January 2018
·
20 Reads
This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.
January 2018
·
20 Reads
November 2016
·
78 Reads
·
13 Citations
Behavior Therapy
Body weight is regulated by a complex interaction of biological, behavioral, and cultural factors. The population as a whole is at risk for obesity because of increased intake of dietary fat, the consumption of calories in fewer meals per day, striking accessibility to palatable foods, and decreased physical activity. This risk may become a reality in individuals with certain biological predispositions (genetic tendency, low metabolic rate, increased fat cell number), specific eating patterns, and susceptibility to the extreme cultural pressure to be lean. These factors must be considered in establishing goals for treatment, which fall into medical and psychosocial categories. This includes defining a “reasonable” as opposed to “ideal” weight. A three-stage process is proposed for identifying the best treatment for an individual. This involves a classification decision, a stepped care decision, and then a matching decision. Criteria are provided for a comprehensive assessment of the overweight individual, and treatment options are reviewed for programs of varying intensity, cost, and risk.
September 2014
·
117 Reads
·
2 Citations
Obesity
January 2014
·
199 Reads
·
824 Citations
January 2013
·
531 Reads
·
43 Citations
International Journal of Obesity
Objective: To investigate the influence of a defendant's weight on simulated jurors' perceptions of guilt. Design and methods: Participants were 471 lean and overweight adults (mean body mass index: 25.34±5.91) who read a vignette describing a case of check fraud while viewing one of four images of the alleged defendant (a lean male, a lean female, an obese male or an obese female). Participants rated the defendant's culpability on a 5-point Likert scale and completed measures of anti-fat attitudes. Results: Male respondents endorsed greater overall weight bias than females (F (1470)=23.815, P<0.01, η(2)=0.048). A three-way interaction was detected between participant sex, defendant sex and defendant weight on perceptions of guilt such that when the defendant was female, male participants were significantly more likely to find her guilty if she was obese than if she was lean (guilt ratings=4.05±0.83; 3.31±1.03, respectively; F(1467)=5.935, P=0.015, R(2)=0.060). In addition, lean male participants were significantly more likely to believe that the obese female defendant met criteria for check fraud, and indicated greater belief she would be a repeat offender, compared with the lean female defendant (t(90)=2.081, P=0.040; t(90)=2.395 P=0.019, respectively). There were no differences in perceptions of guilt or responsibility between the obese male and the lean male defendants. Conclusion: The results of this novel study indicate that both weight and gender of a defendant may affect juror perceptions of guilt and responsibility.
July 2012
·
85 Reads
·
81 Citations
In an article that forms part of the PLoS Medicine series on Big Food, Kelly Brownell offers a perspective on engaging with the food industry, and argues that governments and the public health community should be working for regulation, not collaboration.
January 2012
·
28 Reads
·
8 Citations
This chapter discusses the responsibility of changing public policy to address obesity. The understanding of obesity as an issue of personal responsibility has led to weak government action. The chapter notes that the environment is the major force underlying the obesity crisis and then addresses school food environments, food access and cost, sugared beverage consumption, food marketing, and restaurant food nutrition content and portion size. Comprehensive bans on advertising are difficult to develop, given the range of ways in which foods are advertised, such as on the Internet and through videogames. Menu labeling could promote healthier food choices when dining out. Public health policies that target the school food environment, issues of access and cost of foods, sugar beverage consumption, food marketing, restaurant food nutrition content and portion size can have far-reaching effects in curbing the obesity epidemic and in improving the world's diet and health in cost-effective ways.
July 2011
·
105 Reads
·
2 Citations
Health Psychology
Reports an error in "Mind over milkshakes: Mindsets, not just nutrients, determine ghrelin response" by Alia J. Crum, William R. Corbin, Kelly D. Brownell and Peter Salovey (Health Psychology, np). In the second paragraph on the first page, the Allison & Uhl 1964 citation is incorrect. The corrected sentence and full citation is provided in the erratum. All versions of this article have been corrected. (The following abstract of the original article appeared in record 2011-09907-001.) Objective: To test whether physiological satiation as measured by the gut peptide ghrelin may vary depending on the mindset in which one approaches consumption of food. Methods: On 2 separate occasions, participants (n = 46) consumed a 380-calorie milkshake under the pretense that it was either a 620-calorie "indulgent" shake or a 140-calorie "sensible" shake. Ghrelin was measured via intravenous blood samples at 3 time points: baseline (20 min), anticipatory (60 min), and postconsumption (90 min). During the first interval (between 20 and 60 min) participants were asked to view and rate the (misleading) label of the shake. During the second interval (between 60 and 90 min) participants were asked to drink and rate the milkshake. Results: The mindset of indulgence produced a dramatically steeper decline in ghrelin after consuming the shake, whereas the mindset of sensibility produced a relatively flat ghrelin response. Participants' satiety was consistent with what they believed they were consuming rather than the actual nutritional value of what they consumed. Conclusions: The effect of food consumption on ghrelin may be psychologically mediated, and mindset meaningfully affects physiological responses to food. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2011 APA, all rights reserved).
