Katherine S. White’s research while affiliated with University of Waterloo and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (46)


Accent variation and the development of speech and language abilities
  • Article

May 2025

·

1 Read

Infant Behavior & Development

Katherine S White

·


Who is running our experiments? The influence of experimenter identity in the marshmallow task

January 2023

·

108 Reads

·

6 Citations

Cognitive Development

While developmental researchers take great care to report on the characteristics of their participants, they rarely report on the characteristics of their experimenter(s). This is surprising, given the real potential for experimenter identity (e.g., gender, race, age, etc.), especially as it relates to children’s identities, to influence children’s behavior in experiments. In the current study, we investigate how experimenter identity (as signaled by language and race cues) influences 3- to 5-year-old children’s (N = 159) behavior in the famous marshmallow task. Results show that experimenter identity indeed influenced children’s wait times in the marshmallow task; specifically, we found that racial mismatch between experimenter and child led to longer wait times, and in an exploratory analysis, we found that this effect was exaggerated by an additional mismatch in accent. We thus reveal a previously overlooked factor that may influence children’s behavior in a delayed gratification task—experimenter identity—and discuss the important implications of these findings for developmental research more broadly.


Children’s Accent-based Inferences Depend on Geographic Background

October 2022

·

5 Reads

Young children make inferences about speakers based on their accents. Here, we show that these accent-based inferences are influenced by information about speakers’ geographic backgrounds. In Experiment 1, 4- to 6-year-olds (N = 60) inferred that a speaker would be more likely to have the same cultural preferences as another speaker with the same accent than a speaker with a different accent; in Experiment 2 (N = 90), children made similar inferences about speakers’ friendship preferences. Critically, in both experiments, children were less likely to make accent-based inferences when they were told that the speakers all came from different places (both experiments) or from the same place (Experiment 2). These results suggest that young children’s accent-based inferences hinge on information about geographic background and provide insight into how and why children make accent-based inferences. These findings are also the first to show that young children use accent to infer other people’s social preferences.


Preschoolers are sensitive to accent distance

October 2022

·

20 Reads

Can children tell how different a speaker’s accent is from their own? In Experiment 1 (N = 84), four- and five-year-olds heard speakers with different accents and indicated where they thought each speaker lived relative to a reference point on a map that represented their current location. Five-year-olds generally placed speakers with stronger accents (as judged by adults) at more distant locations than speakers with weaker accents. In contrast, four year-olds did not show differences in where they placed speakers with different accents. In Experiment 2 (N = 56), the same sentences were low-pass filtered so that only prosodic information remained. This time, children judged which of five possible aliens had produced each utterance, given a reference speaker. Children of both ages showed differences in which alien they chose based on accent, and generally rated speakers with foreign accents as more different from their native accent than speakers with regional accents. Together, the findings show that preschoolers perceive accent distance, that children may be sensitive to the distinction between foreign and regional accents, and that preschoolers likely use prosody to differentiate among accents.


Experimenter identity: An invisible, lurking variable in developmental research

June 2022

·

50 Reads

·

6 Citations

Developmental researchers are well aware that children behave differently around different people. Nevertheless, researchers rarely consider (and report on) who is running their studies. Indeed, in a survey of articles published in the last 3 years in 4 top developmental journals, we find that the vast majority of studies fail to report any information about experimenter identity, despite the fact that child–adult interactions may be strongly influenced by the social inferences that individuals draw from one another. We argue that developmental researchers need to acknowledge how experimenter identity could be acting as an invisible, lurking variable, influencing the outcome and generalizability of studies. We provide simple suggestions for how researchers and journals can begin to address this issue, thereby improving the quality and depth of the work in our field.


The Impact of Phonological Biases on Mispronunciation Sensitivity and Novel Accent Adaptation

May 2022

·

23 Reads

·

2 Citations

Language Learning and Development

Successful word recognition requires that listeners attend to differences that are phonemic in the language while also remaining flexible to the variation introduced by different voices and accents. Previous work has demonstrated that American-English-learning 19-month-olds are able to balance these demands: although one-off one-feature mispronunciations typically disrupt English-learning toddlers’ lexical access, they no longer do after toddlers are exposed to a novel accent in which these changes occur systematically. The flexibility to deal with different types of variation may not be the same for toddlers learning different first languages, however, as language structure shapes early phonological biases. We examined French-learning 19-month-olds’ sensitivity and adaptation to a novel accent that shifted either the standard pronunciation of /a/ from [a] to [ɛ] (Experiment 1) or the standard pronunciation of /p/ from [p] to [t] (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, French-learning toddlers recognized words with /a/ produced as [ɛ], regardless of whether they were previously exposed to an accent that contained this vowel shift or not. In Experiment 2, toddlers did not recognize words with /p/ pronounced as [t] at test unless they were first familiarized with an accent that contained this consonant shift. These findings are consistent with evidence that French-learning toddlers privilege consonants over vowels in lexical processing. Together with previous work, these results demonstrate both differences and similarities in how French- and English-learning children treat variation, in line with their language-specific phonological biases.


