Katherine S. Corker’s research while affiliated with Center for Open Science and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (64)


TOP 2025: An Update to the Transparency and Openness Promotion Guidelines
  • Preprint

February 2025

·

28 Reads

Sean Grant

·

Katherine S. Corker

·

David Thomas Mellor

·

[...]

·

Open science remains vital to the progress and functioning of the global research enterprise. Published in 2015, the Transparency and Openness Promotion Guidelines (TOP 2015) was developed as a policy framework to enhance the verifiability of empirical research claims in journal articles. It has been widely used and adopted by publishers and academic journals, but despite its uptake, concerns have been raised about aspects of the TOP 2015 framework and its implementation. In response to the above, the purpose of this manuscript is to introduce an official update to the TOP Guidelines. The final version—TOP 2025—provides updated guidelines for promoting the verifiability of published empirical research claims.


Understanding the Publish-Review-Curate (PRC) Model of Scholarly Communication

October 2024

·

13 Reads

The purpose of this brief is to provide an introduction to an increasingly popular way of communicating outputs of research: the Publish-Review-Curate (PRC) model. This model came into practice in the early 2000s, and it is now beginning to grow more rapidly. Here, we explain the model for the benefit of researchers, research funders, research institutions, and others in the scholarly communication ecosystem, and we provide data on uptake of the model to date.


TOP 2025: An Update to the Transparency and Openness Promotion Guidelines

September 2024

·

109 Reads

·

1 Citation

Open science remains vital to the progress and functioning of the global research enterprise. Published in 2015, the Transparency and Openness Promotion Guidelines (TOP 2015) was developed as a policy framework to enhance the verifiability of empirical research claims in journal articles. It has been widely used and adopted by publishers and academic journals, but despite its uptake, concerns have been raised about aspects of the TOP 2015 framework and its implementation. In response to the above, the purpose of this manuscript is to introduce an official update to the TOP Guidelines. The final version—TOP 2025—provides updated guidelines for promoting the verifiability of published empirical research claims.


Fig. 1 Key topics covered in the full guide
A guide for social science journal editors on easing into open science
  • Article
  • Full-text available

February 2024

·

178 Reads

·

13 Citations

Research Integrity and Peer Review

Journal editors have a large amount of power to advance open science in their respective fields by incentivising and mandating open policies and practices at their journals. The Data PASS Journal Editors Discussion Interface (JEDI, an online community for social science journal editors: www.dpjedi.org ) has collated several resources on embedding open science in journal editing ( www.dpjedi.org/resources ). However, it can be overwhelming as an editor new to open science practices to know where to start. For this reason, we created a guide for journal editors on how to get started with open science. The guide outlines steps that editors can take to implement open policies and practices within their journal, and goes through the what, why, how, and worries of each policy and practice. This manuscript introduces and summarizes the guide (full guide: https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/hstcx ).

Download


COMMENTARY Open Access Increasing the transparency of systematic reviews: presenting a generalized registration form

September 2023

·

85 Reads

·

6 Citations

Systematic Reviews

Abstract This paper presents a generalized registration form for systematic reviews that can be used when currently available forms are not adequate. The form is designed to be applicable across disciplines (i.e., psychology, economics, law, physics, or any other field) and across review types (i.e., scoping review, review of qualitative studies, meta-analysis, or any other type of review). That means that the reviewed records may include research reports as well as archive documents, case law, books, poems, etc. Items were selected and formulated to optimize broad applicability instead of specificity, forgoing some benefits afforded by a tighter focus. This PRISMA 2020 compliant form is a fallback for more specialized forms and can be used if no specialized form or registration platform is available. When accessing this form on the Open Science Framework website, users will therefore first be guided to specialized forms when they exist. In addition to this use case, the form can also serve as a starting point for creating registration forms that cater to specific fields or review types.


A Guide for Social Science Journal Editors on Easing into Open Science

June 2023

·

87 Reads

·

2 Citations

Journal editors have a large amount of power to advance open science in their respective fields by incentivizing and mandating open policies and practices at their journals. The Data PASS Journal Editors Discussion Interface (JEDI, an online community for social science journal editors: www.dpjedi.org) has collated several resources on embedding open science in journal editing (www.dpjedi.org/resources). However, it can be overwhelming as an editor new to open science practices to know where to start. For this reason, we have created a guide for journal editors on how to get started with open science. The guide outlines steps that editors can take to implement open policies and practices within their journal, and goes through the what, why, how, and worries of each policy and practice. This manuscript introduces and summarizes the guide (full guide: https://doi.org/10.31219/osf.io/hstcx)).