May 2011
·
8,500 Reads
·
235 Citations
Health Psychology
To test whether physiological satiation as measured by the gut peptide ghrelin may vary depending on the mindset in which one approaches consumption of food. On 2 separate occasions, participants (n = 46) consumed a 380-calorie milkshake under the pretense that it was either a 620-calorie "indulgent" shake or a 140-calorie "sensible" shake. Ghrelin was measured via intravenous blood samples at 3 time points: baseline (20 min), anticipatory (60 min), and postconsumption (90 min). During the first interval (between 20 and 60 min) participants were asked to view and rate the (misleading) label of the shake. During the second interval (between 60 and 90 min) participants were asked to drink and rate the milkshake. The mindset of indulgence produced a dramatically steeper decline in ghrelin after consuming the shake, whereas the mindset of sensibility produced a relatively flat ghrelin response. Participants' satiety was consistent with what they believed they were consuming rather than the actual nutritional value of what they consumed. The effect of food consumption on ghrelin may be psychologically mediated, and mindset meaningfully affects physiological responses to food.
January 2011
·
7 Reads
·
6 Citations
Public Health Nutrition
... In reviewing research, Puhl and Brownell (2004) stated, Research is clear in showing that obese individuals are highly stigmatized, and that bias and discrimination are often a consequence. Given that half the [American] population is overweight, the number of people potentially faced with discrimination and stigmatization is immense. ...
Reference:
Homeostatic theory of obesity
January 2003
... Manufacturers need to make it easy-to-understand so the goal of usage of nutrition label can be achieve. Updating the Nutrition Facts Panel, developing a uniform labelling system and providing consumers with nutrition information make it easy for consumer to become aware about nutrition label and motivated to make healthier choices [42]. Mostly consumers prefer to have nutrition information listed for 100 grams rather than per serving as its easier to compare the product displayed with 100 grams nutrition label [32,27]. ...
January 2011
Public Health Nutrition
... The good/bad and motivated/lazy weight IATs were significantly correlated with each other, with strength of the correlation replicating that of a previous study (Wang et al., 2004; r = 0.38, p < 0.001). However, implicit weight bias (as measured by both IATs) was not associated with explicit weight bias as measured by the OPTS, supporting prior research that demonstrated a lack of association between implicit and explicit measures of weight bias (Teachman & Brownell, 2001;Wang et al., 2004). This finding provides further evidence that individuals monitor and censor their reported attitudes on explicit measures of anti-fat attitudes. ...
January 2001
International Journal of Obesity
... Weight stigma can manifest in healthcare experiences, the workplace, education, mass media, and personal relationships. These stigmatizing experiences can have negative psychosocial implications, resulting in social isolation, diminished self-esteem, and poor health (Harwood et al., 2022;Puhl & Brownell, 2001;Puhl & Heuer, 2009, 2010. ...
January 2014
... Behavioural Weight Loss (BWL) interventions are reported to be ineffective for individuals living with overweight/obesity [7]. These interventions do not address the psychological factors associated with eating [8] (such as emotional eating (EE) and/or binge eating behaviours) that are associated with overweight, obesity, and poor mental wellbeing [9]. ...
November 2016
Behavior Therapy
... 5 The Framingham study was one of the first studies to show that subjects with higher body weight variability compared with those with low body weight variability had a significantly higher risk of developing CVDs. 6 Other studies also showed that variability in BMI and body weight is associated with a higher risk of mortality and cardiovascular events in coronary disease and diabetes mellitus patients. 6,7 Although there are systematic reviews that study the association of BMI with cardiovascular outcomes and mortality, to our knowledge no studies have explored the associations of variability in BMI with cardiovascular outcomes as a systematic review. ...
December 1991
The New-England Medical Review and Journal
... Dans le même ordre d'idées, compte tenu du temps d'exposition à des messages publicitaires portant sur des aliments, dont la majorité font la promotion d'aliments de faible valeur nutritive, on pourrait adopter des politiques visant à imposer des restrictions au niveau de la publicité. Compte tenu de l'opposition probable des corporations et des défenseurs des libertés civiles, il est important de reconnaître que l'appui du public s'avère essentiel à l'adoption et au succès de telles politiques 59 . Des études s'imposent en vue d'évaluer l'impact sur la saine alimentation des restrictions actuelles imposées en matière de publicité, comme l'interdiction de la publicité destinée aux enfants au Québec 61 . ...
January 1998
... In the current food environment, many consumers prefer to eat junk food rather than healthy food, there are many reasons. Firstly, because calorie-dense foods are widely available, inexpensive, heavily marketed, not clearly labeled, and served in large portions [1][2][3]. This kind of environment makes people easy to choose unhealthy food. ...
January 2012
... This included standard behavioral, weight-management techniques (e.g., self-monitoring, self-efficacy, goal setting, stimulus control, etc.), as well as cognitive-behavioral techniques for restructuring and attitude change. The nutritional and behavioral modification sessions, conducted by dietitian and behavioral psychologist, respectively, were complemented with educational material, including lecture outlines, brochures, and practical tips on how to comply with the meal plan, focusing on maintenance of lifestyle change and relapse prevention [47,48]. ...
January 1996
... It constitutes one of the most socially acceptable forms of discrimination. 112,113 Unfortunately, both implicit and explicit weight biases are common among medical students and physicians, but many are unlikely cognizant of it. [114][115][116][117] Patients who are overweight or obese often report disrespectful treatment (e.g., verbal insults, inappropriate humor) from healthcare professionals, 118 and those experiencing weight discrimination are believed to be at higher risk for cardiometabolic disease 119 and psychological health-related concerns. ...
January 2001