Example of familiarization and test phase of one trial of Experiment 1. Children first saw each informant label a set of familiar objects. This was followed by the test phase, in which each informant used a label to describe a novel object. A red box appeared around each informant as her test utterance played (Photo 13942088 / Smiles © Kurhan | Dreamstime.com).
Proportion of trials in each experimental condition in which children endorsed the novel label provided by an accurate informant over an inaccurate (grammatically incorrect) informant. Error bars indicate 95% CI.
Children Treat Grammatical Errors Differently for Native and Non-Native Speakers
  • Article
  • Full-text available

April 2022

·

188 Reads

·

4 Citations

Both children and adults demonstrate biases against non-native speakers. However, in some situations, adults act more generously towards non-native speakers than towards native speakers. In particular, adults judge errors from non-native speakers less harshly, presumably because they expect such errors. In the present study, we asked whether 5-6-year-old children place less weight on errors from speakers with a foreign accent. In Experiment 1, 5- and 6-year-old children (N = 80) listened to pairs of either native or foreign-accented speakers (between-subjects) label objects. For native speaker pairings, children preferred information provided by grammatical speakers over information from speakers who made subject-verb agreement errors. In contrast, children chose between foreign-accented speakers at chance. In Experiment 2 (N = 40), children preferred information from grammatical foreign-accented speakers over information from foreign-accented speakers who produced word-order violations. These findings constitute the first demonstration that children treat speech errors differently based on a speaker’s language background.

Download

The impact of phonological biases on mispronunciation sensitivity and novel accent adaptation

April 2022

·

16 Reads

Successful word recognition requires that listeners attend to differences that are phonemic in that language while also remaining flexible to the variation introduced by different voices and accents. Previous work has emonstrated that American-English-learning 19-month-olds are able to balance these demands: although one-off one-feature mispronunciations typically disrupt English-learning toddlers’ lexical access, they no longer do after toddlers are exposed to a novel accent in which these changes occur systematically (White & Aslin, 2011; White & Daub, 2021). The flexibility to deal with different types of variation may not be the same for toddlers learning different first languages, however, as language structure shapes early phonological biases. We examined French-learning 19-month-olds’ sensitivity and adaptation to a novel accent that shifted either the standard pronunciation of /a/ from [a] to [E] (Experiment 1) or the standard pronunciation of /p/ from [p] to [t] (Experiment 2). In Experiment 1, French-learning toddlers recognized words with /a/ produced as [E], regardless of whether they were previously exposed to an accent that contained this vowel shift or not. In Experiment 2, toddlers did not recognize words with /p/ pronounced as [t] at test unless they were first familiarized with an accent that contained this consonant shift. These findings are consistent with evidence that French-learning toddlers privilege consonants over vowels in lexical processing. Together with previous work, these results demonstrate both differences and similarities in how French- and English-learning children treat variation, in line with their language-specific phonological biases.


When it's appropriate not to adapt: Toddlers' learning of novel speech patterns is affected by visual information.

September 2021

·

40 Reads

·

2 Citations

Brain and Language

In adults, perceptual learning for speech is constrained, such that learning of novel pronunciations is less likely to occur if the (e.g., visual) context indicates that they are transient. However, adults have had a lifetime of experience with the types of cues that signal stable vs. transient speech variation. We ask whether visual context affects toddlers’ learning of a novel speech pattern. Across conditions, 19-month-olds (N = 117) were exposed to familiar words either pronounced typically or in a novel, consonant-shifting accent. During exposure, some toddlers heard the accented pronunciations without a face present; others saw a video of the speaker producing the words with a lollipop against her cheek or in her mouth. Toddlers showed the weakest learning of the accent when the speaker had the lollipop in her mouth, suggesting that they treated the lollipop as the cause of the atypical pronunciations. These results demonstrate that toddlers’ adaptation to a novel speech pattern is influenced by extra-linguistic context.


Toddlers Link Social and Speech Variation During Word Learning

August 2021

·

145 Reads

·

5 Citations

Within a language, there is considerable variation in the pronunciations of words owing to social factors like age, gender, nationality, and race. In the present study, we investigate whether toddlers link social and linguistic variation during word learning. In Experiment 1, 24- to 26-month-old toddlers were exposed to two talkers whose front vowels differed systematically. One talker trained them on a word-referent mapping. At test, toddlers saw the trained object and a novel object; they heard a single novel label from both talkers. Toddlers responded differently to the label as a function of talker. The following experiments demonstrate that toddlers generalize specific pronunciations across speakers of the same race (Experiment 2), but not across speakers who are simply an unfamiliar race (Experiment 3). They also generalize pronunciations based on previous affiliative behavior (Experiment 4). When affiliative behavior and race are pitted against each other, toddlers' linguistic interpretations are more influenced by affiliative behavior (Experiment 5). These experiments suggest that toddlers attend to and link social and speech variation in their environment. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).


Citations (39)


... Kidd et al. (2013) showed that children's perception of the experimenter's trustworthiness influences their strategy. In fact, the experimenter's identity alone (e.g., perceived as in-group vs. outgroup) had a significant impact on the wait time in the task (Pierre et al., 2023;Strickland, 1972). Furthermore, children's performance depends on their cultural background. ...