A Guide for Social Science Journal Editors on Easing into Open Science (FULL GUIDE)

May 2023

·

105 Reads

·

5 Citations

Journal editors have a large amount of power to advance open science in their respective fields by incentivising and mandating open policies and practices at their journals. The Data PASS Journal Editors Discussion Interface (JEDI, an online community for social science journal editors: www.dpjedi.org) has collated several resources on open science in journal editing (www.dpjedi.org/resources). However, it can be overwhelming as a new editor to know where to start. For this reason, we have created a guide for journal editors on how to get started with open science. The guide outlines steps that editors can take to implement open policies and practices at their journal, and goes through the what, why, how, and worries of each policy/practice.


The effects of temperature on prosocial and antisocial behaviour: A review and meta-analysis

February 2023

·

74 Reads

·

8 Citations

British Journal of Social Psychology

Research from the social sciences suggests an association between higher temperatures and increases in antisocial behaviours, including aggressive, violent, or sabotaging behaviours, and represents a heat-facilitates-aggression perspective. More recently, studies have shown that higher temperature experiences may also be linked to increases in prosocial behaviours, such as altruistic, sharing, or cooperative behaviours, representing a warmth-primes-prosociality view. However, across both literatures, there have been inconsistent findings and failures to replicate key theoretical predictions, leaving the status of temperature-behaviour links unclear. Here we review the literature and conduct meta-analyses of available empirical studies that have either prosocial (e.g., monetary reward, gift giving, helping behaviour) or antisocial (self-rewarding, retaliation, sabotaging behaviour) behavioural outcome variables, with temperature as an independent variable. In an omnibus multivariate analysis (total N = 4577) with 80 effect sizes, we found that there was no reliable effect of temperature on the behavioural outcome measured. Further, we find little support for either the warmth-primes-prosociality view or the heat-facilitates-aggression view. There were no reliable effects if we consider separately the type of behavioural outcome (prosocial or antisocial), different types of temperature experience (haptic or ambient), or potential interactions with the experimental social context (positive, neutral, or negative). We discuss how these findings affect the status of existing theoretical perspectives and provide specific suggestions advancing research in this area.


The effects of temperature on prosocial and antisocial behaviour: A review and meta- analysis

January 2023

·

8 Reads

Research from the social sciences suggests an association between higher temperatures and increases in antisocial behaviours, including aggressive, violent, or sabotaging behaviours, and represents a heat-facilitates-aggression perspective. More recently, studies have shown that higher temperature experiences may also be linked to increases in prosocial behaviours, such as altruistic, sharing, or cooperative behaviours, representing a warmth-primes-prosociality view. However, across both literatures, there have been inconsistent findings and failures to replicate key theoretical predictions, leaving the status of temperature-behaviour links unclear. Here we review the literature and conduct meta-analyses of available empirical studies that have either prosocial (e.g., monetary reward, gift giving, helping behaviour) or antisocial (self-rewarding, retaliation, sabotaging behaviour) behavioural outcome variables, with temperature as an independent variable. In an omnibus multivariate analysis (total N = 4577) with 80 effect sizes, we found that there was no reliable effect of temperature on the behavioural outcomes measured. Further, we find little support for either the warmth-primes-prosociality view or the heat- facilitates-aggression view. There were no reliable effects if we consider separately the type of behavioural outcome (prosocial or antisocial), different types of temperature experience (haptic or ambient), or potential interactions with the experimental social context (positive, neutral or negative). We discuss how these findings affect the status of existing theoretical perspectives, and provide specific suggestions advancing research in this area.


Citations (34)


... On the other hand, critics warn that preregistration's procedural rigidity might not only compound bureaucratic burdens of research but also dampen the flames of scientific discovery (McDermott, 2022;Bazzoli, 2022;Pham & Oh, 2021). While the debate advances with myth-debunking attitudes and skeptical replies, preregistration is increasingly becoming mandatory to publish and receive funding (Silverstein et al., 2024;Ferguson et al., 2023;Robson et al., 2021;Ramjoue, 2015). ...

Reference:

Preregistration is not a ceiling: but it can become one
A guide for social science journal editors on easing into open science

Research Integrity and Peer Review

... What nanotoxicological data related to ICNPs exist so far, relevant to the diagnosis and/or treatment of cancerous cells?". Following the formulation of the review questions, the authors established an a priori protocol, as described by van den Akker et al. [36]. The protocol was pre-registered in the Open Science Framework (OSF) Repository (https://osf.io/84rv5, ...

