Karin Steijven’s research while affiliated with University of Wuerzburg and other places

What is this page?


This page lists works of an author who doesn't have a ResearchGate profile or hasn't added the works to their profile yet. It is automatically generated from public (personal) data to further our legitimate goal of comprehensive and accurate scientific recordkeeping. If you are this author and want this page removed, please let us know.

Publications (12)


Supplemental Information 1
  • Data
  • File available

October 2017

·

12 Reads

Karin Steijven

·

·

·

Learning curves and retention tests of honeybees that were conditioned in an additional experiment to show that bees could associate either odor equally well with a reward In 12 trials bees were presented with either CS+ or CS − (haphazard order) and PER was recorded. After 1 h and 48 h memory of the conditioned respons was tested (retention). Asterisks show the level of statistical difference between CS+ and CS−, with ***: p < 0.001. Regardless which odour was used as CS+ or and as CS−, most bees were able to distinguish odours in the aquisition fase, and during the retention fase. A) Here 1-nonanol was used as CS+ (grey) and 1-hexanol as CS− (black). (B) Here results of the reciprocal conditioning are shown: for CS+ (grey) 1-hexanol was used, and for CS− (black) 1-nonanol was used.

Download


Figure 1 Reconstruction of a brain from an in-lab reared worker. Red, Antennal lobes, AL; Blue, Mushroom bodies, MB (comprising medial and lateral calyx and the pedunculus); Green, central complex, CB (comprising upper and lower central complex and protocerebral bridge); Yellow, Lobula; Orange, Medulla. Full-size DOI: 10.7717/peerj.3858/fig-1 
Figure 2 Learning curves and retention tests of the five treatment groups. Grey depicts the CS+ and black the CS−, light grey corresponds to the treatment groups that were reared in the lab with different quantities of total brood food consumed (A-D), and dark grey is the in-hive control (E). See text for statistics. Full-size DOI: 10.7717/peerj.3858/fig-2 
Figure 4 Volumetric reconstruction of the mushroom bodies (MB, A), containing the pedunculus (B) and the medial (C) and the lateral calyces (D). All treatment groups that were reared in the lab (with 150, 160, 170, and 180 µl of artificial diet) were compared to the in-hive control. Asterisks depict significance levels (p < 0.05, * ; p < 0.01, * * ; p < 0.001, * * * ). Full-size DOI: 10.7717/peerj.3858/fig-4 
Learning performance and brain structure of artificially-reared honey bees fed with different quantities of food

October 2017

·

301 Reads

·

17 Citations

Background Artificial rearing of honey bee larvae is an established method which enables to fully standardize the rearing environment and to manipulate the supplied diet to the brood. However, there are no studies which compare learning performance or neuroanatomic differences of artificially-reared (in-lab) bees in comparison with their in-hive reared counterparts. Methods Here we tested how different quantities of food during larval development affect body size, brain morphology and learning ability of adult honey bees. We used in-lab rearing to be able to manipulate the total quantity of food consumed during larval development. After hatching, a subset of the bees was taken for which we made 3D reconstructions of the brains using confocal laser-scanning microscopy. Learning ability and memory formation of the remaining bees was tested in a differential olfactory conditioning experiment. Finally, we evaluated how bees reared with different quantities of artificial diet compared to in-hive reared bees. Results Thorax and head size of in-lab reared honey bees, when fed the standard diet of 160 µl or less, were slightly smaller than hive bees. The brain structure analyses showed that artificially reared bees had smaller mushroom body (MB) lateral calyces than their in-hive counterparts, independently of the quantity of food they received. However, they showed the same total brain size and the same associative learning ability as in-hive reared bees. In terms of mid-term memory, but not early long-term memory, they performed even better than the in-hive control. Discussion We have demonstrated that bees that are reared artificially (according to the Aupinel protocol) and kept in lab-conditions perform the same or even better than their in-hive sisters in an olfactory conditioning experiment even though their lateral calyces were consistently smaller at emergence. The applied combination of experimental manipulation during the larval phase plus subsequent behavioral and neuro-anatomic analyses is a powerful tool for basic and applied honey bee research.