Reference:

Social context as a source of variability in the psychological sciences
Who is running our experiments? The influence of experimenter identity in the marshmallow task
  • Citing Article
  • January 2023

Cognitive Development

... In the past 50 years, we have learned a lot about the development of speech perception and word recognition abilities in infants and toddlers (e.g. Johnson & White, 2019). And more recently, we have started to learn a great deal about how early language development differs in monolingual and bilingual children (e.g. ...

Six Questions in Infant Speech and Language Development
  • Citing Chapter
  • October 2019

... Moreover, recent work suggests that experimenter characteristics can exert a large influence on children's performance in experimental tasks (St. Pierre et al., 2022, 2023. We minimized the possibility that the experimenter's identity would account for differences between conditions and between our two studies by recording videos of the same informant providing testimony about the location of the cups and standardizing the information that children received about the informant's honesty prior to pla ...

Experimenter identity: An invisible, lurking variable in developmental research
  • Citing Article
  • June 2022

... Word form recognition studies typically examine infants' preference for lists of words presented without visual referentsusing, for example, the head turn paradigm (e.g., Vihman et al., 2004), and testing their sensitivity to phonological mispronunciations. Word recognition studies explore word processing through a recognition response for a meaningful relationship between a word form and its visual referent, usually through the use of the inter-modal looking paradigm (e.g., Mani & Plunkett, 2007) or eyetracking (Von Holzen, van Ommen, White, & Nazzi, 2022), again testing infants' sensitivity to mispronunciations. Of course, although word form recognition studies cannot infer any understanding of word meaning, they cannot exclude it either. ...

The Impact of Phonological Biases on Mispronunciation Sensitivity and Novel Accent Adaptation
  • Citing Article
  • May 2022

Language Learning and Development

... Similarly, Arabic grammar is another challenge because it has complex sentence patterns and word structures. The language has flexible word order, case marking, and a root-based system that needs much study to use them correctly (Rett & White, 2022). Students struggle with verb changes and how root words connect to other word parts. ...

Children Treat Grammatical Errors Differently for Native and Non-Native Speakers

... Infants as young as 6-month-olds are sensitive to speaker race as a possible determiner of linguistic information (Uttley et al., 2013; see also May et al., 2019). By 16 months, infants' interpretations of words change depending on the familiarity of the race of the speaker (Weatherhead & White, 2018), and at 22-month social cues influence whether toddlers think words should be generalized to new speakers (Weatherhead & Werker, 2022;Weatherhead & White, 2021). With age and experience, these sociolinguistic associations become stronger and more specific. ...

Toddlers Link Social and Speech Variation During Word Learning

... MISPRONUNCIATION SENSITIVITY AND NOVEL ACCENT ADAPTATION 2 Abstract Successful word recognition requires that listeners attend to differences that are phonemic in that language while also remaining flexible to the variation introduced by different voices and accents. Previous work has demonstrated that American-English-learning 19-month-olds are able to balance these demands: although one-off one-feature mispronunciations typically disrupt English-learning toddlers' lexical access, they no longer do after toddlers are exposed to a novel accent in which these changes occur systematically (White & Aslin, 2011;White & Daub, 2021). The flexibility to deal with different types of variation may not be the same for toddlers learning different first languages, however, as language structure shapes early phonological biases. ...

When it's appropriate not to adapt: Toddlers' learning of novel speech patterns is affected by visual information.
  • Citing Article
  • September 2021

Brain and Language

... As a result, rather little is known about child perception of child-produced speech, whether the speaker is the individual child (selfperception) or other children. However, this topic has gained interest in recent years (see Bernier & White, 2019a, 2019bCooper, Fecher, & Johnson, 2018; all reviewed in more detail below). One impetus for these studies has been to clarify the nature of children's underlying representations as they develop their lexicons and phonological systems, and how those representations relate to the input they receive, both from themselves and from others around them. ...

Toddlers Process Common and Infrequent Childhood Mispronunciations Differently for Child and Adult Speakers

... This result is not unusual nonetheless. Recent research on how disfluencies affect children's word learning has shown that young learners are able to learn new words equally well from both fluent and disfluent speakers (White et al. 2020). Hence, any reduced confidence a word learner may develop when exposed to less credible speakers does not seem to affect their overall learning. ...

That’s thee, uuh blicket! How does disfluency affect children’s word learning?
  • Citing Article
  • September 2019

First Language

... As a result, rather little is known about child perception of child-produced speech, whether the speaker is the individual child (selfperception) or other children. However, this topic has gained interest in recent years (see Bernier & White, 2019a, 2019bCooper, Fecher, & Johnson, 2018; all reviewed in more detail below). One impetus for these studies has been to clarify the nature of children's underlying representations as they develop their lexicons and phonological systems, and how those representations relate to the input they receive, both from themselves and from others around them. ...

Toddlers’ sensitivity to phonetic detail in child speech
  • Citing Article
  • September 2019

Journal of Experimental Child Psychology