COMMENTARY Open Access Increasing the transparency of systematic reviews: presenting a generalized registration form

Systematic Reviews

... Pre-existing guidelines can be implemented in the journal submission system and can be part of the review process as well. Associate Editors may ensure thoroughness in study details by individually instructing authors to provide additional information, as necessary, to meet the minimum standards (Silverstein et al., 2023). Authors are advised to examine current reporting guidelines corresponding to the nature of their research. ...

A Guide for Social Science Journal Editors on Easing into Open Science (FULL GUIDE)
  • Citing Preprint
  • May 2023

... Willemse et al. [51] and Krause et al. [29] both recently refuted the concept of social warmth created from physical warmth in 2018 and 2023, respectively. Lynott et al. [31] also disputed it in 2023 with a review of the literature. The authors argued that previous studies either had too many biases or that the experimental protocols were not rigorous enough for such affirmations. ...

The effects of temperature on prosocial and antisocial behaviour: A review and meta-analysis
  • Citing Article
  • February 2023

British Journal of Social Psychology

... Social Investment Theory posits that normative age-graded changes in the Big Five across adulthood occur because of the investments in new social roles that many individuals experience in adulthood (Caspi, Roberts & Shiner, 2005;Helson et al., 2002). By investing in new social roles and experiencing related life events, like entering the workforce, getting married, becoming a parent, losing a spouse and retiring from work, individuals are required to meet specific contingencies in the environment (Bleidorn, Hopwood & Lucas, 2018;Corker & Donnellan, 2017;Hutteman et al., 2014;Luhmann et al., 2014;Senia & Donnellan, 2018). If individuals are responsive to the rewards, punishments and contingencies of a given setting, it is possible that long term exposure to, and changes in contingencies, may produce lasting personality changes (e.g., Laub & Sampson, 2003). ...

Person-Situation Transactions across the Lifespan
  • Citing Article
  • May 2017

... Much has been written about research ethics in Applied Linguistics (e.g., De Costa, 2016;De Costa et al, 2020, 2021Isbell et al., 2022;Larsson et al., 2023;Sheen, 2022;Sterling & De Costa, 2018;Sterling & Gass, 2017. See also Chapter 1, this volume) and in other disciplines such as education (e.g., Anderson et al, 2013), science (e.g., Fanelli, 2009), communication (e.g., Plaisance, 2008, psychotherapy (e.g., Sim, 2010) public health (e.g., Steneck, 2007), and social and behavioral sciences (Jussim et al., 2022). With some exceptions (e.g., Angelski et al., 2012;Greenwood, 2016;Manchón, 2022;Starfield & Paltridge, 2019;Wager et al., 2009), relatively little of the discussion in this area has focused on the ethical decisions that journal editors make. ...

Research Integrity: Best Practices for the Social and Behavioral Sciences
  • Citing Article
  • April 2022

... Personality research and statistics are closely related (Atherton et al., 2021). One of the empirical roads that researchers took in this field was based on the lexical hypothesis study of Allport and Odbert in 1936. ...

Why Has Personality Psychology Played an Outsized Role in the Credibility Revolution?

Personality Science

... This may be particularly important since there is evidence that many survey respondents are not paying full attention (see Snowberg & Yariv, 2021, p. 707). The combination of these recommendations, along with the OSPs used in this replication, will increase data quality and produce more credible evidence in the future (Nosek et al., 2022). ABBREVIATIONS: b = unstandardized regression coefficient; DV = dependent variable; RSE = robust standard error; ß = standardized regression coefficient *p < .05; ...

Replicability, Robustness, and Reproducibility in Psychological Science
  • Citing Article
  • January 2022

Annual Review of Psychology

... At least since 2020, an active conversation has developed among communication scholars (Dienlin et al., 2021). But in contrast to psychology, which in many large-scale projects havewithout success-attempted to replicate prior findings (Nosek et al., 2021), such endeavors are still missing in communication, where direct replications are largely absent (Keating & Totzkay, 2019). Yet, such attempts in psychology started with individual scholars' practices, which hence, necessitate the present exploration of open science practices among communication scholars. ...

Replicability, Robustness, and Reproducibility in Psychological Science
  • Citing Preprint
  • February 2021

... Even if an unconfounded effect of prompts on PEB exists, the robustness and trustworthiness of this effect is currently unclear. This is because systematic replication efforts, such as the Open Science Collaboration (2015) or the Many Labs studies (e.g., Ebersole et al., 2020), suggest that psychological research can be difficult to replicate and, therefore, is not always trustworthy. Possible reasons underlying this low replicability are publication bias and questionable research practices. ...

Many Labs 5: Testing Pre-Data-Collection Peer Review as an Intervention to Increase Replicability

Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science