FIGURE 1. A tripped and untripped flower of D. setigerum (above, photo DS), and a small solitary bee (Lasioglossum/Pseudapis sp.) foraging at tip of keel complex in untripped D. setigerum; an illegitimate visit that does not trigger explosive pollen release (below, photo CN).  
FIGURE 2. Mean number of seeds produced per flower of D. setigerum following pollination treatments. Error bars indicate standard error, and letters indicate significant differences (P < 0.05). N = 270 flowers (54 flowers per treatment).  
FIGURE 3. The number of untripped D. setigerum flowers available across the day in two transects. Numbers of flowers increase initially as flowers open in the morning, but steadily decrease during the day as nearly all flowers become tripped by 1800 h. on stigmas from 30 further flowers that had been artificially tripped (Tab. 1). The number of pollen grains that adhered to stigmas ranged from 0 (hand-triggered; illegitimate visitors Lasioglossum sp. 2/Pseudapis sp.); to 404 (Megachile sp. 1), but pollen was deposited on at least some flowers by each visitor bee species represented in the analysis. Hand-triggered flowers had extremely low pollen deposition (6.8 ± 3.32 grains; N = 30 flowers), as did flowers receiving illegitimate visitation (37.4 ± 6.03; N = 27) (Tab. 1). We found a highly significant effect of species on the number of pollen grains deposited (ANOVA: F13,138 = 7.85, P < 0.001, Fig. 4); " Megachile sp. 1 " deposited a larger number of pollen grains than most other species. We also found an impact of pollen placement on the number of pollen grains deposited (ANOVA: F4,147 = 7.31, P < 0.001);  
Polliantion ecology of Desmodium setigerum (Fabaceae) in Uganda; do big bees do it better?

August 2016

·

856 Reads

·

7 Citations

Journal of Pollination Ecology

·

·

Karin Steijven

·

[...]

·

Explosive pollen release is documented in many plant families, including the Fabaceae. Desmodium setigerum E. Mey (Fabaceae) is a perennial herb with single trip explosive pollen release found in eastern Africa, and the unique ability to reverse floral colour change if insufficient pollination has occurred. However, little else is known about the pollination ecology of this species, what visitors can trigger explosive pollen release, and whether bee body size is related to pollination efficiency. We investigated: 1) the breeding system of D. setigerum, and whether it is pollen limited; 2) whether flowers are visited early in the day allowing sufficient time for a second opportunity for pollination; and 3) what insect species visit D. setigerum and the relative efficacy of different flower visitors in relation to visitor size and pollination success. We found that although self-compatible, D. setigerum requires insect visitation to set seed as explosive pollen release is needed even for selfing. Most flowers are initially visited before 1400h, and by 1800h nearly all flowers have been tripped. Flowers were not pollen limited in this study, and were visited primarily by bees. We observed 16 visiting species, and there was a wide variation (0-404 grains) in the amount of pollen deposited on stigmas. Although almost all bees deposited some pollen, the mean number of pollen grains deposited in a single visit per species was negatively related to body size. However, one particular megachilid species deposited significantly more pollen grains than any other visitor and so is likely an important pollinator of this species. This provides insights into the pollination biology of this unique plant species, and adds to increasing literature on the relationships between bee body size, explosive pollen release and pollination effectiveness.



