plays an integral role in cognitive function through its interactions with association cortices like the medial frontal cortex (MFC). It is unknown precisely how the cerebellum influences the frontal cortex and what type of information is reciprocally relayed between these two regions. A subset of neurons in the cerebellar dentate nuclei, or the homologous lateral cerebellar nuclei (LCN) in rodents, express D1 dopamine receptors (D1DRs) and may play a role in cognitive processes. We investigated how pharmacologically blocking LCN D1DRs influences performance in an interval timing task and impacts neuronal activity in the frontal cortex. Interval timing requires executive processes such as working memory, attention, and planning and is known to rely on both the frontal cortex and cerebellum. In our interval timing task, male rats indicated their estimates of the passage of a period of several seconds by making lever presses for a water reward. We have shown that a cue-evoked burst of low-frequency activity in the MFC initiates ramping activity (i.e., monotonic increases or decreases of firing rate over time) in single MFC neurons. These patterns of activity are associated with successful interval timing performance. Here we explored how blocking right LCN D1DRs with the D1DR antagonist SCH23390 influences timing performance and neural activity in the contralateral (left) MFC. Our results indicate that blocking LCN D1DRs impaired some measures of interval timing performance. Additionally, ramping activity of MFC single units was significantly attenuated. These data provide insight into how catecholamines in the LCN may drive MFC neuronal dynamics to influence cognitive function.


Citations (9)


... This may be essential as abnormal function of the cerebrocerebellar pathway is implicated in a myriad of neuropsychiatric illnesses like schizophrenia. During interval timing, previous work from our lab has reported inconsistent results from inactivation of the lateral cerebellar nucleus with muscimol [54,55]. Given the role for the cerebellum in learning, studies investigating cerebellar involvement in Prickle2 −/− mice on the interval timing task may still be warranted at developmental timepoints, or with altered temporal durations, cues, or expected responses timing. ...

Reference:

Knockdown of the Non-canonical Wnt Gene Prickle2 Leads to Cerebellar Purkinje Cell Abnormalities While Cerebellar-Mediated Behaviors Remain Intact
A Limited Cerebellar Contribution to Suprasecond Timing Across Differing Task Demands

Behavioral Neuroscience

... The prominent role of hippocampal-associated networks was evident in their divergence across groups of mice that adopted different behavioural strategies during learning as well as in groups of mice that showed evidence of more or less reliance on the formation of temporal expectations to optimize motor behaviour. This is in line with previously described functions of the hippocampus in temporal processing as well as related sensory-driven representations (De Corte et al., 2022;Dylda & Pakan, 2021;Finnie et al., 2021;Pastalkova et al., 2008;Taxidis et al., 2020;Terada et al., 2017). Interestingly, here we show that pre-existing differences in BOLD responses and functional connectivity in hippocampal networks are strongly related to a later capacity for attending to the temporal structure of the task. ...

The dorsal hippocampus’ role in context-based timing in rodents
  • Citing Article
  • August 2022

Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

... As effects such as temporal averaging, Vierordt's law, and covariance expectations often operate cross-modally, one might expect a multisensory area to be involved. Functionally, the MD plays a broad role in cognition (Markowitsch, 1982;Peräkylä et al., 2017), and while data are limited, manipulations of the MD disrupt baseline timing performance (Yu et al., 2010;Lusk et al., 2020;De Corte et al., 2021). As a notable example, Lusk et al. (2020) recently inhibited the MD optogenetically during the peak-procedure and found that peak times shifted later. ...

Communication between the mediodorsal thalamus and prelimbic cortex regulates timing performance in rats

... Authors suggested that this behavior could be explained by a desire for social contact, which is often considered a reward in rescuing tasks [17,53,55,70,24,96]. One study found that rats preferentially opted to play with a free conspecific (e.g., social opportunity) rather than liberate a trapped congener in an adjacent compartment [109]. Six studies reported distress/alarm calls (22 kHz) through ultrasonic vocalizations in restrained rats [17,70]. ...

No preference for prosocial helping behavior in rats with concurrent social interaction opportunities
  • Citing Article
  • April 2021

Learning & Behavior

... Parvalbumin (PV)-positive neurons account for about half of the GABAergic neurons in the cortex and are implicated in regulating neuronal excitability 13 . Recent studies reported the role of PV interneurons in regulating Alzheimer's disease-related anxiety and anxiety-like behaviors under chronic stress conditions in rodents 14,15 . However, whether PV-positive neurons participate in neuropathic pain-associated anxiety is still elusive. ...

Prefrontal parvalbumin cells are sensitive to stress and mediate anxiety-related behaviors in female mice

... Other sources of cerebellar DA are the locus coeruleus (Canton-Josh et al. 2022) and TH-positive cerebellar Purkinje cells (Lazarov et al. 1998;Glaser et al. 2006;Kim et al. 2009;Locke et al. 2020). Irrespective of the dopaminergic signal source, D1R and D2R expressed in the DCN have been ascribed roles in nonmotor functions, such as social behavior and cognition (Locke et al. 2018;Heskje et al. 2020;Cutando et al. 2022). Furthermore, monosynaptic efferent projections from the FN to the VTA (Carta et al. 2019), substantia nigra (Snider et al. 1976;Washburn et al.), and locus coeruleus (Snider 1975), have been described. ...

Cerebellar D1DR-expressing neurons modulate the frontal cortex during timing tasks
  • Citing Article
  • August 2019

Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

... Npas4 (Neuronal Per/Arnt/Sim (PAS) domain protein 4) is a member of the helix-loop-helix class of transcriptional regulators involved in numerous developmental events via neuronal activity [24,25]. In particular, research shows that Npas4 is expressed in diverse interneurons, including somatostatin, parvalbumin, and olfactory bulb interneurons and regulated by neuronal activity [26][27][28]. It has been reported that Npas4 plays a role in the development of inhibitory synapses in hippocampal neurons and the visual system [29]. ...

Downregulation of Npas4 in parvalbumin interneurons and cognitive deficits after neonatal NMDA receptor blockade: relevance for schizophrenia

Translational Psychiatry

... These results are consistent with some previous observations. There are reports that chronic stress prenatally (Allgäuer et al., 2023;Heslin & Coutellier, 2018;Uchida et al., 2014;van de Looij et al., 2019), neonatally (Murthy et al., 2019) and in adult rodents (Banasr et al., 2017;Hu et al., 2010) decreases cortical Pvalb expression, although increased expression after adult stress has also been reported (Page et al., 2019). High GC concentrations for 72h also reportedly suppress Gad1 (but not Gad2) expression in 10 DIV cultured cortical neurons (Banasr et al., 2017), and prenatal GC exposure in vivo reduces offspring Pvalb expression in hippocampus (Zhang et al., 2023). ...

Npas4 deficiency and prenatal stress interact to affect social recognition in mice
  • Citing Article
  • December 2017

... The accurate maintenance of inhibitory signaling is especially crucial during development and adolescence. Npas4 is important for inhibitory synapse development in the prefrontal cortex during adolescence [38], implicating Npas4 dysregulation in disorders such as schizophrenia [39]. The deletion of Npas4 with a Cre construct in the CA3 of mice impairs contextual fear memory and inhibits mossy fiber connections [40]. ...

The transcription factor Npas4 contributes to adolescent development of prefrontal inhibitory circuits, and to cognitive and emotional functions: Implications for neuropsychiatric disorders
  • Citing Article
  • December 2016

Neurobiology of Disease