Testing dose-dependent effects of stacked Bt maize pollen on in vitro-reared honey bee larvae

October 2015

·

91 Reads

·

12 Citations

Apidologie

In agricultural landscapes all over the world, honey bees are exposed to genetically modified (GM) pollen. Stacked Bt-maize varieties combine multiple Bt proteins against different insect taxa with herbicide resistance. To test for potential effects on non-target organisms, we conducted an in vitro larvae-rearing experiment where we fed increasing amounts of stacked Bt pollen to honey bee larvae. Bt pollen was equally well digested as two control maize varieties. No dose-dependent effects of Bt maize were detected in terms of survival and developmental delay. However, for prepupal weight we did find a dose-dependent response, suggesting a pleiotropic effect. Comparing this finding with the literature we conclude that the found effect is not likely to occur in a field situation. We could however show that dose-dependent effects are detectable. Our results underline the importance of testing dose-depending effects of GM plant material in an environmental risk assessment (ERA).


Co-occurrence and floral isolation in two Sisyrinchium species (Iridaceae)

September 2014

·

206 Reads


Citations (4)


... Unfortunately, the environment used for larval rearing differs between in vivo and in vitro conditions and can affect the morphology, physiology, and behavior of bees at the adult stage. For instance, bees reared larval in vitro (RL vitro ) can have smaller body sizes and smaller lateral calyces than bees reared larval in vivo (RL vivo ; Steijven et al., 2017). The flight behavior and performance of suckling and foraging tasks are reduced in RL vitro bees compared with RL vivo bees (Brodschneider et al., 2009;Schilcher et al., 2022). ...

Reference:

Experimental Ecotoxicology Procedures Interfere with Honey Bee Life History
Learning performance and brain structure of artificially-reared honey bees fed with different quantities of food

... In flowers with 2PP, pollen follows a longer path from anther dehiscence to pollen transfer, relative to flowers with primary Nascimento et al. 2018 Cannaceae (1) 1 (Canna) Style Deposition Stiles and Freeman 1993;Yeo 1993;Ladd 1994;Glinos and Cocucci 2011;Maruyama et al. 2015 Pump, brush, deposition Arroyo 1981; Lavin and Delgado 1990;Yeo 1993;Westerkamp 1997;Westerkamp and Weber 1999;Etcheverry et al. 2008Etcheverry et al. , 2012aEtcheverry et al. , 2012bGalloni et al. 2007;Willmer et al. 2009;Delgado-Salinas et al. 2011;Alemán et al. 2014;Stanley et al. 2016;Fleming and Etcheverry 2017;Etcheverry and Vogel 2018 Polygalaceae (29) 3 Style Pump, deposition Yeo 1993;Ladd 1994;Westerkamp 1997;Westerkamp and Weber 1999;Castro et al. 2008aCastro et al. , 2008bDe Kock et al. 2018;Uluer et al. 2022 Malpighiales: Rhizophoraceae ( Rosids-malvids clade: ...

Polliantion ecology of Desmodium setigerum (Fabaceae) in Uganda; do big bees do it better?

Journal of Pollination Ecology

... Exposing immatures of Apis mellifera to different pollen diets For exposures, (i) 10 mg of Eucalyptus pollen 751K032 was added in 30 µL of larval food (Steijven et al. 2016). Similarly, the control groups had the addition of, (ii) 10 mg of conventional Eucalyptus clone FGN-K (1st negative control) and (iii) 10 mg of Pictures showing different moments of study. ...

Testing dose-dependent effects of stacked Bt maize pollen on in vitro-reared honey bee larvae
  • Citing Article
  • October 2015

Apidologie

... Functions of anthocyanins might differ depending on developmental state, plant organ, or environmental conditions. For example, anthocyanins in leaves are likely protecting against high light intensities and other stress factors (Pringsheim, 1881;Wheldale, 1916;Landi et al., 2015), while anthocyanins in flowers and fruits are most likely attracting pollinators and seed dispersers (Weiss, 1991;Willmer et al., 2009;Ruxton & Schaefer, 2016;Garcia et al., 2022). Understanding the ecological functions of anthocyanins is crucial when generating hypotheses about evolutionary trajectories that lead to the anthocyanin biosynthesis as we see it today. ...

Bidirectional Flower Color and Shape Changes Allow a Second Opportunity for Pollination
  • Citing Article
  • May 2009

Current Biology